E S S A Y Decision Making: It's Not What You Think Sometimes decisions defy purely step-by-step logic. To be effective, companies also should embrace intuitive or action-oriented forms of decision making. Henry Mintzberg and Frances Westley How should decisions be made? Easy, we figured that out long ago. First define the problem, then diagnose its causes, next design possible solutions, and finally decide which is best. And, of course, implement the choice. But do people always make decisions that way? We propose that this rational, or "thinking first," model of decision making should be supplemented with two very different models — a "seeing first" and a "doing first" model. When practicing managers use all three models, they can improve the quality of their decisions. Healthy organizations, like healthy people, have the capacity for all three. Consider how a real decision was made, a personal one in this case. It begins with a call from an aunt. "Hi, kiddo. I want to buy you a housewarming present. What's the color scheme in your new apartment?" "Coior scheme? Betty, you've got to be kidding. Til have to ask Lisa. Lisa, Betty wants to know the color scheme of the apartment." "Black," daughter Lisa says. "Black? Lisa, I've got to live there." "Black," she repeats. A few days later, father and daughter find themselves in a furniture store. They try every desk, every chair: Nothing works. Shopper's lethargy sets in. Then Lisa spots a black stool: "Wouldn't that look great against the white counter?" And they're off. Within an hour, they have picked out everything — in black, white and steel gray. The extraordinary thing about this ordinary story is that our conventional theories of decision making can't explain it. It is not even clear what the final decision was: to buy the stool; to get on with furnishing an apartment; to do so in black and white; to create a new lifestyle? Decision making can be mysterious. The Limits of "Thinking First" Rational decision making has a clearly identified process: define -*• diagnose -*• design •*• decide. However, the rational approach turns out to be uncommon. Years ago, one of us studied a host of decisions, delineating the steps and then laying them out. A illuiiratlon:fflJonnS. SPRINC 2001 MIT SLOAN MANACEMENT REVIEW 89 Insight: Groping Precedes Zeroing In Choices Looking for Problems t Insight 1 Insight 2 I dcti^itin process for building a new plant was typical. The process kept cycling back, interrupted by new events, diverted by opportunities and so on, going round and round until finally .1 solution emerged. The final action was as clear as a wave breaking on the shore, but explaining how it came to be is as liLird as tracing the origin of that wave back into the ocean. Often decisions do not so much emerge as erupt. Here is how Alexander Kotov, the chess master, has described a sudden insight that followed lengthy analysis: "So, I mustn't move the knight. Try the rook move again.... At this point you glance at the clock. 'My goodness! Already 30 minutes gone on thinking about whether to move the rook or the knight. If it goes on like this you'll really be in time trouble.' And then suddenly you are .struck by the happy idea — why move rook or knight? What about B-QNl? And without any more ado, without analysis at all, you move the bishop. lust like that." Perhaps, then, decision making means periods of groping followed by sudden sharp insights that lead to crystallization, as A. Limgley and co-authors suggested in a 1995 Organizational Science article. (See "Insight: Groping Precedes Zeroing In.") Or perhaps it is a form of "organized anarchy," as Stanford professor James March and colleagues have written. They characterize decision making as "collections of choices looking for problems, issues and feelings looking for decision situations in which they may be aired, solutions looking for issues to which iliey might be an answer, and decision makers looking for work." (See "Choices Looking for Problems.") But is the confusion, as described by those authors, in the process, or is it in the observers? Maybe messy, real-life decision making makes more sense than we think, precisely because so iiuich of it is beyond conscious thought. "Seeing First" Insight — "seeing into" — suggests that decisions, or at least actions, may be driven as much by what is seen as by what is thought. As Mozart said, the best part about creating a symphony was being able to "see the whole of it at a single glance in my mind." So, understanding can be visual as well as conceptual. In W. Koehler's well-known 1920s experiment, an ape struggled to reach a banana placed high in its cage. Then it saw ihe box in the corner — not just noticed it, but realized what could be done with it — and its problem was solved. Likewise after Alexander Fleming really .sciwthe mold that had killed the bacteria in some of bis research samples (in otber words, when he realized how that mold could be used), he and his colleague were able to give us penicillin. The same can be true for strategic vision. Vi.sion requires the courage to see what others do not — and that means having both the confidence and the experience to recognize the sudden insight for what it is. A theory in Gestalt psychology developed by G. Wallas in the 1920s identifies four steps in creative discovery; preparation *• incubation -*• illumination -*• verification. Preparation must come first. As Louis Pasteur put it, "Chance favors only the prepared mind." Deep knowledge, usually developed over years, is followed by incubation, during which the unconscious mind mulls over the issue. Then with luck (as with Archimedes in the bathtub), there is that flash of illumination. That eureka moment often comes after sleep — because in sleep, rational thinking is turned off, and the unconscious has greater freedom. The conscious mind returns later to make the logical argument. But that verification (reasoning it all out in linear order for purposes of elaboration and proof} takes time. There is a story of a 90 MIT SLOAN MANAGEMENT REVIEW SPRING 2001 mathematician who solved a formula in his sleep. Holding it in his mind's eye, he was in no rush to write it down. When he did, it took him four months! Great insights may be rare, but what industry cannot trace its origins to one or more of them? Moreover, little insights occur to all of us all the time. No one should accept any theory of decision making that ignores insight. "Doing First" But what happens when you don't see it and can't think it up? Just do it. That is how pragmatic people function when stymied: They get on with it, believing that if they do "something," the necessary thinking could follow. It's experimentation — trying something so that you can learn. A theory for "doing first," popularized in academia by organizational-behavior professor Karl Weick, goes like this: enactment -*' selection -*• retention. That means doing various things, finding out which among ihem works, making sense of that and repeating the successful behaviors while discarding the rest. Successful people know that when they are stuck, they must experiment. Thinking may drive doing, but doing just as surely drives thinking. We don't just think in order to act, we act in order to think. Show us almost any company that has diversified successfully, and we will show you a company that has learned by doing, one whose diversification strategy emerged through experience. Such a company at the outset may have laid out a tidy strategy on the basis of assessing its weaknesses and strengths (or, if after 1990, its "core competenverbal (comprising words in linear order), the second is visual, the third is visceral. Those who favor thinking are people who cherish facts, those who favor seeing cherish ideas and those who favor doing cherish experiences. (See "Characteristics of the Three Approaches to Making Decisions.") We have for some years conducted workshops on the three approaches with midcareer managers sent by Asian, European and North American companies to our International Masters Program in Practicing Management {www.impm.org). We begin with a general discussion about the relationship between analysis, ideas and action. It soon becomes evident that practicing managers recognize the iterative and connected nature of those elements. We then ask small groups first to discuss an issue for about an hour (one of their own or else what we call a "provocative question." For example: "How do you manage customer service when you never see a customer?" or "How do you organize without structure?"), summarize their conclusions on a flip chart and report back to the full group. Next we give the groups colored paper, pens, scissors and glue. Each small group must create a collage about the issue they discussed in the thinking-first session. At the end of that second workshop, the groups view one another's images and compare "seeing first" with "thinking first" — in terms of both process and results. Finally, each group, with only a few minutes of preparation time permitted, improvises a skit to act out its issue. Again, the groups consider the results. Reactions to the approaches are revealing. Participants note that in the thinking-first workshop, the initial discussions start Thinking first" features the qualities of science planning, programming the verbal facts cies"), which it almost certainly got wrong. How can you tell a strength from a weakness when you are entering a new sphere? You have no choice but to try things out. Then you can identify the competencies that are really core. Action is important; if you insist on "thinking first" and, for example, doing formalized strategic planning (which is really part of the same thing), you may in fact discourage learning. Making Decisions Through Discussion, Coiioge ond Improvisotion Thus the three major approaches to decision making are "thinking first," "seeing first" and "doing first." They correlate with conventional views of science, art and craft. The first is mainly Characteristics of the Three Approaches to Making Decisions "Seeing first" features the qualities of art visioning, imagining the visual ideas "Doing first" features the qualities of craft venturing, learning the visceral experiences off easily enough, no matter what the mix of nationalities or work backgrounds. Participants list comments on flip charts and spontaneously use bulleted items and numbers ™ with the occasional graph thrown in. Almost no time is spent in discussing howto go about analyzing the problem. Groups quickly converge on one of several conventional analytic frameworks: cause and effect, problem and solution, pros and cons, and so on. Many participants observe that such frameworks, particuSPRING 2001 MIT SLOAN MANAGEMENT REVIEW 9 1 liiily when adopted early, blunt exploration. Quality and depth o\ AUii\ys\s may be sacrificed for process efficiency. Thinkingllrsl workshops encourage linear, rational and rather categorical iU'guinciils. All too often, the result is a wish list, with disagreements hidden in the different points. In other words, there may he less discipline in thinking first than wf believe. riiiiikiiiL; conies too easily U> most ol us. But when a group must make a picuiie, members have to reaeh consensus. Ihal requires deeper integration of the ideas. "We liad to think more to do this," a participant reported. The artistic exercise "really forces you to capture ihe essence of an issue," another added. People ask mure quesliuns in ihe seeitig-llrst exercise; they Ixxome more playful and creative. "In 'thinking first,' we foeused on ihe [iKiblems: in 'seeing first,' we Ineuscd on ihe solutions," one person said. One group believed it had agreemenl on ihe issue after the ihinkingInst workshop. Only when the picture making begim did its members realize hmv superficial thai agreement was — mine tif a cnmproniise. In contrast, when you really do see, as someone said. "The message jumps out at you." lUit lo achieve ihal. the group members have lo luui out more about one another's capabilities and collaborate uiorc closely. "I felt il became a group project, not just my project," said a patiicipant who had chosen the topic for his group. Ihe seeing-first exercise also draws out more emotions; there is more laughter and a higher energy level. This suggests that being ahle to see a trajectory — having a vision about what you are doing — energi/es people and .so stimulate.s action. In comparing ihe .seeing-first exercise with the thinking-first discussion, a parliiipaiit remarked, "We felt more liberated." The pictures maybe more ambiguous iban the words, but they are also more involving, A liet|ueiit conunent:" They invite interpretation." One particularly interesting observation about the pictures was ihal "ihe impression lasts longer." Studies indicate that we remember pictures much longer and more accurately than words. As R. Haber demonstrated in Scientific American in 1470, recall of images, even as many as 10,000 shown at one.second Intervals, is nearly 98% — a capability that may be In the "seeing first" workshop, midoareer managers find making a picture of problems required deeper integration of ideas. linked to evolution. Humans survived by learning to registei' danger and safety signals fast. Emotion, memory, recall and stimulation are powerfully bundled in "seeing first." (^ontrasl that with one comment after the thinking-first workshop: "Twenty-four hours later, we won't remember what this meant." In fact, although many participants have not made a picture since grade school, the art produced in the seeing-first workshops is t)ften remarkable. Creativity fiows freely among the managers, suggesting that they could come up with more creative ideas in their home organizations it they more often used symbols beyond words or numbers. Our multicultural groups may like the art workshop for overcoming language barriers, but groups of managers from tbe same company, country or language group have responded equally well, t^ne British participant who was working on a joint venture with an American partner found that out. He met with his U.S. counterpart a few days after the workshops. "We talked past each other for two hours," he reported. When he suggested they create a picture of their common concerns, they finally were able to connect. The improvisation skits — "doing first" — generate more spontaneity. Participants respond to one another intuitively and viscerally, letting out concerns held back in conversation and even in artwork. For example, turf battles become evident in the way people stand and talk. Humor, power, fear and anger surface. (M. Crossen and M. Sorrenti discuss improvisation at lenglh in a helpful article published in 1997 in Advances in Strategic Management.) Weick has suggested that a key aspect of effective action in organizations is the ability to remain open to signals from others, even under extreme pressure. He believes that such heedfulness, as he calls it, is a finely honed skill among groiiji improvisers such as jazz musicians. Organizations that recognize opportunities for improvisation — and hone the skills required — increase their capacity for learning. In improvisation, people have to respond with a speed that eliminates many inhibitions. "Having to just act gets rid of the fears," a participant said. 92 MM SLOAN MANAGEMENT REVIEW SPRING 2001 Anoihcr iultled, after watching a colleague play the role of a frustrated bank customer, "The output can be scarily real." Mere words, in contrast, feel more abstract and disconnected — numbers, even more so — just as the aggregations of marketing are more abstract than the experience of seUing. The .skits bring out what the words and numbers do not say — indeed, what problems they cause. "Not everything is unsayable ill words," claimed playwright Eugene Ionesco, "only the living truth." Or as Isadora Duncan, the modern-dance pioneer, insisted, "If I could say it, I wouldn't have to dance it." Thus "doing first" facilitates the dancing that is so lacking in many of today's organizations. Enough Thinking? Ihe implications for our large, formalized, thinking-obsessed organizations are clear enough: not to suspend thinking so much as put it in its place, alongside seeing and doing. Isn't it time we got past our ob.session with planning and programming, and opened the doors more widely to venturing and visioning? A glance at corporate reports, e-mail and meetings reveals that art is usually something reserved for report covers — or company walls. And when organizations separate the thinking from the doing, with the former coming from the heads of powerful formulators and the latter assigned to the hands of ostensibly docile implementers, those formulators lose the benefits of experimenting — and learning. Each approach has its own strengths and weaknesses. (See "When Each Decision-Making Approach Works Best.") "Thinking first" works best when the issue is clear, the data reliable and the world structured; when thoughts can be pinned down and discipline applied, as in an established production process. "Seeing first" is necessary when many elements have to be combined into creative solutions and when commitment to those solutions is key, as in much new-product development. The organization has to break away from the conventional, encourage communication across boundaries, bust up cerebral logjams and engage the heart as well as the head. "Doing first" is preferred when the situation is novel and confusing, and things need to be worked out. That is often the case in a new industry — or in an old industry thrown into turmoil by a new technology. Under such circumstances, complicated specifications get in the way, and a few simple relationship rules can help people move forward in a coordinated yet spontaneous manner. That suggests the advantages of combining all three approaches. In order to learn, a company group might tackle a new issue first by craft, which is tied to doing; then, in order to imagine, by art, which is tied to seeing; finally, in order to proWhen Each Decision-Making Approach Works Best •THINKING FIRST" WORKS BEST WHEN: • the issue is clear; • the data is reliable: • the context is structured; • thoughts can be pinned down: and • discipline can be applied as in an established production process. "SEEINC FIRST" WORKS BEST WHEN: • many elements have to be combined into creative solutions: • commitment to those solutions is key: and • communication across boundaries is essential as in new-product deveiopment. "DOING FIRST" WORKS BEST WHEN: • the situation is novel and confusing: • complicated specifications would get in the way; and • a few simple relationship rules can help people move forward for example, when companies face a disruptive technology. gram, by science, which is tied to thinking. In ongoing sitiui tions, art provides the overview, or vision; science specifies the structure, or plan; and craft produces the action, or energy. In other words, science keeps you straight, art keeps you intei' ested, and craft keeps you going. No organization can ilo willi out any one approach. Isn't it time, then, to move beyond our narrow thinking about decision making: to get in !oiuh, to sec another point of view? Henry Mintztxrg and Frances Westley are professors oi management at McGilt University in Montreal. Contact them al mintzber@management.mcgill.ca and westley®managemenl.mcgill.ca. Reprint 4238 Copyright ©2001 by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. All riglui rcscrvcil. 2001 MIT SLOAN MANAGEMENT REVIEW 93