Qualitative Psychology or the assumption of a unitary .scientific method of observation, experimentation, logic, and evidence, Positivistic beliefs in scientific logic, objectivity, and truth supported and legitimized reducing qualities of human experience to quantifiable variables. Positivistic methods assumed an unbiased and passive observer, the separation of fae! from value, the existence of an external world separate Írom scientific observers and their methods, and the accumulation of knowledge about this world. Hence, positivism led to a quest for valid instruments, replicabie research designs, and reliable findings. Most quantitative methodologists of the 1960s ignored human problems that did not fit positivistic research designs, if they acknowledged qualitative research at all, proponents of quantification considered it to be a preiiminary exercise for refining quantitative instruments. Simultaneously, the division between theory and research grew. At that time, theory informed quantitative research through the logico-deductive model of inquiry, which relied on deducing testable hypotheses from an existing theory. Yet this research seldom led to new theory construction. In their initial statement of grounded theory, Glaser and Strauss (1967) challenged: • the arbitrary division of theory and research « prevailing views of qualitative research as a precursor to more 'rigorous' quantitative methods • beliefs that qualitative methods were impressionistic and unsystematic o separation of data collection and analysis phases of research • assumptions that qualitative research could not generate theory. Glaser and Strauss built on their qualitative predecessors' implicit analytic procedures and research strategies and made them explicit. Earlier qualitative researchers had taught generations of students through mentoring and immersion in field experience {Rock, 1979). Glaser and Strauss's written guidelines for conducting qualitative research changed that oral tradition. The epistemologica! assumptions, logic, and systematic approach of grounded theory methods reflect Glaser's rigorous quantitative training at Columbia University. Strong links to symbolic interaction, with its stress on human reflection, choice, and action, stem from Strauss's training at the University of Chicago with Herbert Blurner and Robert Park. Through their influence, Strauss adopted both the pragmatist philosophical tradition with its emphasis on studying process, action, and meaning (Blumer, 1969; Mead, 1934) and the Chicago legacy of ethnographic research (Park and Burgess, 1921). Grounded theory 85 Grounded theory contains both positivistic and interpretive elements, lis emphasis on using systematic techniques to study an external world remains consistent with positivism. Its stress on how people construct actions, meanings, and intentions is in keeping with interpretive traditions. Some grounded theorists join me (see, for example, Clarke, 1998) in assuming that a researcher's disciplinary and theoretical proclivities, relationships and interactions with respondents all shape the collection, content, and analysis of data. Grounded theory can bridge traditional positivistic methods with interpretive methods in disciplines such as psychology that have embraced quantification. These methods allow psychologists to study aspects of human experience that remain inaccessible with traditional verification methods. The grounded theory emphasis on process enables psychologists to study how individual and interpersonal processes develop, are maintained, or change. Generating Data With grounded theory, you begin by exploring general questions about a research topic of interest. You collect data about what people who have relevant experience of this topic say and do about it. Grounded theorists' background assumptions and disciplinary interests alert them to look for certain issues and processes in their data. Consistent with Blumer's (1969) depiction of 'sensitizing concepts', grounded theorists often begin their studies with certain research interests and a set of general concepts. These concepts give you ideas to pursue and sensitize you to ask particular kinds of questions about your topic. For example, ! began my studies of people with chronic illnesses with an interest in how they experienced time and how their experiences of illness affected them. My guiding interests brought concepts such as self-concept, identity, and duration into the study. But that was only the start. 1 used those concepts as points of departure to form interview questions, to look at data, to listen to interviewees, and to think analytically about the data. Guiding interests should provide you with such points of departure for developing, rather than limiting, your ideas. 7'hcn you develop specific concepts by examining your ideas through successive stages of analysis and studying your data. Thus, sensitizing concepts provide a place to start, not end. A thorough foundation in a discipline provides such concepts. Professional researchers already hold epistemologica! assumptions about the world, disciplinary perspectives, and often an intimate familiarity with the research topic and the pertinent literature. Yet every grounded theory researcher should remain as open as possible to new views during the research. Hence, grounded theorists develop their sensitizing concepts in relation to the processes they define in their data. In contrast, the logico-deductive Qualitative Psychoiogy Grounded theory 87 model of traditional model of research necessitates operalionaiizing the previously established concept as accurately as possible. In grounded theory research, you begin analysing what you gather early in your data collection. Simultaneous involvement in data collection and analysis means that your emerging analysis shapes your data collection decisions. Early analytic work leads you lo collect more data around emerging themes and questions. For example, we sense Susan Nelson's efforts to account for her pain and fatigue in the interview excerpt above. Her remarks alert the interviewer to ask how she discovered her other conditions and how other people responded to both her search and her conclusions. Then, further questions may be built into subsequent interviews with other participants. Through simultaneous involvement in data collection and analysis, you avoid being overwhelmed by volumes of general, unfocused data that do not lead lo anything new. If you already have collected a substantial amount of data, begin with it, but subsequently collect additional data about your emerging analytic interests and themes. That way, you can follow up topics that are explicit in one interview or observation but remain implicit or absent in others. For example, a woman with multiple sclerosis mentioned having 'bad days'. She said, '! deal with time differently (during a bad day when she felt sick] and time has a different meaning lo me' {Charmaz, 1991a: 52). When we discussed meanings of time, 1 saw how she connected experiencing time with images of self. On a bad day, her day shortened because all her daily routines - such as bathing, dressing, exercising, and resting - lengthened substantially. As her daily routines stretched, her preferred self shrunk- After I saw how she defined herself in relation to mundane daily routines, I asked interview questions that directly addressed this relationship. Her comment provided a valuable source of comparison, along with ideas to corroborate in other interviews. For example, this piece of data aliowed me to frame new questions. To what extent do people view themselves as separated from or embedded in their daily routines? Which daily routines? How does sickness affect their views? When do they claim the self that they experience while ill? When do they reject it? The core components of grounded theory studies are analytic categories developed while studying the data rather than preconceived concepts or hypotheses. These categories move your study toward abstract analyses yet simultaneously elucidate what happens in the empirical world. From the beginning, researchers actively construct their data with study participants. The first question to ask is, 'What is happening here?' (Glaser, 1978, 1992; Glaser and Strauss, 1967). Then you have to think of ways to find out. Perhaps their enthusiasm for developing an inductive methodology that anchored emergent theory in data led Glaser and Strauss (1967; Glaser, 1978) to imply in their early works that categories inhere in the data and may even leap out. 3 disagree. Rather, categories reflect interactions between the observer and observed. Certainly, social researchers' world-views, disciplinary assumptions, theoretical proclivities and research interests shape what f hey create (see also Dey, 1999) by influencing their observations and emerging categories. What happens if the data do not illuminate your initial interests? Grounded theorists evaluate the fit between their initial research interests and the emerging data. We do not force preconceived ideas and theories directly upon our data. However, what might stand as a viable means of gathering data to one grounded theorist might be defined as forcing the data into a preconceived framework to another. For example, Glaser (1998: 94) cautions against preconceiving 'interview guides, units for data collection, samples, received codes, following diagrams, rules for proper memoing and so forth'. Grounded theorists do agree on starting analysis with the data. We follow leads that we define in the data. Thus, I started with research interests m time and self-concept but also pursued other topics that my respondents defined as crucial. For example, 1 felt compelled to explore their concerns about disclosing illness, which I had not anticipated studying but which had emerged as a significant theme in the data. I studied how, when, and why ill people talk about their conditions. However, my interest in time alerted me to see whether people's accounts of disclosing their conditions changed over lime. What kind of data should you gather for grounded theory studies? To ihe extent possible, I advocate going inside the studied phenomenon and gathering extensive, rich data about it, while simultaneously using grounded theory strategies to direct my data collection. Rich data reveal participants' thoughts, feelings, intentions, and actions as well as context and structure. My call for rich, detailed data means seeking full or 'thick' description (Geeríz, J973) such as writing extensive field notes of observations, collecting respondents' written personal accounts, and compiling detailed narratives of experience (such as transcribed tapes of interviews). Seidman (1998) advocates sequential intensive interviewing to build trust and to elicit detailed data. Transcribed tape recordings of interviews provide details for nuanced views and reviews of data, f find that studying the transcriptions gives me new insights and more codes with which to work, in contrast, Glaser (1998) argues that transcribing wastes lime and fosters becoming lost in data. Grounded theorists take different, sometimes contradictory approaches to data collection, although all assume that the strength of grounded theory lies in its empirical foundation. Glaser (1992; 1998) consistently stresses discovering what is happening in the setting without forcing the data into preconceived categories through such errors as applying extant theories to it, assuming the significance of demographic variables (such as age, sex, race, marital status, and occupation; also called face-sheet variables) before beginning the study, imposing evidentiary rules (a priori prescriptions about 88 Qualitative Psychology whal stands as sufficient evidence) on the data, or failing to make theoretical distinctions with empirical description. However, he also advocates short cuts such as moving quickly from one empirical world to another to develop a category, not transcribing interviews, and accepting a group's overt statements about itself, a practice which may obfuscate members' fundamental concerns. Such short cuts can cause problems. Researchers may obtain only a surface view of a group when they move quickly from one research site to another. In addition, people may not offer much beyond a public relations viewpoint until they trust the researcher. Furthermore, members may reveal their most important values and priorities through actions and assumptions, not careful statements. In effect, short cuts may curtail discoveries, miss basic social processes, overlook subtle meanings, and force data into categories prematurely. Strauss and Oorbin (1990) imply that concrete observed behaviour with scant interpretation constitutes solid data when they give hypothetical examples that lack nuanced description of a setting and the social actors and interaction within it. Their hypothetical example of the restaurant hostess describes overt movements and, thus, misses what actual hostesses do, feel, and think about their situations (1990: 63-5). However, their interview data in Unending Work and Care (Corbin and Strauss, 1.988) contains research participants' nuanced stories and meaningful statements. Situate your data within settings and scenes, collective meanings, and individual interpretations, actions, and processes. Then, your descriptions will have more substance and form than mere observed behaviour. Even if you have detailed raw data, such as the typed transcription of a patient conference, elaborate on it. Provide the context by describing the structure of the conference, the events preceding it, the players in it, and their unstated concerns (if known or implicit in their non-verbai behaviour). Similarly, place a personal interview into perspective by describing the situation, the interaction, the person's affect, and your perception of how the interview went. Thorough written texts give you data to study. In short, get as much material down on paper as possible. Rich data afford views of human experience that etiquette, social conventions, and inaccessibility hide or minimize in ordinary discourse. To obtain rich data: • describe participants' views and actions in detail • record observations that reveal participants' unstated intentions • construct interview questions that allow participants to reflect anew on the research topic * look for and explore taken-for-granted meanings and actions. Grounded theory 89 Tell me about', 'how', 'what', and 'when' questions yield rich data, particularly when you buttress them with queries to elaborate or to specify, such as 'Could you describe -— further' (for a sample interview guide, see Charmaz, 2001). Look for the 'urns' and 'you know's'; explore what they Indicate. How might they reflect a struggle to find words? When might a 'you know' signal taken-for-granted meanings? What do long pauses indicate? When might 'you know' seek the interviewer's concurrence or suggest that the respondent is struggling to articulate an experience? In my research, however, respondents' stories about illness often spilled out non-stop. For example, Christine Danforth stated: if you have lupus, I mean one day it's my liver; one day it's my joints; one day it's my head, and it's like people really think you're a hypochondriac if you keep complaining about different ailments. . . . it's like yon don't want to say anything because people 6~8). Again, make your categories as conceptual as possible - with abstract power, genera! reach, analytic direction, and precise wording. Simultaneously, remain consistent with your data. By making focused codes active (to reflect what people are doing or what is happening) and brief, you can view them as potential categories. Processes gain visibility when you keep codes active. Succinct focused codes lead to sharp, clear categories. That way, you can establish criteria for your categories to make further comparisons. Grounded theorists look for substantive processes that they develop from their codes. 'Keeping illness contained', 'packaging illness', and 'living one day at a time' above are three such processes. As grounded theorists create conceptual handles to explain what is happening in the setting, they may move toward defining generic processes (Prus, 1987). A generic process cuts across different empirical settings and problems; it can be applied to varied substantive areas. The two codes above, 'avoiding disclosure' and 'assessing potential losses and risks of disclosing', reflect fundamental, generic processes of personal information control. Although these processes Qualitative Psychology describe choices people wilh illness make in disclosing information, people with other problems may treat information control similarly. For sociologists, generic processes are basic to social life; for psychologists, generic-processes are fundamental for psychological existence. Thus, a grounded theorist can elaborate and refine the generic process by gathering more data from diverse arenas where this process is evident. For example, personal information control and choices in disclosing are often problematic for homosexuals, sexual abuse survivors, drug users, and ex-convicts, as well as for people with chronic conditions. Concentrate on analysing a generic process that you define in your codes; then you can raise codes relevant to theoretical categories that lead to expianations of the process and predictions concerning it. These categories reflect what you think about the data as well as what you find in them. As Dey (1999) observes, categorization in grounded theory is more complex and problematic than its originators suggest and involves making inferences as well as classifications. As you raise a code to a category, you begin to write narrative statements in memos, as I outline beiow, that: • explicate the properties of the category • specify the conditions under which the category arises, is maintained, and changes • describe its consequences • show how this category relates to other categories. Categories may consist of in vivo codes that you take directly from your respondents' discourse, or they may represent your theoretical or substantive definition of what is happening in the data. For example, my terms 'good days and bad days' and 'living one day at a time' came directly from my respondents' voices. In contrast, my categories 'recapturing the past' and 'time in immersion and immersion in time' reflect theoretical definitions of actions and events. Furthermore, categories such as 'pulling in', 'facing dependency', and 'making trade-offs' address my respondents' substantive realities of grappling with a serious illness. í created these codes and used them as categories, but they reflect my respondents' concerns and actions. Novice researchers may find that they rely most on in vivo and substantive codes. What results is often a grounded description more than a theory. Nonetheless, studying how these codes fit together in categories can help you treat them more theoretically. Through focused coding, you build and clarify your category by examining all the data it covers and by identifying variations within it and between other categories. You also wi!I become aware of gaps in your Grounded theory 1 Oox 5.3 The category of 'existing from day to day' Existing from day to day occurs when a person plummets into continued crises that rip life apart. I! reflects a toss of control of health and the wherewithal to keep life together. Existing from day to day means a constant struggle for daily survival. Poverty and lack of support contribute to and complicate that struggle. Hence, poor and isolated people usually plummet further and faster than affiueni individuals with concerned families. Loss of control extends to being unable to obtain necessities -food, shelter, heat, and medical care. The struggle to exist keeps people in the present, especially if they have continued problems in getting fhe basic necessities that middle-class adults take for granted. Yet other problems can assume much greater significance for these people than their iliness - a violent husband, a runaway child, an alcoholic spouse, or the overdue rent. Living one day at a time differs from existing from day to day. Living one day at a time provides a strategy for controlling emotions, managing life, dimming the future, and getting through a troublesome period. St involves managing stress, illness, or regimen, and dealing with these things each day to control them as best one can. it means concentrating on the here and now and relinquishing other goals, pursuits, and obligations (Channaz, 1991a: 185). analysis. For example, I developed my category of 'existing from day to day' when ! realized that 'living one day at a time' did not fully covet impoverished people's level of desperation, in short, I had data about a daily struggle to survive that were not subsumed by my first category of living one day at a time. The finished narrative can be seen in Box 5.3. Note the comparisons between the two categories above. To generate categories through focused coding, you need to compare data, incidents, contexts, and concepts. Making the following comparisons helps: o comparing different people (about their beliefs, situations, actions, accounts, or experiences) * comparing data from the same individuals at different points in time e comparing specific data with the criteria for the category e comparing categories in the analysis with other categories. As ! compared different people's experiences, I realized that some peopäe's situations forced them into the present. 1 then looked at how my rendering of living one day at a time did not apply to them. 1 reviewed 102 Qualitative Psychology earlier interviews and began 10 seek published accounts lhal might clarify the comparison. As is evident in the distinctions between these |wo categories above, focused coding prompts you to begin to see the relationships and patterns between categories. Memo-writing You may have thought of memos as business communications to state policies, procedures, and proposals. However, in grounded theory, memos serve analytic purposes. Memo-writing consists of taking your categories apart by breaking them into their components. It is the pivotal intermediate step between defining categories and the first draft of your completed analysis. This step spurs you to develop your ideas in narrative form and fullness early in the analytic process. Memo-writing is the logical next step after you define categories; however, it is also helpful for clarification and direction during coding. Writing memos prompts you to elaborate processes, assumptions, and actions covered by your codes or categories. Memos also help you to identify which codes to treat as analytic categories, if you have not already defined them. (Then you further develop your category through more memo-writing.) Think about including as many of the following points in your memos as is possible and useful: • defining each code or category by its analytic properties • spelling out and detailing processes subsumed by the codes or catgories • making comparisons between data and between codes and categories « bringing raw data into the memo o providing sufficient empirical evidence to support your definitions of the category and analytic claims about it » offering conjectures to check in the empirical research • identifying gaps in the analysis. Grounded theorists look for patterns, even when focusing on a single case (Strauss and Glaser, 1970). Because they stress identifying patterns, grounded theorists typically invoke respondents' stories to illustrate points -rather than provide complete portrayals of their lives. By bringing raw data right into your memo, you preserve telling evidence for your ideas from Grounded theory lhe start of your analytic narratives. Through providing ample verbatim material, you not only ground the abstract analysis, but also lay the foundation for making claims about it. Including verbatim material from different sources permits you to make precise comparisons. Thus, memo-writing moves your work beyond individual cases through defining patterns. Begin your memo with careful definitions of each category. That means you identify its properties or characteristics, look for its underlying assumptions, and show how and when the category develops and changes. To illustrate, 1 found thai people frequently referred to living one day at a time when they suffered a medical crisis or faced continued uncertainty. So 1 began to ask questions about what living one day at a time was like for them. From their responses as well as from published autobiographical accounts, 1 began to define the category and its characteristics. The term 'living one day at a time' condenses a whole series of implicit meanings and assumptions, it becomes a strategy for handling unruly feelings, for exerting some control over a now uncontrollable life, for facing uncertainty, and for handling a conceivably foreshortened future. Memo-writing spurs you to dig into implicit, unstated, and condensed meanings. Start writing memos as soon as you have some interesting ideas and categories to pursue, if at a loss about what to write, elaborate on codes that you adopted repeatedly. Keep collecting data, keep coding, and keep refining your ideas through writing more and further developed memos. Some researchers who use grounded theory methods discover a few interesting findings early in their data collection and then truncate their research. Their work lacks the 'intimate familiarity' with the setting or experience that Lofiand and Lofland (1995) avow meets the standards for good qualitative research. Cover your topic in depth by exploring sufficient cases and by elaborating your categories fully. Memo-writing frees you to explore your ideas about your categories. Treat memos as partial, preliminary, and eminently correctable, just note where you are on firm ground and where you are making conjectures. Then go back to the field to check your conjectures. Memo-writing is much like free-writing or prewriting (Elbow, 1981). Use memos to help you think about the data and to discover your ideas about them. You can write memos for your eyes only. Do not worry about verb tense, overuse of prepositional phrases, or lengthy sentences at this point. Just get your ideas down as quickly and clearly as you can. You are writing to render the data, not to communicate it to an audience. Later, after you turn your memo into a section of a paper, revise the material to make it accessible to a reader. Writing memos quickly without editing them fosters developing and preserving your natural voice. Then your memo reads as though written by a living, thinking, feeling human being rather than a pedantic social scientist. You can write memos at different levels of abstraction - from the concrete to the highly theoretical. Some of your memos will find their way directly into