11. tfden - KultivaEni teorie PovinnB Eetba: - Gerbner, G.: Toward ,,cultural indicators'" the analysis of mass mediated public message systems. In: AllenW.D. (ed.): AV Communication Review. Vol 17,no. 2, pp. 137-48. - Morgan, M., Signorielli,N.: Cultivati.oaAnalysis. Sage 1990str. 13- 34. KliEovC pojmy: medialni kultivace, kulti~acizidgerencidl, mainstreaming, rezonance. GRAPICKE PULOHY: Graf E. 1 Mainstreming 1 . , Určeno pouze pro studijní účely Toward 'cultural indicators': the analysis of mass mediated public message systems From Allen, W.I?, (ed.) (1969)AV CortrnlzlnicatiorlReview,Department of Audiovisual Instruction, Washington DC, Vol. 17, No. 2, pp. 137-48. 'I'liesystematic analysis of message content is a traditional area of study in cornrnunication research and related fields. Recent deyelopments led to a revival of interest 'in the area. But none of the new frameworks and approaches presented consider the analysis of message systems addressed to heterogeneous and anonymous publics, such as mass communications, a source af theoretical development not necessarilygenerated in other areas ofintcrcst.The purpose of this paper is to suggest an approach that justifies such developmentand can alsolead to results of practical policy sigruhcance, such asaschemeofsocialaccountingfortrendsin the compositionandstructure of mass-mediated public message systems. The approach is based on a conccptionof these message systemsas the common culture through which communities cultivate shared and public notions about facts, values, and conlingcncies of human existence. Change in the symbolic environment i awareness and cultivationofthe issues,siyles,and points a[ divcrgenctl Illat I make public contention and contest possible.The struggles for power a11i.i privilege, for participation in the conduct of affairs, for lllc rcdistribulion of resources,and for all formsof social recapition and juslicc, are increasingly sl~iftirlgfrom the older arenas to the newer spheres of public attention and control in mass-produced communications.) Selectivehabits of participation in one's ct.ilturalenvironment limit each of us to risky, and often faulty, extrapolation about the cultural experience of heterogeneous communities. Informed policy making and the valiri interpretation of social response increasingly require gcner~land cornpar- ative indicators of the prevailing climate of the man-made syniholic i environment.But knowledge of a message system,overand above that which we selectforour own informationor entertwent,and which has significance for a collectivity such as an entire cultural community, cannot be given ill the lifetime experience of any single person. What con be given is a representative abstraction from 111c colleclivcly experienced total texture of messages, relevant to certain investigative purposes. Sampling is not the major problem, and neithcr is Ihc efficient processingof largequantitiesof data, although these are important procedural considerations. Nor is great theoretical challenge involved In Lhe analysis ofmassmediamessagesforspeaficaitical,control,evduative,or policy purpox.;. The outstanding problems are the development of a generalized schcnic applicable to the investigation of the broadest terms of collectivecultivalion in differentcultural communities,and making these termssalientto clenients of existencerepresented in public message systems.Philosophers,historians, anthropologists, and others have, of course, addressed themselves to such problems before. But the tise of the institutionalized and corporately managed cultivationof collectiveconsciousnessby mass media hasgiven a new urgency and socialpoticy signihcanceto the inquiry, -5 Cultivation of public cansclousness through mass communication 'I'he 'Cultural Revolution' is not only a Chinese slogan. It is~alsoa fact of social life whenever a particular political-industrial order permeates the sphere of public message production. A change in the social bases and economic goals of message mass-production leads, sooner or later, to a transformation of the common symbolic environment that gives public meaning and sense ofdirection to human activity. The need is for a theory that can lead to the development of 'cultural indicators' taking the pulse of the nature and tempo of that transformation. Our theoretical point of departure, then, is that changes in the mass production and rapid distribution of messages across previous barriers of time, space,and social groupingbring about systematic variations in public message coptent whose full significance rests in the cultivation of collcctivc consciousness about elements of existence. (It should be noted at the outset that the terms con~rtron,shnred, public, or collective cultivation do rrot necessarily mean consensus. On the contrary, the public rccognitio~tof subcultural; class, generational, and ideological difference^ even conflictsamong scatteredgroupsof people requiressomecommon I A A A word on cultiuntion. I use the term to indicate that my primary concern in this discussion is not with information, education, persuasion, ctc., or with any kind of direct communication 'effects'. I am concerned with the collectivecontextwithin which, and in response to which, differentindividual and group selectionsand interpretations of messages take place, In thal sense, a message(ormessage system)cultivatesconscio~snessof the terms reqtlirecl forits meaningful perception. Whether I accept its 'meaning' or not, like it or not, or agree or disagree, is another problem. First l n~ustatlend to and grasp what it is about. Just how that occurs, how items of information are integrated into given frameworksof cognition, is alsoanother problem. My interest here centres on the fact that any attention arid understanding dltivates the terms upon which it is achieved,And to the considerableextent to which these terms are common to large groups, the cultivation of sharccl terms provides the basis for public inleraction. Ptiblic is another word of special significancehere, It means both a quality of information and 'an amophous social structure whose members sl~area community-of-interestwhich hasbeen produced by impersonalcon~munication Určeno pouze pro studijní účely afldcontact' (Gould and Kolb, 1964, p. 558).As a quality of information, the response. For example, it means little to know that 'Johl\ believes in Sa, awarenessthat a certain ilem of knowledge is publicly held (i.e,not only known Claus' until we also know in what culture, at what point in time, and in thcto many,brrt curnmot~lykr~ozurltltnt it is known to tnnny)makescollectivethought context of what public message systems cultivating the rcinforccment or and action possible. Such knowledge gives individuals their awareness of I inhibitionof suchbeliefs.Similarly,interpretationsofpublic opinion (i.e.rcsponses collective strength (or weakness), and a feeling of social identifilzation or toquestionselicitedin specificculturalcontats),ahd of many socialand cultural alienation As an 'amorphous social structure, etc,' a public is a basic unit of policy matters,require ~e background knowledgeofgeneral'cultural i~~diators' and requirementforself-governmentamongdiverseand scattered groupsThe similarto the economicindicatorscompiled to guide economic policy ancl the creationof both the consciousnessand the socialstructurecalled public is the social indicators proposed to informsocial policy making. result ofthe'public-making' activityapproximately named publication.'Public What distinguishes the analysis of public, mass-mediated n~essagcopinion' isactuallythe outcomeof somesortofelicitingand someprivate vie~vs systems as a socialscientificenterprise from other types of observation,corn-through Fhcir publication -as in the publication of polls. mentary, or criticism is the attempt to deal comprehensively,systematically, Publication as a general social process is the creation and cultivation of and generally rather than specificallyand selectivelyor nil /rocwith problen~s shared ways of selecting and viewing events and aspects of life. Mass of collective cultural life, This approach makes no prior assumptions about production and distribution of message systems transformsselected private pcrspectives into broad public perspectives, and brings mass publics into existence.?'hese publics are maintained through continued publication.They arc supplied with selections of information and entertainment, factsand fiction, news and fantasy or 'escape' materials which are considered important or interesting or entertaining and profitable (or all of these) in tcrms of the perspectives to be cultivated. Publication is thus the basis of community consciousness and self- government among large groups of people too numerous or too dispersed to interact faceto faceor in any other personally mediated fashion.The huly revolutionary significance of moderri mass communication is its 'public- making' ability.That is the ability to fonn historically new bases forcollective thought and action quickly,tontinuously, and pervasively across previous boundaries of time, space, and culture. ?'he tcrms ofbroadest socialinteractionarethoseavailablein the most widely shared message systems of a culture. Increasingly these are mass-produced messagesystems.?hat iswhy mass media havebeencalled the'agenda-setters' ofmodern societyWhetherone iswidely conversantwith orunaware of large portions of them, supportive or critic4 of them, or even alienated from or rebellious ofthem, the terms of the culture shape the course of the response. The approach I am suggesting is, therefore, concerned with the overall patterns and boundary conditions within which the processesof individual cognition, message utilization, and social interaction occur.The approach is directed toward answering the most general questions about the broadest terms of collective concept-formation, given in mass-produced public messagesystems.What perspectivesand whatchoicesdothey make available to entire communities over time, across cultures, and in different societies? With what kindsand proportion^ ofpropertiesand qualitiesare thesechoices \veighted?What are the underlying structures of associationinlargemessage systems that are not apparent in their separate component units? The need for 'cultural ihdicators' Wc nced to know what general termsof collectivecultivation about existence, pr-ioril~rs,values, and relationships are given in collectively shared public message systemsbeforewe can reliably interpret factsofindividualand social suchconventionally demarcated. functionsas'in formation' and 'entertainment,' or'high culture' and 'low culture'. Styleofexpression,quality of reprezntation, dsticexcellence,or thequalityof individualexperienceassociatedwith selective exposae toand participation inmasscultural activityarenot consideredaitical for this purpose. What is informative,entertaining (orboth), good, bad, or indifferentby any standard of quality are selectivejudgements applied to messagesquiteindependently from thesocialfunctiopsthey ach~allyperform in the context of large message systems touching the collective life of a whole community. Conventional and formal judgements applied to sclccted cammunications may be irrelevant to general questions about the presentation ofwhat is, what is itnportant,what is right, and what is lelnted to what in mass-produced composite message systems. Non-relevance of some conventional distinctions Justaswe-m&e noa priori assumptions about the significanceof style,quality, and subjectiveexperienceassociated with different types of messagesystems, we do not recognize the validity of conventional distinctions of function attached to non-fictional vs fictional modes of presentation. 'Fact' may be stranger than fiction, and the veracity of 'fiction' greater than that of the presumably fadual. Regardlessof verisimilitude,credibility,or what isactually 'believed' in a presentation, message systemscultivate the terms upon which they present subjects or aspects of life.There is no reason for assuming that the cultivationof these terms depends in any significantway upon the mode ofpresentation, upon agreement or disagreement with or belief or disbelief in the presentations involved, or upon whether these presentations arc presu~ablyfactual or imaginary. This does not mean, of course, that we d~ not normally attach greater credibility to a news story, a presumably factual report, a trusted source, a familiar account, than to a fairy tale or to what we regard as falseor inimical.What it does mean is that in the general process of image formation and cultivation, fact and fable play equally significant and inter-related roles. There is, however, an Important differencebetween the ways fiction and non-fiction deal with life. Reportage, exposition, explanation, argun~ent- whether based on fact, fancy, opinion, or all of these -ordinarily deal wilt^ aspects of life or thought extracted from total situations. \.Yf~ntgivcs Určeno pouze pro studijní účely 12 cultivalion Analysis lion of cultivation analysis to sludy a distinct religious subculture, the h'fcnnonitcs (Chnpfcr 8). 'l'ltc next several chapters focus on the application of cultivation ~hcoryand analysis in inlernational and intercultural contexts. In Chap- ter 9, Ron Tamborini and Jeonghwa Choi examine the role of cultural diversity in cultivation research using findings from a number of studies conduc~cdon samples of foreign-born respondents in the United States itnd mernbcrs of other cultures. Then, Bo Reimer and Karl Erik Rosen- grcn (Chrplcr 10) examine cultivation from a Swedish perspective, iipplying a life-style framework to study cultivation and human values. la Chapter 11, Mallory Wober discusses a number of different studies that altcmpt lo apply aspects of cultivation theory and analysis in Great Oritsin in a similar vein, Michael Morgan (Chapter 12) explores culti- v;ition analysis in the international setting, using findings from samples of rcspondcnts from Argentina, the People's Republic of China, and South Korea. Finally, George Gerbner, the "founding father" of cultivation theory, plcscnts an cpilogue in which he reflects on some of the advances dcacribcd in the book and develops what he sees as the most appropriate i~gcndafor cultivation analysts to Dursue. -. Overall, this collection provides a broad glimpse into the ways cultivation analysis has evolved on both micro and macro levels. We hope thc book advances the important role that the theory, methods, and iindings of cultivation analysis have played and will continue to play i l l communication rescarch, CultivationAnalysis: Conceptualization arzd Methodology MICHAEL MORGAN and NANCY SIGNORIELLI We are a niass mediated society. The mass media, especially television, play important, if often invisible and taken-for-granted, roles in our daily lives. Television sets are usually placed in prominent positions in our homes, whetherin the family room, the living room, the kitchen, the bedroom, or all of the above. Few can remember, or care to remem- ber, what life was like before television. Each day, in the average American household, a television set is turned on for over 7 hours. Individual family members watch it for about 3 hours. Children and older people watch the most; adolescents \yam11the least, but even they view an average of 20 or more hours each wcck. Although most Americans report that they read a daily news- paper, television is often cited as their major source of news and infatmation. Television has become our nation's (and increasingly the world's) rilost r:omfnon and constant learning environment, It both (selectively) mifkors and leads soqiety. Television is first and foremost, however, a storyteller- it tells most of the stories to most of the people most of the tihe, As such, television is the wholesale distributor of images and forhjthe mainstream of our popular cplture. Our children are born into Určeno pouze pro studijní účely shape, hrcus, and purpose lo the non-fictional mode of presentation isthat it is ana\y\ica\: it imp\it:.!Ei-i-l ~ c c ~ s(sucl~as collcgc studcnts in introductory communication classes) who :Ire usu;~llyno1 particul;crly representative of the larger population, -1'hls sccnarlo of classic laboratory experiments in mass comn~unica- Ilon has, wc hclicvc, influenced a great deal of popular as well us scholarly thinking about media effects. It leads to thinking about Corn* nlunicntinn (and television's messages) as foreign "objects" somchow ~nscrtcclor injected into us, as discrete, scattered "bullets" that either 1111 c>rmiss us. In contrast, c~rltivationanalysis looks at those messages ;IS an cnvironrncnt within which people live, define themselves and othcrs. :lnd devclop and maintain thcir beliefs and assumptions about soci,~lrcnlity. Othcrs huvc, of course, suggested that mass media may Involve functions and proccsscs otlicr than overt change. Forty years ago, Li\zi;r~f~ldand Merton (1948i1974) argued thq the primary impact of cxposurc to mass communication was not likely to be change, but rvaintcnancc of the status quo. Similar notions have been expressed sincu then by Glynn (1956) and Bogart (1956). Similarly, "cultivation" does not imply any sort of simple, linear "stimulus-rcsponsc" model of the relationships between media content qnd uudicnces, Rather, it implies long-term, cumulative congcquenccs of cxposurc to an cssentially repetitive and stable system of messages, not immcdiatc short-term responses or individual interpretations of contcnt, It is concerned with continuity, stabilization, and gradual shifts rnthcr thm outright chtinge. A slight but pervasive shift in the culti* vi~tionof common perspectives may not change much in individual ourlooks and behavior but may later change the meaning of those pcrspcctivcs and actions profoundly. Thus, thc usc of the term cultivatiotl for televisior.'~contribution to conceptions of social rcality is not simply a fancier word for "effacts," Most of all, it docs not imply a one-way, rnonolithicprocess. Cultivation a150 should not bc confused with "mere" reinforcement (as if reaffirms- lion nrld stability irl Ihc face of intense pressures for change were a ~riviall'citt); nor shoulcl it suggest that television viewing is simply The cultivation process is oot thought of as a unidirsct~oadii\ow (ii influence from television to audiences, hut rather part ok a col~tlnual. dynamic, ongoing process of interaction among messages and contexts. This holds true even though (and in a sense espcciolly becausei i11c hallmark of the process is either stability or slow change. Jiabils an(( styles of media exposure tend to be stable ovcr long pcrioci5 of IiIIli. (Himmelweit & Swift, 1976), and cultivation analysis 5c1:ks ir1 ~il:~n!- nate the consequences of the presence of televisian iil sizbit sr:;i~.~~( 1 life and environments. It is designed to understand g r n d i ~ ~ ~ .i.rop-rc Ti]\ shifts and transformations in the way generations are snclallzed IIll;i short-term, dramatic changes in individuals' belicfs or bcha-dtors). successive generations become enculturated into t!le tnslnstieam 4-I! television's version of the world, the former traditionul di.;ttoctioni become blurred. Cultivation thus means the steady elitrcnchnlent c~i Mainstream orientations in most cases and the syslemalic bur aln~osl imperceptible modification of previous orientations in others; i n othcr wards, affirmation for the believers and indoctrination lor the deviants Procedures Used in Cultivation Analysis Cultivation analysis begins with aontent (rnessagc system) analysis: identifying and assessing the most recurrent and stablc patterns of television content (the consistent images, portrayals, and values that cut across-most types of programs). There are many critical discrepancies between the world and the world as portrayed on televisioe. Thc shape and contours of the television world rarely match objcc~ivcreality. though they often do match dominant ideologies and values. Findings from systematic analyses of television's cuulcnt are tiil:a used to formulate questions about people's conceptions of SOI:I~!r ~ r l ' ~ i j . . Some of the questions are scmiprojective. sonic IISC 7 1 is:12-c-rcr format, and other simply measure beliefs, opinior~c,i i ~ ' i t u i i . - r , t It-- haviors. Using standard techniques of survey rnethodol12g$.v ' - ';a: . - 1 3 posed to samples (national probability, rcgionoi. - . -* 1 1 dren, adolescents, or adults. Secondary rlnal:~x-s' : I ;,.. - a $ 1 TC;\ surveys (for example, the National Opinic711~:-cF\::!~~II i S.;it- -:.ll,!$)~ Social Surveys) have often been used wnen t.1;y iik"-'l.r~,?c;i:I.:o\;t?nS l n ? t relate to identifiable aspects of the television world as well atstckl?i7~:?!firl viewing, Určeno pouze pro studijní účely I 70 Conceptualization and Methodology I I'clcvision vicwing is usually assessed by asking how much time the rcspondcnl watches television on an average day. Since amount of vicwing is sccn as in relative terms, the determination of what consti- tutes light, mcdium, a ~ dheavy viewing is made on a sample-by-sample busis, using as closc to a thrce-way split of hours of self-reported daily tclcvision vicwing as possible. What is important is that there are basic dificrcnccs in vicwing levels, not the actual or specific amoust of vicwing. 'I'hc qucslions posod to respondents do not mention television, and thc respondents' awareness of the source of their information is seen as irrelevant. The resulting relationships, if any, between amount of view- ing and thc tcqdency to respond to these questions in the terms of the dominant and repetitive facts, values, and ideologies of the World of lclcvision (again, other factors held constant) iliurninate television's con~ributionto viewers' conceptions of social reality. 'The obscrvable empirical evidence of cultivation is likely to be rnodcst in tcrms of its absolute size. Even light viewersmay bewatching up to 7 hours of television a week; a trivial, and demographically cclcctic, haridful say they do not watch at all. But, if we argue that the mcssagcs are stable, that the medium is virtually ubiquitous, and that it is accumulated exposure that counts, then almost everyone should be affectcd, regardless of how much they watch. Even light viewers may watch a substantial amount of television per week and in any case live in thc same cultural environment as heavy viewers; what they do not gct through television can be acquired indirectly from others who do watch more. It is clear, then, that the cards are stacked against finding cvidencc of cultivation. Therefore, the discovery of a systematic pattern of cvcn small but pervasive differences between light and heavy view- crs may indicate far-reaching consequences, Accordingly, we: should not dismiss what appear to be small effects, bccause small effects may have profound consequences. For example, a slight but pcrvasive (e.g., generational) shift in the cultivation of common perspectives may alter the cultural climate and upset the balance of social and political decision making without necessarily changing observable behavior. A single percentage point difference in ratings is worth many millions of dollars in advertising revenue. It takes but a few degrees' shift in the average global temperature to have an ice A range of 3 perccnt to 15 percent margins (typical of most dirfcrcncesbctwecn light and heavy viewcrs) in a large and otherwise MICHAEL.MORGAN and NANCY SIGNORIELL1 21 stable field often signals a landslide, a market takeover, or an epidemic, and it certainly tips the scale of any closely balanced choice or decision. Kariations in Cultivation We have noted that cultivatioq is not a unidirectiohal flow of influ- ence from television to addience, but part of a continual, dynamic, ongoing process of interaction between messages and contexts. In sonle cases, those who watch more television (the heavy viewers) are more likely -in all or most subgroups- to give the "television answers." But, in many cases the patterns are more compleq. Television viewing usually relates in different ways to different gtoups' life situations and world views. Cultivation is both dependent on and a manifestation of the extent to which television's imagery dominates viewers' sources of information. For example, personal interaction makes a difference. Parental co- viewing patterns and orientations toward television can either increase (Gross & Morgan, 1985) or decrease (Rothschild & Morgan, 1987) cultivation among adolescents; also, children who are more integrated into cohesive peer or family groups are relatively immune to cultivation (Rothschild, 1984). Direct experience also plays a role. The relationship between amount of viewing and fear of crime is strongest among those who live in high crime urban areas (a phenomenon called resonance, which in everyday reality and television provides a double dose of messages that "reso- nate" and amplify cultivation). Further, relationships between amount of viewing and the tendency to hold exaggerated perceptions of vio- lence are more pronounced within those real-world demographic sub- grovps (minorities) whose fictional counterparts arc more frequently victimized on television (Morgan, 1983). Television viewing usually relates in different but consistent ways to different groups' life situations and world views. A major theoretical and analytical thrust of mady recent cultivatibn analyses has been directed toward the determination of the conditional processes that enhance, diminish, or otherwise mediate cultivatipn. Many rcscnrchers are trying to figure out what types of people are most vulnerable to television's messages, in what specific substantive areas, and why. This typd of research, well represented in this volume, investigates which subgroups are more or less susceptible to television on which issues, Určeno pouze pro studijní účely 22 Conrcp~u;rlizalionand Methodology MICHAEL MORGAN and NANCY SIGNORIELL1 23 :inti hits signjficantly enlarged our understanding of the more subtle arid funclamcntirl conscqucnccs of living with television. -1 llcr~: i ~ r ta ~ f i ~ l evariety of factors and processes that prpduce systcn~nticiincl Lhcurctically meaningful variations in cultivation pat- lcrns. C)nc proccss, I~owcvcr,slands out, both as an indicator of differ- rn~ialvul~icrabilityand as a general, consistent pattern representing one of' the most profound consequcnces of living with television: main- strcilrning. Our culturc consists of many diverse currents, somc weak, some strong. SOITICflow iri the same general directions, some at crosscurrents. Yct thcre is a dominant set of cultural beliefs, vabes, and practices, in somc ways at the core of all the other currents, and in some ways surrounding them. This domirkant current is not simply the sum total of all the crosscurrents and subcurrents; rather, it is the post general and stahlc (though not static) mainstream, representing the broadkst and must common dimensions of shared meanings and assumptions. It ultimately dcfincs all the other crosscurrents and subcurrents. Because of ~clcvision'sunique role in our society, it is obvious that television can and should be seen as the primary manifestation of the mainstreaq of oi~rculture. Transcending historic barriers of literacy and mobility, television has hccomc a primary, common source of everyday culture of an otherwise hc~crogcneouspopulation, Television provides, perhaps for the first time sincc preindustrial religion, a strong cultural link between the clircs and all othcr publics. It provides a shared daily ritual of highly compelling arid informative content for millions of otherwise diverse people in all regions, ethnic groups, social classes, and walks of life, Tclcvision provides a relatively restricted set of choices for a virtually unrcstrictcd variety of interests and publics; its programs elimirlate hountlnrics of age, class, and region and tire designed by commercial ncccssity to bc watchcd by ncarly everyone, Tllc mainstream can thus be thought of as a relative commonality of ou~lnoksand valucs that heavy exposure to the features and dynamics 1 1 i rllc tclcvisipn world tends to cultivate. Mginstreamit~gmeans that ,., ..... .~::i~ir:gmay absorb or override differences in perspectives and I~c~l:~i~r~:iI):,\! :~r~linarilystem fr'rc?n~othcr factors and influences, In other \vol.~.l.~.~ti;'~t-rc*f~ccsl'ou~din the rcsponses of different groups of view- er's, differences that usually are associated with the varied cultural, social, and political characteristics of thesc groups, arc diminished or even absent from the responses of heavy viewers in rheso same groups. As a process, mainstreaming represents the theoretical elaborat~on and empitical verification of the assertion that television'crillivatcs common perspectives. It represents a relative homogenization, an ab- sorption of divergent views, and a convergence of disparate viewers. Former and traditional distinctions (which flourished, in part. through the diversity provided by print culture) become blurred as successive generations and groups become enculturated into television's version of the world. Through the process of mainstreqming, teletrision has in essence become the true 20th century melting pot of tlla American people. In summary, the theory of cultivation is an attempt to understand and explain the dynamics of television as a distinctive feature of our age. I1 is not a substitute for, but a complement to, traditional approaches to media effects research concerned with processes yore applicable to other media. Designed primarily for television and focusing on ics pervasive and recurrent patterns of representation and viewing, cultiva- tion analysis concentrates on the enduring aad common consequenccs of growing up with and living with television: the cultivation of stable, resistant, and widely shared assumptions, images, and conceptions reflecting the institutional characteristics atld interests of the medium itself and the larger society. Television has become the common sym- bolic eilvdronment that interacts with most of the things we think and do, Therefore, understanding its dynamics can help develop ahd main- tain a sense of alternatives and independence essenlial for self-direction and self-government in the television age, Ti1r: Battles The methodology and findings of message-system analysis, particu- larly in the area of violence, were the focus of a number ol colloquies in the 1970s. Most of these stemmed from critiques bv inclustry re- searchers and involved differences over definitions (What is violence? What is a violent act? How is violence unitized? elc.). They also addressed concerns over sample size, reliability, validity, and numerous related issues (see Blank, 19773,1977b; Coffin c ! Tuchman, 1972-73a, Určeno pouze pro studijní účely ' I ('oi~~-cll[uitli~.ntionand Methodology MlCHAEL hlORGAN and NANCY SIGNORIELLl . - 1072-73b; Iilccy et al., 1972-73a, 1972-73b; Gerbner ct al., 1977b, 1077~). Soon iiflcr thc first cultivation results were published (Gerbner & Gross, 107h),cqltivalion analysis became the focal point of the Cultural fndic;~tnrsproject. Mcssagc system analysis continued to be conducted tach ycar, but the industry's critiques abated. The lull in the Cultural Indicirtors (CI) storm was brief, howcver, and a period of intense debate ovcr cullivntian soon began. Onc oI' thc first critiques of cultivation (Newcomb, 1975) in part was hascd on somc supposcd differences between a quantitative and quali- tativc approach and again reflected differences in definitional perspec- livcs (scc also Gcrbner Sc Gross, 1979). Around the same time, research conducted by the Independent Broadcasting Aufhority in the United Kingdon~wi~srcportcd as failing to replicate what was called the "paranoid effcct of television" (Wober, 1978).This research, however, ciln hc sccn more as serving to point out the cultural and institutional differences betwecn the United Statcs and Great Britain than as dis- confirmation of cultivation (Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, & Signorielli, 1979; sce also Neville, 1980; Wobcr, 1979). The early 1980s brought a new, massive, unprecedented round of attacks, rcsponscs, and rejoinders. The regular publication of cultiva- tion fivdings in several annual Violence Profiles (Gerbner et al., 1977a; Ccrbncr ct al., 1978; Gerbner, Gross, Signorielli, Mbrgan, & Jackson- Ijccck, 1979) had set the stage for the onslaught that was to come, And cornc it did. Fierce, prolonged battles (occasionally acrimonious and vagucly ad hominem at times) consumed hundreds of pages of scholarly and research journals; their repercussions were vividly felt at academic confcrcnces and the controversies even spilled over into such popular ~ncdiaas Time magazine. The conflicts grew harsher and louder, and the rivcrs ran rcd with dead data and mutilated statistical techniques. Why all the fighting? It is not possible to review all the arguments and countcrargurnents here (but see Doob & Macdonald, 1979; Hughes, 1980; Gcrbner ct al., 1980a, 1980b; Hirsch, 1980b; Gerbner et al., 19SIc; Hirsch, 1980a, 1981b; Gerbner et a]., 1981b; Hirsch, 1981a; Gcrbner et al., 198la-preferably in that order-for a relatively com- plcce account). Among many other charges and countercharges, it is safe lo say that the m a j ~ rissues revolved around questions of spuriousness ;inrl controls. A11 al iiround the same time, Hirsch, Hughes, the Cultural Indicators rcscnrch tciun, and others wcre independently finding that many of [he relationships reported earlier using the National Opin~onResearch Center's (NORC) General Social Surveys looked different under mul- tiple controls. For the most part, cultivation analyses had been i~nplc- menting controls by examining associations between amount of view- ing and attitudes within subgroups, one at a time, That is, [he rcsulrs were presented for males, older people, those with less education. and so on, separately. On reanalysis of those same data, it was raund th;~t miiltiple controls (i.e., cbntrolling for age, sex, education, erc.. all ,it once), tended to reduce or completely eliminate those rclationsliips. Yet, we found that thc absence of an overall relationsh~punder multiple controls did not mean. that there were not nonspurious and theoretically meaningful associations within specific subgroups (Gerir- ner et al., 1980b), This discovery had profoudd conceptdal and analyt* ical implications for cultivation theory, and ultimqtely led to importadt refinements and enhancements, The most central of these was the idca of mainstreaming, first noted in research relating to conceptions about sex roles (Signorielli, 1979); it has been found since in more and more substantive areas, including interpersonal mistrust (Mean World Syn- drome), alienation/gloom (Anomie), political orientations, and marly other issues. In sum, these battles have been characterized as everything from "healthy scholarly exchanges" to "scathing exposees" to "vicious and unprofessional spats." They were challenging, unpleasant, and in some whys; 'fun. They pttest to the importance of cultivation theory in the discipline and to the fact that cultivation analysis has not been a static research approach, but one that has evolved and developed in numerous ways, making it not only more cornpleq and intricate but also more dynamic and intriguing. Arguments concerning cultivation analysis did produce significant new issues and questions, many of which are ad- dressed in the chapters in this book. Current Issues in CultivationAnalysis AS cultivation analysis has evolved it has continucd to raise more and more questions about underlying processes and broader conse- quences. There is general (though not univetsaj) acceptance of the conclusion that there are statistical relationships between how much people watch television and what they think and do; thera is far less consensus on a host of related q~lestiorisand probl~rns.Space does not Určeno pouze pro studijní účely _ 11 C c~nccplui~liralionand Melhodology MICHAEL MORGAN and NANCY SIGNOKIEI~LI 27 ~)crrr~il;I ~ L I I Iand comprehensive review of all of these questions and rolcvant rcscarcll, but this section outlines some of the major issues that l~avccvolvcd from cultivation analysis and which numerous indepen- tlcnt investigators arc pursuing. tlow does cultivation occur? What are the psychological processes and cognitive mechanisms that best explain the ways in which heavy viewers incorp~rntetelevision content into their concdptions of social reality? What principles of learning, If any, are relevant? These have turned out to be challenging and complex questions. While some conceptual and empirical answers are beginning to emerge (see D'Alessio, 1987; Haw- kins Pr Pingree, 1980, 1982; Hawkins et at., 1987: Hawkins & Pingree, this volume; Pingree, 1983), much work in this area remains to be done. What demographic subgroups are more likely to show evidence of cultiva- tion? As previously noted, cultivation patterns are zarely uniform ilCrQSS all subgroups in a sample. Many differential conditional associations spggest mainstreaming, in which the heavy viewers of a subgroup that is relatively "out" of the mainstream tend to express views that match those of their counlerparts (Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, & Signorielli, 1980b, 1982, 1984; Morgan, 1986; see also Perry, 3987, for an account of some possible statistical artifacts). Other patterns of results sometimes suggest "resonance," in which cultivation is enhanced among those for whom a certaio issue has some special salience. More work needs to be done to determine if certain socio-demographic groups are cossistently and sys- tematically more likely to be vulnerable to television, and to explore why they are (Morgan, 1983). How is cultivation mediated by interpersonal and family relations? Television viewing is often a family activity; farnily members may influence each others' interpretation of television content in direct and intentional as well as indirect and uhintentional ways, and the nature of the family's inter- actions apart from the television viewing context can also relate to varia- tions in vulnerability to cultjvation (Gross & Morgan, 1985; Rothscl~ild & Morgan, 1987), Again, differences in children's levels of peer integra- tion also indicate differences in susceptibility to cultivation (Rothschild, 1984),and such factors as "sdciabilily" may plan important roles (Geiger, 1987).Overall, further research is needed to understand how the dynamics of interpersonal interaction mediate cultivation. What are the levels of cultivation? Explorations of cultivation have suggested that there are important differencesbetween the cultivation of(1) concep- tioqs of the "faCts of life," such as estimates of how many people are involved in violence, how many people work in different occupations, and so on (sometimes referred lo as first-ordercubivdlion), and (2) the c.ul~iv:~iionnf more global extrapolations from those facts, such as degree of interpersonal mistrust, of some political orientations (a kind of second- order cultivation) (Gerbner et al., 1986; vawkins, et al., 1987). Similarly, there are potential differences between societal~le~elbeliefs and personal- level beliefs; for example, images of the amount of violence in society may or may not be related to perceived chances of personal victimization, or fear in one's own home or neighborhood (Geiger, 1987; Gerbner et al., 1981a; Tyler, 1984), Finally, there may be differences between the culti- vation of various attitudes and their speoific behavioral manilestations (Morgan, 1987). Conceptions of social reality have branched off in vari- ous directions, and future work should provide greater specificity of the level(s) at which such conceptions apply. What is the role of personal experience in cultivalion? It is a truism that media qffects will be greater for issues about which we have less direct personal experience. But this heed not be the case; it is indeed possible for one to assume that his or. her own experience is a(ypica1, apd that the television version or the cultural stereotype is more accurate, In any case, the issue of personal experience is closely tied in with the issue of perceived reality of television content, Studies of these issues have produced some useful findings, but for the most part they have only focused on violence (Geiger, 1987; Potter, 1986; Elliott & Slater, 1980; Slater & Elliott, 1982; Weaver and Wakshlag, 1986). Research should be undertaken to explore the implications of both personal experience and perceived reality as they might relate to other subslantive areas besides violence. How do viewers' orientalions toward television influence cultivation? By "orientations toward television," we mean such phenomena as "active" VS. "passive" viewing, "selective" viewing, uses, and gratifications, "in- volvement" with television, and conscious interpretations of television content 3 s well as perceived reality. All these have been the focus of numerous studies of cultivalio~~(Carveth & Alexander, 1985; Gunter & Furnham, 1984; Perse, 1986; Perse, this volume; Rouner, 1984; Robin et al., 1988; Wakshlag et al., 1983). Some of these issues need more work in terms of conceptualization and operationalization, and too often they are used as independent variables along with amount of viewing; they are more likely to offer further understanding of cultivation when they are implemented as interveningvariables in order to explore the within-group conditional associations they might produce. What are the roles of specific programs and genres in c~ltivation?A frequent concern raised about cultivation analysis is the focus on overall mount of viewing without regard to exactly what programs people watch. Culti- vation theory insists that (he message elements that are likely to lead to cultivation (as opposed to other effects) are those that cut across most programs and are inescapable for heavy viewers, and therefore how much is far more important than whal. Many researchers, hovever, believe thal Určeno pouze pro studijní účely 28 Conceptualization and Methodology cultivatiop pust be traced to exposure to specific types of programs, For example, some have explored the cultivation potential of soap operas (Buerkel-Rolhfuss Sr Mayes, 1981; Carveth & Alexander, 1985; Persc, 11)86), family programs (Buerkel-Rothfuss el al,, 1982), or have parti. tioned cullivation relationships according to specific viewing patterns (Hawkins & Pingree, 1981; Potler, 1986). Yet, except when VCRs are used to time-Shift, viewing is limited by what is on at a particular time. More specifically, in the case of network programming, situation comedies usually are seen in the early evening while action adventure programs are shown during the late evening (Signorielli,l986). Certainly, there may be some heavy viewers who only watch news, or sitcoms, etc, But by and large, lo the extent that common economic imperatives and production influences mean that mqst programming conveys complementary cultural values, and if most heavy viewers indeed see rnpre of everythidg, +en idiosyncratic viewing patterns are less relevant. Most of all, while specific programs and genres may certainly have effects, those effects are indioa- tions of cllllivalion only if they occur at the aggregate level, Researchers who wish to explore genre-specific relationships should not neglect to consider overall viewing as an important theoretical constrdct and as an empirical measure. How and what do other media cu!tivate? Little cultivation work has explicitly compared the relative contributions of television and other media, In terms of political self-designation, a greater tendency to describe oneself as moderate was associated with greater viewing, but not with newspaper reading or radio listening (Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, B Signorielli, 1982); movies, music, and magazines (Preston, this volume) offer important avenues for future cultivatioh analysis. Extensive typologies of media exposure profiles would allow for more precise understanding of the degree of interaction among various media. How will new technologies influence cultivation? Television is not the'same institution it was when the Cultural Indicators project began, when com- rnercial*network,over-the-air broadcasting essentially had the medium to itselr. But more channels do not necessarily mean more diversity, espe- cially given increasing concenlration of ownership and the relatively small number of production companies. In fact, Morgan and Rothschild (1983) found greater cultivation among cable-viewing adolescenls. The rapid proliferaljon of VCRs may have similar implications for cultivation (Dobrow, this vblume). Does cultiv~tionoccur in other countries? The extension of cultivation analysis Lo other countries asd cultures represents a major development in the approach (Morgan, this volume). Just as ;cultivationpatterns are no1 uniform across subgroups in the United States, they vary tremendously across differen1cultural contexts. As evidence from cross-cultural repli- MICHAEL MORGAN and NANCY SlGNORIELLI 29 cations of cullivation continues to accumulate, it will be necessary to begin to analyze and explain what accounts for these differences. Interna- tional cultivation analysis will a190 need to differentiate between the contributions of d~mesticand imported programming (Pingree 6r Haw- kins, 1981). These are just some of the conceptual issues that have becn develop- ing from cultivation analysis. In addition, there are many methodolog- ical issues that cultivation analyses must continue to explore. These include the appropriateness of the samples used (tao niany studies present data from vndergraduate communication majors, which raises severe problems of external validity); problerns of question order and sensitization of respondents (too often respondents are clued in to the fact that the survey they're filling out is about the effects of television); and the use of simultaneous multiple controls, both overall and within groups, coupled with persistent problems of neglected potential sources of spuriousness and curtailment of vdriance within groups (Carlson, 1985; Hawkins & Pingree, 1982).This list could be ezpanded consid- erably; the point is that these methodological concerns are as complex, critical, and challenging as are the more qubstantive issues previously described. The F u l f ~ eof CultivationAnalysis As the above list demonstrates, our knowledge of the cultivation process is by no means complete. A great deal of work is underway on exploring the conceptual and methodological implications of cultiva- tion on numerous levels, from the micro to ths macro. The development of oross-cultural studies (including an ongoing global-level interna- tional cooperative venture) will provide even more information about the generalizability of cultivation as a phenomenon. New technologies, alternative delivery systems, new program gen- res, and changes in institutional structures may have varied conse- quences for cultivation theory in the future. Predicting the future is always dangerous, but we believe that the likely-to-continue decline in the dominance of network broadcasting will not reduce the relevance of cultivation analysis, All current (and historical) indications point to increasing concentration and interdependence in media industries, im- itation of successful formats and styles, and greater competition for the Určeno pouze pro studijní účely .70 Cor~ceplunlizationarld hfelhodology MICHAEL MORGAN and NANCY SltiNORlELLl 31 I>ro;ldcsl, most atlractive, mainstream audiences. These are conditions t11itI cnhance, rather than fragment, the cultivation of standardized V ~ I ~ U C Sand ideologies. The h;mn is likely to be the same, even if the choir is larger. l'llcre itrc miihy additional substantive areas to address. We have barely scratched the surface of the 20-year archives of message system :~nalysis.Whilc cultivation analysis has already clearly moved well bcyond violence, the range of issues and dimensions that may be fruitful arcas for cultivation analysis will continue to expand, In sum, thc chaptcrs in this volume makc it abundantly clear that whatever advances we are making, cultivation analysis will lead to m o r c and morc questions about the role of television in our lives, We rxpcct cullivation research to continue to flourish and to provoke lively controversy about media effects and how to study them. While much work has been done, there is far more to do. The arguments, findings, and issues raised in this book arc intended to contribute to the further development, elaboration, and refinement of the theory and melhod of cultivation analysis. Hejcrerr ccs Raker. R.K.,& Ball, S,J. (Eds,). (1969). Violence in themedia. Staffreport to the National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence. Washington, DC: U.S. Govern- ment Printing Office. Blank, D. M. (1977a). Final commehts on the violence profile. Journal of Broadcasling, ?1(3), 287-296. Illank. D. M.(1977b). The Gerbner violence profile. Journal of Broadcasting, 21(3), 273-279. llogarl, 1,. (1956). Tlrc age of television: A study of viewing habits and the impact of rcs/cvisronon American life. New York: Ungar. Ducrkcl-Rothruss, N. L., Greenberg, B. S., Atkin, C. K., & Neuendorf, K. (1982), 1.cnrning aboul the family from television. Journal of Communication, 32(3), 191-201, Il~icrkcl-Rothfuss,N. I.., I% Mayes, S. (1981). Soap opera viewing: The cultivation effect. .Iurrrnolo.f Communication, 31(3), 108-115. Carlson, J. M. (1985). Prime tinre law eirforcemellt. New York: Praeger. C,rrvctli, R., Alexnndcr, A. (1985). Soap opera viewing molivarions and the clrlti~~atlon procegs. .lorrrnnl o/Broadcasting and Elcctrorrichfcdia, 29, 259-273. ('(.!Tin. T. E.. ~kTuchman, S. (1972-73a). A question of validity: Some cornmenls on "~\rpii:s, nrailgcs, and Ihc kitchen sink." Jorrrnal of Brondcpsting. 17(1), 31-32. ~~:1~11.f. li., B Tuchinar~,S. (1972-73b). Rating television programs for riolencc: A .,. !; \ : I < I ~vr ~ I V Csurvcys. Jorlrnal of Broadcasting, 17(1),3-20. !) ' \.h c;1.4, 1) ( IUP7. May). A psyclropl~vsiologicalmodel of lelcvision viewi/rg, Paper ['rc'clll~ll It; thc intcrdnlional Communication &socialion, Montreal. Doob, A. N., & Macdonald, G.E. (1979). Televiqion viewing and fear of Victimization: IS [herelationship causal?Journal o~Personalityand SocitrlPsychologb, 37(2), 170-179. Eleey, M., Gerbner, G., & Signorielli (Teiiesco), N. (1972-73a). Validity indeed! Jo~rrnal of Broadcasting, 17(1), 34-35. Elecy, M., Gerbner, G., & Signorielli (Tedesco), N. (1972-73b). Apples, oranges, and the kitchen sink: An analysis and guide to the comparison of 'violence ralings.'J~;urnalcl/' Brpadcasting, 17(1),21-31. Elliott, W. R., & Slater, D. (1980). Exposure, experience, and perceived TV reality for adolescents. Journalism Quarterly, 57(3), 409-414, 431. Gelger, S, F. (1987, May). Social reality and mass media cflecrs: Source dcpendencie.r, issues relevance, and levels of signficance. Paper presented to the Inlernntional Com- munication Association, Montreal. Gerbner, G. (1969). Dimensions of violence iq Lelevislon drama. In R. K. Baker & S. J. Ball (Eds.), Violence in the media. Staff report \o the National Cornmission on kh? Causes and Prevention of Violence (pp. 311-340). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Gerbner, G. (1972). Violence and lelevision drama: Trends and symbolic fdnclions. In G. A, Comstock & E, Rubinstein (Eds.), Television and social behavior, i'ol. I, Content drrd control (pp. 28-181), Washington, DC: U.S. Goverrlment Prinling Office. Gerbner, GI,& Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The violence prdfile. Journal o j Comniunication, 26(2), 173-199, Gerbper, G., & Gross, L. (1979). Editorial response: A reply to Newcomb's "Humanistic critique." Communication Research, 6(2), 223-230. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Eleey, M., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffried-Fox, S.,LPL Signorielli, N. (1977a). TV violence profile no. 8; The highlights. Journal of Communicalion, 27(2), 171-180. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Elety, M., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., & Signorielli, N. (1977b). One more time: An analysis of the CBS 'Final comments on the violence p~ofile.'Journalof Broadcasting, 21(3), 297-303. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Eleey, M., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., & Signorielli, N. (1977.~2_The Gerbner violence profile-An dnalysis of the CBS report. Journal of Broadcasting, 21(3), 280-286. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Hoover, S., Morgan, M., Signorielli, N., & Wuthnow, R. (1983). Relinion and televisiorr. Philadelphia: The Apnenberg School of Communicalions, uni;ersity of Pennsylvania. Oerbne~,G., Gross, L., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., & Signuriclli, N.11!J7R) Cultural indicators: Violence profile no, 9.Joctrna[ ~~Cornhtunicalion,28(3), 176-207. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N, (1979). On Wober's 'Televised violence and oaranoid oercaotion: The view from Great Eritai~l.'F11i.iic l.:!linion Quarterly, 43(i),123-1i4. Gerbner, G., Gross, L,, Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N,(1950a). Tr?rnc nf!dltir?i\,ilCC!I!P:FI\IS oh cultivation analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44(3j. 408-6 111. . . Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (IOSUb!. TIIP .!i'illns[lcci?ii~c: r:f America: Violence proflle no. 11. journal of Conrmulricnriur~,. i t t i : l : !!!->I.. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N.(I$h!Gc. .:!)'i!lI. ri!p!r'l,# rr~rrii!re family: Images and impact. Paper for the National Rcsearub Fonlrn !In i- zlir~iyIssll-S. White House Conference on Families. Getbner, G., Gross, L.,Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N,(IOHla). l'lsat r~)*)l' to I-ilrsch. Communicarion Research, 8(3), 259-280. Určeno pouze pro studijní účely Určeno pouze pro studijní účely Rothhcltilil. N.,& Morgan, M, (1987). Cohesion and control: Adolescents' relationship# wrfl~percnls as mediators of television. Journal ofEorly Adolescence, 7(3), 299-314, Kouncr, D.(IY84), Active television viewing and the cultivation hypolhesis, Journa~isrn Qrmrrerly, 61(1). 168-174. Kutlin. A. M., Perse, E.M., Pc Taylor, D. S.(1988). A melhodological investigation bt cultiv,~Lion.Commroricariort Research, 15(2), 107-334. Si~nc,riclli,N. (11)79,April). Televisiorr's contriburion to sex role socialization. Pnpcr prcseritcd to the Scvcnth Annual Telecommunications Policy Research Conference, S ~ Y L ( I P .PA. Stgnoriclli, N. (1986). Seleclivc lelevision viewing: A limited possibility. Journal of C'or~rnrc~trtro~in,~,36(3), 64-75. Signoriclli, N. (1989). Television and conceptions about sex-roles: Maintaining cotlven, Divergent Psychological Process~!siji lionalily and thc status quo. Sex Roles, 21(5/6), 337.356, Slater. D.,~r I:tlio~~,W. R. (1982). Television's influence on social reality. Quarterly Constructing Social Renlity fiolflibfass Jourttal of .Tpecclr. 68(1), 69-79. 'I:vicr,T. R. (1984). Asse~singthe risk of crime victimization: The integration of personal Media Content victimi~ationexperience and socinlly lransmilted information. Journal ofSocia1Issue$, 10(1). 27-38. ,, . Wnkshl;~g,J. J., Val, V., Pc Tamborini, R.(3983). Selecling crime drama and apprehension about crime. Human Comn~unicationResearch, 10(2), 217-242. Wcavcr. J., & Wakshlag, J. (1986). Perceived vulnerability to crime, criminal victimiza* lion experience, and televisiou viewing. Journal ofBroadcasring andElec~ronicMedia, 30(2), 141-158. Wobcr. J. M. (1978). Televised violence and paranoid perception: The view from Great Britain. Ptrblic Opinion Quarterly, 42(3), 315-321. \Vobcr. J. M.(1979). Televised violence nnd viewcrs' perceptions of reality: A reply lo criticisms of some British research. Public Opinioh Quarterly, 43(2), 271.273, ROBERT P, HAWKINS and SUZANNE PINGREE The cultivation hypothesis, the proposal that television's presentation of social reality influences the social reality beliefs of its viewers, has been characterized by two research orientations: (1) irtvestigations Into the simple existence or robustness of the effect, and (2) its integration with sociological theories in work examining group and soctal setting differences in the effect. We have argued at various tirrles (Nawkins & Pingree, 1980, 1982; Hawkins, Pingrce, cYc Adler, 1987; l'mgrec, 15;8?) for research into the p~ychologicalprocesses that nlay uridelilc culil~2- tion effects. That is, how is it that watching televisiu~tconlri~ute5:I> certain social reality beliefs and not others? What arc till: p7,r tlclogczi processes that lead individuals to construct their era.:! f r (.I-?' ia.i!tr" :: ways that mirror both the facts and the ostensic!:~ -- 3;j;:i. <.; ! - t - - vision's social reality? We think that these questions are crucial tt:, rfis.:ar:j I-.' ; :-.zl:l as well as to our understanding of social rcaiiiy :I,?; GI-;J. i:. 7 . 1--nk83 such questions can be successfully addressed: I if::,,'-;-I: ;7 1 ' ; I '?: !'I> c of being dismissed as a label for a single C V I ~ S ~ ; ? ~ :t, -.1:.vi510n time! masquerading as a program of research, aild 6s an !clcul~~gicalposit~vrl more than as a focus for social scientific resen~ch.'i'1.c ~robl5i:i1: :.l,I Určeno pouze pro studijní účely Abortion - . Light Medium Heavy 1.1 1. Light Medium Heavy Určeno pouze pro studijní účely