NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PERGAMON Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1091-1113 ■ www.clscvicr.com/locatc/ncuropsyctiologia Cognitive representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia Laurel J. Buxbaumab'*, Angela Siriguc, Myrna F. Schwartz ad, Roberta Klatzkye a Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Korman 213, 1200 W. Tabor Road, Philadelphia, PA 19141, USA b Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA c Institut des Sciences Cognitives, Lyon, France d Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA e Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Received 12 July 2001; received in revised form 6 November 2002; accepted 6 November 2002 Abstract Ideomotor apraxia (IM) is a disorder of skilled action characterized by spatiotemporal errors in pantomiming object use and in using objects. Recent evidence suggests that at least some patients with IM may exhibit particular deficits in forming hand configurations appropriate for object use. Sirigu etal. [Cortex 31 (1995) 41] reported anapraxic who positioned her hand inappropriately when attempting to use objects in accordance with stored knowledge of object-specific manipulation, but in reaching tasks could grasp the same objects appropriately in response to their structure. To this point, however, apraxics' ability to respond to functional and structural attributes of objects has not been empirically assessed. We investigated the hypothesis that patients with IM (n = 9) due to left inferior parietal damage would be impaired in producing and recognizing hand postures associated with familiar objects, indicating deficient memorial representations for object-specific hand postures. In contrast, we predicted relatively unimpaired ability to produce and recognize appropriate hand postures with novel objects, indicating integrity of "on line" spatiomotor procedures coding hand position in response to object structure. Apraxics' performance was contrasted with 10 healthy controls and 8 brain-lesioned non-apraxics. Consistent with our predictions, the apraxics responded abnormally with familiar objects but normally in recognizing hand postures appropriate for novel objects. In addition, performance with objects calling for a prehensile response (pinch or clench) was superior to that with objects evoking a non-prehensile response (palm or poke). These data suggest degradation of inferior parietally-mediated representations of the precise hand postures associated with familiar objects. Furthermore, they are consistent with possible over-reliance upon dorsal system procedures for calculating precision and power grip in response to object structure. © 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Apraxia; Praxis; Ideomotor; Hand posture; Gesture; Affordance; Grasping; Action; Object use; Tool use; Object structure; Object function 1. Introduction Patients with ideomotor apraxia (IM) make spatiotemporal errors in pantomiming skilled movements associated with object use, and, often, in actual use of objects [2]. For example, an apraxic might pantomime a toothbrushing gesture with oscillations of greatly exaggerated amplitude, use the forefinger as though it were the toothbrush rather than pantomiming how the handle of the brush should be held, and/or orient the arm and hand inappropriately relative to the head. The disorder occurs most frequently after lesions to the dominant (usually left) inferior parietal lobule, but can also be seen with superior parietal [3,4] and more anterior [5] damage. In some cases, IM patients also have difficulty distinguishing correct from erroneous gestures, suggesting * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-215-456-5953; fax: +1-215-456-5926. E-mail address: lbuxbaum@einstein.edu (L.J. Buxbaum). that gesture representations are damaged, and not merely inaccessible to motor output mechanisms [5]. Heilman et al. (e.g. [5]) have demonstrated that gesture recognition may be impaired in subjects with inferior parietal, but not more anterior damage, suggesting that gesture representations may be localized to the left inferior parietal lobe (and see [21]). While much of the literature to date focuses on IM patients' spatiotemporal difficulties in the production of gestures with the arm (e.g. hammering, sawing, etc.), there are indications that some patients may exhibit particular deficits in forming hand configurations appropriate for object use. Sirigu et al. [1] reported the case of an apraxic subject, LL, who appeared deficient in knowledge of the hand postures associated with familiar objects. Although LL could grasp objects normally in the context of reaching tasks, and exhibited appropriate trajectories of the arm when demonstrating object use, she was unable to position her hand properly. For example, when asked to demonstrate 0028-3932/03/$ - see front matter © 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved, doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00314-7 1092 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 "eating soup with a spoon", LL clenched the handle of the spoon with the entire hand, rather than demonstrating the precise learned finger position associated with spoons. Nevertheless, the trajectory of the arm from an imagined bowl to the mouth was entirely appropriate. These data appear consistent with integrity of hand positioning in response to object structure (grasping task) but degradation of hand posture guided by function (object use task). This suggests that apraxics may suffer impairments in a subset of gesture engrams coding learned postural configurations of the hand. To our knowledge, however, LL is the only apraxic for whom production and knowledge of hand postures has been put to empirical test. In this study, we assessed whether hand posture deficits are (a) prevalent in IM, and (b) specific to the apraxia syndrome, rather than reflecting neurologic damage more generally. We also assessed whether hand posture deficits, when present, are attributable to a motor production deficit, or rather to a loss of the representations underlying skilled hand configurations. Finally, we assessed whether particular patterns of hand posture impairment might be observed. To the degree that patients with IM suffer deficits specific to stored knowledge of the hand postures appropriate to objects, response to structural features of objects is predicted to be intact. If, on the other hand, apraxics' difficulties with hand posture reflect some degree of deficit external to the stored gesture system (e.g. within spatiomotor systems mediating object grasping), then impairment might be expected to emerge in responses to structural features of objects as well. 2. Study 1: Normative study with healthy adult subjects The design of the experiments was inspired by a series of studies by Klatzky et al. [6] indicating that there is a strong relationship between object knowledge and knowledge of hand configurations. In one study, undergraduate subjects were asked to rate the likelihood of using one of four canonical hand configurations with each of 73 objects. The configurations, which resulted from the crossing of the factors prehensility and size of the contacting surface, were poke (small, non-prehensile), pinch (small, prehensile), palm (large, non-prehensile), and clench (large, prehensile). The ratings indicated that objects were reliably associated with certain hand postures (e.g. hammer-clench). Subjects also listed a functional context in which a given object might be contacted with a particular hand shape. There emerged three classes of functional context: hold/pick up (palm, pinch, and clench); feel/touch (poke, palm); and use (varied) [6]. This study indicated that there is strong relationship between object knowledge and gesture knowledge. Hand posture representations are reliably evoked by objects, and moreover, can be cognitively discriminated. A second study in the series was designed to provide normative data on the link between structure and hand pos- ture categories, and to assess whether structure alone was sufficient to predict the responses in the first study. Undergraduate subjects viewed 90 abstract shapes including 3D parallelograms (i.e. parallelepipeds) and spheres and rated which hand configuration they would be most likely to use to contact the object. A discriminant analysis was then performed to see if the modal response to each object was predicted by its structure (surface area and depth). The discriminant analysis assigned 63 of the 90 objects appropriately, i.e. to the modal response class. Flat shapes with a large surface area, for example, tended to be assigned to "palm", whereas indented shapes with a small surface area were frequently assigned to "poke". Projectile shapes with large and small surface areas were assigned to "clench" and "pinch", respectively. Objects incorrectly assigned tended to be those for which subjects themselves had difficulty agreeing. Finally, the investigators assessed whether the discriminant function derived from the novel objects accurately classified the real objects of Study 1. There was good agreement for most objects, but there was a subset of objects for which functional considerations over-rode structural factors. One such subset were objects assigned by the discriminant analysis to the "poke" category by virtue of their shallow depth, but which are actually pinched in a functional context (e.g. nail, paperclip, and zipper). Another subset was categorized by the discriminant function as pinch, but actually function with a clench (e.g. hammer, knife, and saltshaker). Thus, structure alone was not sufficient to predict subjects' knowledge of the hand postures associated with these objects. In Study 1, we adopted some of the methods used by Klatzky et al. [6] to assess healthy adults' responses to real and novel objects. The primary aim of the study was to provide normative data on the experimental tasks with which to compare brain-lesioned subjects. 2.1. Method 2.1.1. Participants Ten healthy older adults were recruited from the community surrounding MossRehab. There were six females and four males. All subjects were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing, and had no history of learning difficulties or central nervous system dysfunction. Their mean age was 65.8 (range 47-83) and mean years of education was 12.6 (range 9-18). 2.1.2. Materials Photographs of four hand postures (clench, pinch, palm, poke; see Fig. 1) remained in view 10 cm to the right of subjects' midline throughout the experiment. Stimuli were 35 manipulable real objects (e.g. tools, kitchen implements, office supplies, etc.; see Table 1) drawn from the object corpus of Klatzky et al. [6] and 20 novel objects, each consisting of a blue parallelepiped positioned upon a white base. Parallelepipeds were constructed similarly to those used in the Klatzky et al. [6] study. They ranged in depth from L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1093 Fig. 1. Photographs of the clench, pinch, palm, and poke hand configurations used in the experiments. The photographs remained on the table-top, and subjects were permitted to refer to them throughout. — 1 cm below the surface of the base to 9.5 cm above the base surface. Horizontal (top) surfaces ranged from 0.64 cm2 to 100 cm2 (see Table 1 for dimensions). 2.1.3. Procedure Subjects were seated at a table upon which the hand posture photographs and experimental stimuli were placed. Prior to initiation of the experiment, subjects examined the photographs of the four hand postures as the postures were named by the experimenter. To ensure subjects' appreciation of differences between postures, they were required to reproduce each posture with the left hand, and the examiner corrected configurations inconsistent with the photographs. Subjects were informed that on each trial, they would see a real or novel object, and that they would be required to select the hand posture most appropriate for each object. They were also informed that all four postures were relevant in the course of the experiment, and that they should be certain to try to produce all of them. Subjects began each trial with the left hand positioned at midline palm-up on the tabletop. On each trial the examiner placed an object at the subject's midline, approximately 40 cm anterior to the body, and the subject was instructed to respond as soon as he or she was certain of the response. The left hand was used to enable comparison with the left-hemisphere lesioned apraxic subjects to be run in Study 2, who performed with the ipsilesional hand. In the "Point" condition, subjects were asked to respond to each stimulus object by touching with the left hand the photograph showing the hand posture they would be "most likely to make to interact with or use the object". To discourage subjects from attempting to program a specific configuration of the hand in this condition, they were told that they should simply turn over their hand from the palm-up resting state, and touch the target photograph in whatever manner was easiest. In the "Pantomime" condition, instruc- tions were the same, except that subjects were asked to actually form (pantomime) the target hand posture rather than touching a photograph. Subjects were instructed to form the hand posture precisely as it appeared in the photograph, using the left hand in the space directly anterior to the body wall, rather than reaching toward the object. Blocks of real and novel object trials were presented in ABBA sequence. Five subjects performed the Point condition first, followed after a 15-min rest period by the Pantomime condition, and the other five subjects performed the experiment in the reverse sequence. Each block began with five practice trials, during which any "incorrect" responses were pointed out by the examiner and a correct response elicited from subjects. Each of the 35 real objects was presented once and each of the 20 novel objects presented twice in both Pantomime and Point conditions, for a total of 150 experimental trials. Performance was videotaped for subsequent analysis. Subjects' responses in Point conditions were tallied and entered on prepared coding sheets during the test session. For Pantomime condition scores, two trained judges independently viewed each videotape and assigned each response to a hand posture category. Videotapes showed subjects' hands only, and judges were not aware of subjects' identities or the object to which the response was made. Judges also indicated via a 3-point rating scale their confidence that the posture produced was assigned accurately to a hand posture category (1: very confident, 2: somewhat confident, 3: not at all confident). Judges were instructed that "hybrid" postures should be given ratings of 2 or 3, depending upon the judges' certainty. For example, a posture approximating a pinch (fingers opposed) but including three digits might be assigned a "2" or "3" rating. Confidence ratings of the two judges were tallied and averaged. The following scoring guidelines were provided to the judges for the Pantomime condition (1) the four hand postures to be coded result from the "crossing" of the factors finger (few versus all) and hand shape (open versus closed). Poke and pinch responses use 1 and 2 fingers, respectively, whereas palm and clench incorporate all fingers. Poke and palm responses have an "open" hand shape, whereas pinch and clench have a "closed" hand shape, (2) postures that are ambiguous with respect to one or both of the above factors (finger and hand shape) are to be assigned to the posture category judged to be closest, and assigned a confidence rating less than 1, (3) if more than 1 response is produced on a given trial, the first response is to be coded, (4) for a response to be "scoreable", a given hand posture must be maintained for at least 1 s. Subsequent to independent scoring by the two coders, discrepant scores were reviewed and one response category was agreed upon. This affected <2% of trials. For each object, the percentages of responses in each hand posture category were tallied. The data from the Point condition with the novel objects were then entered into discriminant function analyses that Table 1 Real and novel objects, dimensions, control responses, and classification Real objects Area Depth Poke Pinch Palm Clench Category assigned Category assigned Agreement of discrin (width x height2) (% responses) (% responses) (% responses) (% responses) by controls' modal by discriminant function and control response function response Rolling pin 237.9 7.8 0 0 0 100 Clench Palm Conflict Glass 117 7.8 0 0 0 100 Clench Palm Conflict Wall switch 2 1 90 10 0 0 Poke Pinch Conflict Typewriter 2.25 1 70 0 30 0 Poke Pinch Conflict Keyboard 29 1.5 70 0 30 0 Poke Clench Conflict Leaf 0.4 6 0 90 10 0 Pinch Clench Conflict Envelope 19.2 10.5 0 80 20 0 Pinch Clench Conflict Clothespin 9.8 1.2 0 100 0 0 Pinch Clench Conflict Pencil 14.4 0.8 0 100 0 0 Pinch Clench Conflict Shoelaces 30.2 0.5 10 80 10 0 Pinch Clench Conflict Zipper 1.6 0.5 10 90 0 0 Pinch Poke Conflict Nail 4 0.5 0 100 0 0 Pinch Poke Conflict Hammer 68.4 3.8 0 0 10 90 Clench Clench No-conflict Saltshaker 29.6 3.8 0 0 0 100 Clench Clench No-conflict Hairbrush 12 2.5 0 0 0 100 Clench Clench No-conflict Pliers 9 5 0 40 10 50 Clench Clench No-conflict Apple 60 7.6 0 0 0 100 Clench Clench No-conflict Doorknob 25 7 0 0 10 90 Clench Clench No-conflict Lemon 56 7.5 0 0 0 100 Clench Clench No-conflict Telephone receiver 32 5 0 0 0 100 Clench Clench No-conflict Fur 930 12.8 0 14.3 71.4 14.3 Palm Palm No-conflict Cushion 930 14 10 0 70 20 Palm Palm No-conflict Sandpaper 307.5 0 0 30 70 0 Palm Palm No-conflict Basketball 620.25 25 0 0 70 30 Palm Palm No-conflict Bongo drums 400 0 0 0 100 0 Palm Palm No-conflict Deadbolt 3.6 1.2 10 60 10 20 Pinch Pinch No-conflict Chalk 9 1 10 90 0 0 Pinch Pinch No-conflict Paperclip 3.3 0.85 10 80 10 0 Pinch Pinch No-conflict Plug 2.4 3 0 80 10 10 Pinch Pinch No-conflict Guitar pick 2 2.5 10 70 20 0 Pinch Pinch No-conflict Key 4.4 2.2 0 100 0 0 Pinch Pinch No-conflict Push button phone 2.2 -0.5 90 0 10 0 Poke Poke No-conflict Thimble 2.2 -1 60 40 0 0 Poke Poke No-conflict Doorbell 2.2 0.5 100 0 0 0 Poke Poke No-conflict Calculator 0.64 0.5 80 0 20 0 Poke Poke No-conflict Novel object code Area Depth no. (width x height2) (cm) 1 0.64 i 5 0.64 3 10 1.44 6 9 25 3 2 4 -1 7 1.44 -1 6 64 6 24 16 i 3 1.44 0 4 6.25 -1 25 6.25 6 28 6.25 0.5 23 25 0 30 25 1 8 64 -1 26 6.25 3 27 4 6 32 16 1 29 25 0.5 31 16 0.5 Poke Pinch Palm (% responses) (% responses) (% responses) 14 86 0 14 86 0 11 86 0 0 7 11 75 14 11 75 14 11 0 0 29 0 18 14 64 7 29 64 14 18 0 32 7 14 57 18 21 14 54 0 18 29 32 7 50 0 39 11 4 43 4 4 29 18 0 21 39 4 43 21 Clench Category assigned Modal (% responses) by control's percent modal response 0 Pinch 86 0 Pinch 86 4 Pinch 86 82 Clench 82 0 Poke 75 0 Poke 75 71 Clench 71 68 Clench 68 0 Poke 64 4 Poke 64 60 Clench 60 11 Pinch 57 11 Palm 54 54 Clench 54 11 Palm 50 50 Clench 50 50 Clench 50 50 Clench 50 39 Palm 39 32 Pinch 32 1096 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 predicted the modal response to each object from its area and depth. The analysis assigned 16 of the 20 objects (80%) appropriately, that is, to the modal response class, indicating that the form variables were strong predictors of response to the novel objects. Not surprisingly, the four objects misas-signed by the analyses tended to be those for which subjects had difficulty agreeing; that is, those for which the modal response percentage was lowest. The standardized coefficients from the discriminant function analysis were then used to assign each real object to a predicted response class. The following four equations were calculated, and real objects assigned to the class with the highest value for the equation. In each equation, a is the area and d the depth. Clench: Value -3.144 - I- 0.0983a H h 0.834a1 Palm: Value -6.681 - 1-0.196a - 0.04127a1 Pinch: Value -2.187 - I- 0.02668a + 0.612a1 Poke: Value -1.699 - I-0.01313a - 0.182a1 For ease of exposition, we will hereafter refer to the response predicted by the discriminant function analysis as the structural response. 2.1.4. Results Table 1 provides a list of the real objects, the distribution of responses to each object, and the structural response predicted by the discriminant function analysis. There were 12 real objects for which the controls' modal response and the structural response disagreed. These were designated "real conflict" objects. There were 23 objects for which the modal response and structural response agreed ("real no-conflict" objects). We also examined whether novel objects evoked a particular hand posture. There were 10 novel objects for which there was no strong modal response by control subjects (<60% of responses in any one category), and 10 novel objects for which there was a strong modal response (>60% of responses). 2.1.5. Discussion Consistent with the data of Klatzky et al. [6], subjects were able to agree upon a hand posture configuration for real objects, and many novel objects as well. Had subjects been responding to the real objects on the basis of shape alone, all objects would have garnered high agreement between the structural response and subject response. In fact, there was a subset of real objects for which functional considerations over-rode structural factors (real conflict objects). These included objects whose structure predicted "pinch" but whose function required "poke" (piano key, typewriter key, wall switch), objects whose structure predicted "palm" but which are nevertheless "clenched" (rolling pin, glass), a third set whose structure predicted "poke" but which are instead "pinched" (e.g. zipper, nail), and a fourth set whose structure predicted "clench" but which are "pinched" (e.g. clothes pin, pencil). Some general rules appeared to emerge for the novel objects, consistent with the findings of Klatzky et al. [6]. Objects with small surface areas were usually assigned to "pinch" if depth was positive (i.e. raised), and to "poke" if depth was neutral (flat) or negative (indented). Objects with large surface area were assigned to clench (if depth was positive; i.e. raised) or palm (if depth was neutral or negative). These data support the findings of Klatzky et al. [6]: canonical hand posture categories are reliably associated with objects, and can be cognitively discriminated. 3. Study 2: Study of brain-lesioned subjects In this study, we compared the performance of nine apraxic and eight non-apraxic brain-lesioned subjects to the healthy controls run in Study 1. As in that study, we used pantomime and multiple-choice tasks to obviate the possibility of on line adjustment of hand posture in response to structural object properties. The experimental task enabled us to assess whether performance differed in tasks requiring pantomiming versus posture recognition. If both are impaired in the apraxic subjects, this would suggest that hand posture representations are degraded rather than merely inaccessible to motor output mechanisms. In addition, the experiment enabled us to assess responses to structural and functional factors. If gesture representations are impaired in apraxia, and structural factors are guiding the selection of hand posture, performance should be impaired with "real conflict" objects, but better with "no-conflict" objects. Subjects should also perform relatively well with novel objects, for which there is no stored functional information. In addition, errors should reflect the influence of structural factors. This is the pattern we predicted in the majority of patients with IM. If there are some apraxic patients for whom hand posture representations are relatively intact, then we expect responses to real objects to be better preserved than responses to novel objects. If both gesture representations and spatiomotor procedures enabling hand-shaping in response to object structure are impaired, then responses should be poor for both real and novel objects. 3.1. Method 3.1.1. Participants Nine left hemisphere stroke patients were referred for the study by a neurologist or speech therapist based on clinical information consistent with probable IM. Four of these patients were referred from the outpatient population of Hopital de la Salpetriere in Paris, and the remaining five L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1097 patients were referred from the outpatient population of MossRehab in Philadelphia. Following Heilman and Gonzalez Rothi [7], presence of ideomotor apraxia was defined by spatiotemporal errors in pantomime. After an initial practice trial in which errors were corrected, subjects were shown one of IO common objects (scissors, watch, razor, fork, comb, toothbrush, bottle opener, eraser, and cigarette lighter) and asked to demonstrate how to use each object with the ipsilesional hand. Performance was scored "leniently" on the basis of gesture recognizability (1 point — recognizable as the target gesture; 0 points — unrecognizable as the target gesture). Performance was videotaped and scored from the tapes. There were no semantic gesture substitutions; all errors were spatiotemporal distortions. This suggested that all subjects understood the requirements of the task. Scores ranged from 10-70% correct (mean 31%, S.D. 21%). Nine additional stroke patients were referred for the study based on probable absence of ideomotor apraxia. From this group, eight patients (five with left-hemisphere and three with right-hemisphere damage (hereafter referred to as LCVA and RCVA, respectively) were selected who, like the apraxics, had contralesional motor weakness (hemi-paresis), but who made no errors on the gesture pantomime screening task described above (100% correct). These eight patients served as brain-lesioned controls. Table 2 shows demographic and lesion information for the 17 brain-lesioned subjects. Lesion location for French patients was coded by a neurologist (Pascale Pradat-Diehl), and for American patients by a neurologist (H. Branch Coslett) and the first author, all of whom were blinded to subjects' group membership, from clinical MRI and/or CT scans using the templates of Damasio and Damasio [8]. Lesions for French subjects were coded from chronic scans performed near the time of testing. Lesions of American subjects were coded from acute scans performed during initial stroke-related hospitalizations, which were the only scans available for these subjects. Percent agreement of the two coders of the American scans was 93%. Disagreements were reconciled through additional review of the data. 3.1.2. Background testing In addition to the main experimental tasks, to be described below, brain-lesioned subjects performed a battery of background tests to assess praxis, language function, gesture recognition, object knowledge, motor function, and tactile sensation. Detailed scoring of praxis tasks enabled assessment of the relationship of hand posture in gesture tasks and hand posture production and recognition on the experimental task. There were several additional aims of the background testing, which were exploratory in nature. One purpose of the battery was to provide data on the relationships between hand posture in grasping tasks and hand posture on the experimental task. Another question was the relationship of gesture recognition—a classic test of the integrity of gesture engrams—to hand posture recognition. A third question concerned the relationship of semantic manipulation knowledge for objects and hand posture knowledge. As exploration of these relationships were secondary to the major aims of the study, we did not include sufficient numbers of subjects for statistical (e.g. correlational) analyses. Results of backround testing are nevertheless included insofar as they may inform future work in this area. Results are displayed in Tables 3 and 4. As can be seen in Table 3, all apraxic subjects had difficulties with all spatiotemporal components of gesture. Hand posture was rated particularly poorly on pantomime tasks for most subjects, but improved when actual objects were held. An example of the types of erroneous hand postures observed comes from a subject who produced a loose clench/palm hybrid with thumb out in pantomiming use of a bottle opener. Hand posture was significantly more impaired for transitive (object-related) gestures than intransitive (symbolic) gestures (t — 4.1, P — 0.003). Two of the brain-lesioned controls tended to exhibit the same pattern, leaving open the possibility that the transitive hand postures were more demanding (but see below for data suggesting an alternative possibility). Ipsilesional grip strength, tactile sensation, and proprioception were grossly intact in all subjects. Table 4 shows that several subjects had mildly reduced ipsilesional dexterity. Grasping objects was normal for all subjects except A5. Gesture recognition scores (which were available only for subjects A5-A9) were >2 SDs below the control mean (i.e. < raw score 27.7, or 92.3%, see Appendix A) for all apraxic subjects except A9, who was within the normal range. Only A7 showed more than mild impairment. One of the brain-lesioned controls (L3) was below the normal range as well.1 Static posture imitation, on the other hand, was >3 SDs below the control mean (i.e. raw score 17.9, or 92.5%; see Appendix A) for the majority of the apraxics tested (subjects A5-A8) and <2 SDs for the remaining subject, A9. Again, one of the brain-lesioned controls (LI) was below the normal range as well. All of the American apraxics exhibited aphasia on the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) [9]. Performance was generally less impaired on the Philadelphia Comprehension Battery, a test of word-picture matching in which an auditory word must be matched to one of three within-category or across-category pictures [10]. Brain-lesioned control subjects L3, L4, and L5 also exhibited comprehension deficits on the WAB. On the Moss Object Probes Test, apraxic subjects performed at least somewhat better on items testing function knowledge as compared to manipulation knowledge (except A8, whose performance was equal on both). This pattern is consistent with previous findings by our group [11,12]. 1 The medical records from the acute hospitalization of this subject indicated that he was apraxic at that time. Table 2 Subject demographics and lesion information Group Subject Gender Age Handed Locus of Lesion Brodmann's areas Time since Aphasia: original ness lesion clinical diagnosis Apraxie Al M 75 R Small left temporo-parietal 22, 40 2 months Conduction A2 M 74 R Large left parietal 1, 5, 39, 40 2 years Broca A3 M 62 R Small left temporo-parietal 22, 39, 40 6 months Conduction A4 F 75 Small left parietal 39, 40 4 years Conduction A5 F 52 R Large left frontotemporoparietal 4, 6, 7, s, 18, 19, 22, 28, 36, 37, 39, 40, 44, 45, 46 12 years Broca A6 M 55 R Left frontotemporoparietal 4, s, 6, 21, 22, 39, 40, 41-42 2 years Broca A7 M 56 R Large left frontotemporoparietal 4, 6, s, 21, 22, 25, 37, 39, 40, 41-42, 44, 45, 47 16 years Broca A8 M 59 R Large left frontotemporoparietal 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, s, 22, 23, 24, 31, 32, 39, 40, 41-42, 46 5 years Broca A9 M 64 R Large left frontotemporoparietal, subcortical 6, s, 21, 22, 39, 40, 41-42, 44, 45 3 years Broca Right CVA Rl M 61 R Right frontal, subcortical 47, putamen, globus pallidus 7 months - R2 M 54 L Right dorsolateral frontal, subcortical 4, 6, 8, s 6 months - R3 M 54 R Large right basal ganglia/external capsule 22, 37, putamen, basal ganglia, external capsule 7 months - Left CVA LI M 55 R Left pontine, cerebellar peduncle - 2 years - (Dysarthria) L2 F 79 R Left pontine - 2 years - (Dysarthria) L3 M 67 R Large left dorsolateral frontotemporoparietal 40, 41, 42, 44, 45, 21, 22 8 years Broca L4 M 50 R Large left frontotemporal 1, 2, 3, 6, 22, 44, insula 1 year Broca L5 F 54 R Large left mesial frontal, dorsolateral prefrontal 23 , 24, 32, 4, 6 8 months Transcortical motor and frontal L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1099 Table 3 Scores on apraxia tests (percent correct) Group Subject Content Hand Arm Amplit. Timing Total spatiotemporal Transitive gesture to sij ?ht Apraxie Al 100 40 90 70 90 73 A2 100 20 10 10 0 10 A3 100 20 90 50 90 63 A4 100 30 90 40 70 58 A5 90 22 78 50 90 64 A6 100 10 60 60 90 55 A7 100 60 70 70 100 75 A8 89 50 63 56 44 56 A9 100 60 100 70 70 75 Mean 98 35 72 53 72 59 Right CVA Rl 100 100 90 100 90 95 R2 100 100 90 90 100 95 R3 100 100 100 100 100 100 Left CVA LI 100 90 100 70 90 88 L2 100 100 100 100 100 100 L3 100 90 100 80 100 93 L4 100 90 100 90 100 95 L5 100 90 90 90 100 93 Transitive gesture to imitation Apraxie A5 100 20 80 70 80 63 A6 100 50 60 100 90 73 A7 100 30 80 90 100 75 A8 100 30 70 70 70 60 A9 100 70 60 80 100 78 Mean 100 40 70 82 86 70 Right CVA Rl 100 100 100 100 100 100 R2 100 100 100 100 100 100 R3 100 100 100 100 100 100 Left CVA LI 100 100 100 90 90 95 L2 100 90 100 100 100 98 L3 100 100 100 80 100 95 L4 100 100 90 100 100 98 L5 100 100 90 100 100 98 Object use Apraxie A5 100 100 70 30 90 73 A6 100 90 90 80 90 88 A7 100 90 70 100 90 88 A8 100 70 60 80 80 73 A9 100 50 80 70 100 75 Mean 100 80 74 72 90 79 Right CVA Rl 100 100 90 100 100 98 R2 100 100 90 100 100 98 R3 100 100 100 100 100 100 Left CVA LI 100 90 100 90 100 95 L2 100 100 100 100 90 98 L3 100 10 10 9 10 98 L4 100 100 90 90 100 95 L5 100 90 90 100 100 95 Intransitive gesture to command Apraxie Al 100 80 100 100 100 95 A2 80 25 75 50 75 56 A3 80 75 100 75 100 88 A4 60 67 100 100 100 92 A5 60 67 33 100 100 75 A6 100 60 60 100 100 80 A7 100 60 60 100 100 80 A8 100 100 80 100 100 95 1100 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 Table 3 {Continued) Group Subject Content Hand Arm Amplit. Timing Total spatiotemporal A9 100 60 80 80 100 80 Mean 87 66 76 89 97 82 Right CVA Rl 100 100 100 100 100 100 R2 100 100 100 100 100 100 R3 100 100 100 100 100 100 Left CVA LI 100 100 100 80 100 95 L2 100 100 100 100 100 100 L3 40 100 100 100 100 100 L4 100 100 100 100 100 100 L5 100 100 100 100 100 100 Intransitive gesture to imitation Apraxic A5 100 50 75 100 100 81 A6 100 60 60 100 100 80 A7 100 100 50 100 100 88 A8 100 80 60 100 100 85 A9 100 100 100 100 100 100 Mean 100 78 69 100 100 87 Right CVA Rl 100 100 100 100 100 100 R2 100 100 100 100 100 100 R3 100 100 100 100 100 100 Left apraxic LI 100 100 100 100 100 100 L2 100 100 100 100 100 100 L3 100 100 100 100 100 100 L4 100 100 100 100 100 100 L5 100 100 100 100 100 100 3.1.3. Materials and procedure Materials and procedure for the experimental tasks of Study 2 were the same as Study 1, except that the objects were limited to the 35 real objects and 10 novel objects for which there had been a modal response in Study 1. Each of the 35 real objects was presented once, and each of the 10 novel objects was presented twice, in both Pantomime and Point conditions, for a total of 110 experimental trials. Care was taken to ensure that subjects comprehended all task instructions, and practice trials were repeated until they were performed without error. Nine of the 17 brain-lesioned subjects performed the Pantomime condition first, and the other eight subjects performed the Point condition first. Subjects performed all experimental tasks with the ipsilesional hand. As in Study 1, performance in the Pantomime condition was scored from the videotapes by two independent coders. Disagreements arose on few trials(<3%), indicating that the scoring system was reliable. Trials with disagreements were reconciled by discussion. 3.1.3.1. Data analysis part 1: comparison of patient and control data; assessment of structural and functional response determinants. Control data from Study 1 were used as the basis for comparison of the patient data in Study 2. Given that there was some inherent ambiguity in coding of responses in the Pantomime condition, but no such ambiguity in the Point condition, control responses in the Point condition of Study 1 were taken as the "normative" response in most comparisons. To maximize the opportunity to observe differences between patients, many comparisons were performed using a "case-study" model in which each patient's individual data were compared to a control mean. 3.1.3.1.1. Response ambiguity analysis. The first analysis assessed whether brain-lesioned subjects' gestures were more ambiguous than control gestures. Mean confidence ratings of subjects were tallied for each response condition. Since the response ambiguity (confidence rating) data were not normally distributed due to restricted range (minimum — 1, maximum — 3), also tabulated were the number of trials on which subjects received confidence ratings of 2 or 3 (somewhat confident or not at all confident). The frequency counts of such trials in control, apraxic, RCVA, and LCVA groups was compared by Mann-Whitney [/-tests. 3.1.3.1.2. Modal/non-modal analysis. The second analysis assessed whether brain-lesioned subjects produced unusual responses to real and/or novel objects. Based on controls' responses in the Point condition of Study 1, we first classified the modal, less frequent, and "never made" responses for each object. For example, if for "hammer" there were eight clenches and two pinches, then "clench" was modal, "pinch" was less frequent, and "palm" and "poke" were never made. We then tallied patients' responses according to these categories. For example, if a patient produced a "pinch" for "hammer", the response was tallied as Table 4 Subjects' performance on background tests Group Subject Dexterity3 Graspingb Gest Recog Static posture (%) imitation (%) Philadelphia Comprehension Battery0 Western Aphasia Battery1 Moss Object Probes Test L R L R Within catagory (%) Across category (%) Spontaneous speech (%) compr (%) Repet {%) Name (%) Funct (%) Manip (%) Phys (%) Apraxie Al 12.3 9.7 20 20 A2 4.3 0.7 20 NA A3 9.3 3.3 20 20 A4 5.0 4.7 20 17 A5 6.7 0.0 17 NA 90 SO 100 97 60 66 14 49 86 74 97 A6 6.7 0.0 20 NA 90 10 88 100 50 S9 40 86 97 91 94 A7 9.7 0.0 20 NA SO 70 88 100 NA NA NA 34 94 83 83 A8 10.3 0.7 19 20 90 50 94 96 75 73 75 91 97 97 91 A9 8.7 0.0 19 NA 93 90 81 86 35 S5 69 43 100 80 94 Right CVA Rl 0.0 10.0 NA 20 100 100 NA NA NA NA NA 100 100 100 97 R2 7.3 8.7 20 20 100 100 NA NA XA NA NA 100 100 100 97 R3 5.0 11.3 20 20 100 100 NA XA XA NA NA 77 100 100 94 Left CVA LI 7.7 6.0 20 20 100 90 NA XA 100 99 98 97 100 100 100 L2 8.3 8.3 20 20 100 100 NA XA 100 100 100 66 97 97 94 L3 15.0 16.6 20 20 ')o 95 XA XA 65 ss 57 89 91 94 100 L4 6.7 4.0 20 20 100 90 NA XA 95 79 96 100 100 97 97 L5 6.3 5.7 20 20 100 100 NA XA 95 S5 100 100 100 100 100 aMean oscillations per 10 s. b n = 10, maximum score = 20. CWAB data not available for subject A7 or RCVAs. 1102 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 "less frequent". As frequency counts in the "never made" cells were frequently extremely low, particularly for the brain-lesioned controls (e.g. 0 or 1), for purposes of statistical analysis we collapsed across the "less frequent" and "never made" categories to derive the total number of non-modal responses. The average (mean) control distribution of modal and non-modal responses was then compared to the distribution of each brain-lesioned subject by x2 analysis. 3.1.3.1.3. Response distribution analysis. The third analysis assessed whether brain-lesioned and control subjects produced different patterns of responses across the four hand posture categories. The distribution of responses to each object (number of pokes, pinches, palms, and clenches) was tallied for each brain-lesioned subject. Each patient's distribute was compared to the average (mean) control distribution of responses in the relevant Point condition by x2 analysis. 3.1.3.1.4. Pantomime versus point analysis. The fourth analysis assessed whether brain-lesioned subjects' performance differed in Pantomime versus Point conditions. Responses matching the modal control response (i.e. "correct" responses) were tallied in the Pantomime and Point conditions, and the two conditions compared by x2 analysis. 3.1.3.1.5. Error rate analyses: structural responses. This analysis assessed whether subjects' errors with real conflict objects were more likely to be based on structural factors than would be predicted by chance. The expected rate of structural response errors (number of possible structural response errors over number of all possible errors) was calculated for real conflict objects in both Pantomime and Point conditions. The observed rate of structural response errors was calculated for the same conditions, and differences in the expected and observed rates assessed by Binomial Test. This procedure was performed separately on the data from the apraxic and control groups (the observed rate of errors in the brain-lesioned controls, 8 and 5 in Pantomime and Point conditions respectively, was too low to be analyzed). 3.1.3.1.6. Item analyses. In this analysis, brain-lesioned subjects' "correct" responses to each real object in Pantomime and Point conditions were tallied to ascertain whether there were some items that were inherently difficult for apraxic subjects. An "Item" was defined as a given object in a specific condition, e.g. "apple" in the Pantomime condition. 3.1.3.1.7. Regression analyses of gesture task and experimental tasks. Finally, to ascertain whether subjects' performance on the experimental tasks could be predicted from their praxis performance, we performed multiple regressions with the hand posture, arm posture, amplitude, and timing component of the gesture to sight of objects task of the background battery as independent variables, and modal responses in the Pantomime and Point conditions as dependent variables. We expected the hand posture component of transitive gestures to emerge as a strong predictor of experimental performance. 3.2. Results 3.2.1. Response ambiguity analysis Apraxics' hand postures were qualitatively poor, resulting in coders' perception that it was difficult to assign them to a hand posture category. It should be emphasized that despite this subjective sense of difficulty, inter-rater reliability was strong. Disagreement between the two coders arose on few (<2%) trials. Fig. 2 shows examples of postures assigned a "2" or "3" ("somewhat confident" or "not at all confident") rating from the videotapes of apraxic subjects. Fig. 2. (a and b) Still photographs taken from the videotape of an apraxic subject showing the subject's response to a novel object with negative (indented) depth and small surface area, which evoked a "poke" modal response in control subjects. The apraxic subject responded by producing two ambiguous postures in sequence. In accordance with our scoring guidelines, the first response (Fig. 2a) was assigned to a posture category ("clench"), and the judges indicated that they were only "somewhat confident" that the posture was indeed a clench (confidence score of 2). L.J. Buxbaum et all'Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1103 Table 5 Subjects' responses coded with respect to modal, less frequent, and never-made responses of controls in Point condition Group Subject Responses (number of trials) x2 Significance P Modal Less frequent Never (Modal/non-modal) Pantomime condition Control Range 37-52 3-18 0-1 Mean 45 9 1 Apraxic Al 34 9 12 5.4 * 0.03 A2 22 18 15 20.2 *** 0.0001 A3 36 11 8 3.8 0.08 A4 26 15 14 14.3 *** 0.0003 A5 19 29 16 25.2 *** 0.0001 A6 26 11 18 14.3 *** 0.0003 A7 35 13 7 4.6 * 0.05 A8 12 21 21 38.8 *** 0.0001 A9 21 23 11 22.6 *** 0.0001 RCVA Rl 52 3 0 4.3 0.07 R2 51 4 0 2.9 0.15 R3 52 3 0 4.3 0.07 LCVA LI 48 6 1 0.6 0.59 L2 46 8 1 0.2 0.79 L3 51 4 0 2.9 0.15 L4 51 4 0 2.9 0.15 L5 51 1 3 2.9 0.15 Point condition Control Range 36-53 2-19 0 Mean 45 10 0 Apraxic Al 46 7 2 0.1 0.999 A2 29 12 14 10.6 ** 0.002 A3 19 19 17 25.2 *** 0.0001 A4 33 16 6 6.3 * 0.02 A5 25 14 16 15.7 *** 0.0001 A6 19 6 10 7.9 ** 0.008 A7 41 9 5 0.9 0.48 A8 18 17 20 27.1 *** 0.0001 A9 35 11 9 4.6 * 0.03 RCVA Rl 52 3 0 4.3 0.07 R2 53 2 0 5.9 More modal* 0.03 R3 51 4 0 2.9 0.15 LCVA LI 47 7 1 0.3 0.07 L2 50 5 0 1.9 0.16 L3 48 5 2 0.6 0.15 L4 49 0 6 1.2 0.27 L5 50 1 4 1.9 0.16 * P < 0.05. '* P< 0.01. •* P< 0.001 Recall that scores of 1 reflect high confidence and 3 low confidence. The mean confidence rating for apraxics' pantomimes was 1.6 (S.D. 0.7). For LCVA, the mean rating was 1.0 (S.D. 0.2), for RCVA it was 1.1 (S.D. 0.2) and for controls it was 1.0 (S.D. 0.1). In the apraxic group, a mean of 8.6 responses (S.D. 4.5) was given a 2 or 3 confidence rating. For the other groups, results indicated that relatively few responses were given scores of 2 or 3: LCVA: mean 0.4 responses (sd. 0.2); RCVA: mean 1.7 responses (S.D. 1.0); controls: mean 0.3 responses (S.D. 0.6). Mann-Whitney [/-tests performed on the number of trials with confidence ratings >1 confirmed that significantly more of the apraxics' gestures were rated with low confidence than controls' gestures for all conditions (novel, real conflict, and real no-conflict objects) (U Prime >83, P < 0.001 for all). Apraxics were also rated with lower confidence than RCVA and LCVA for novel objects (U Prime >26.5, P < 0.01 for both). RCVA, LCVA, and controls did not differ in any comparisons. 3.2.2. Modal/non-modal analyses Table 5 shows the distributions of modal, less frequent, and "never made" responses for the brain-lesioned subjects in Pantomime and Point condition, collapsed across real 1104 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 Table 6 Distribution of responses: summary of significant differences from control subjects Group Subject Distribution of modal/non-modal responses Pantomime Point Real conflict Real no-conflict Novel Real conflict Real no-conflict Novel Apraxie Al * A2 *** ** t * A3 ** ** ** A4 * ** A5 *** ** * *** ** A6 * * ** * A7 ** * A8 *** *** *** *** A9 ** ** ** ** RCVA Rl R2 R3 LCVA LI L2 L3 L4 L5 t = trend (P < 0.1). * P < 0.05. ** P< 0.01. *** P< 0.001. conflict, real no-conflict, and novel objects. In statistical comparisons of modal versus non-modal responses, all of the apraxics, but none of the brain-lesioned controls, differed significantly from the normative data (healthy controls in the Point condition),2 in part because they produced an abnormally high number of "never made" responses. Table 6 provides summaries of the significant and non-significant effects when the analyses comparing controls and patients were performed for real conflict, real no-conflict, and novel objects separately.3 Without exception, patterns of response differing significantly from the healthy controls were found Note that because the Point condition was used as the normative distribution for determination of modal, less-frequent, and never-made response, it was possible for the controls themselves to differ from this in the Pantomime condition (hence the "1" tallied in the "never-made" column of the Pantomime condition). 3 To address the concern that cultural factors may have differently affected French and American subjects' responses, we also ran five French age-matched control subjects at the Institute des Sciences Cog-nitives in Lyon using the methods of Study 2. The distributions of modal/non-modal/never made responses in each condition (Real Conflict, No-Conflict, and Novel, for both Pantomime and Point) were compared for the French and American controls by /2 analyses. The distributions of French and Americans' poke, pinch, palm, and clench responses in each condition were also compared by /2 analyses. Of the 12 comparisons performed, there was only 1 reaching significance: in the Pantomime condition, French controls made significantly more modal responses and fewer non-modal responses to novel objects than Americans (x2 = 7.1, P < 0.05) (i.e. their responses were slightly more canonical). The comparability of American and French control performance suggests that examining the French and American apraxic patients together is not likely to obscure cultural differences between these groups. only in the apraxics, and not in the LCVA or RCVA groups.4 As can be seen, the apraxics exhibited many differences from the controls, particularly with real objects (both conflict and no-conflict). With the exception of subject A3, subjects performed normally with novel objects in the Point condition. Several subjects performed normally with novel objects in both Pantomime and Point conditions (Al, A4, and A7). 3.2.3. Response distribution analysis The response distributions differing from controls are shown in Table 7. Again, performance patterns differing from the healthy control distribution were found only in the apraxic group. Although numerous patterns were evident, the following generalities emerged: nearly all significant patterns entailed fewer Poke responses than expected based on the control distribution, and many entailed more Pinch and/or Clench responses than expected. In general, fewer differences from normal performance were found in this analysis than in the preceding modal/non-modal analysis, probably because the pattern of responses was distributed over four possible response categories rather than three. 3.2.4. Pantomime versus point analysis In the apraxic group, subjects Al and A9 produced significantly more "correct" responses in the Point as compared 4 Given the number of statistical comparisons performed, we considered performing statistical (e.g. Bonferroni) corrections to determine acceptable P-values. One difficulty is that too-stringent correction risks obscuring potentially meaningful patterns in the data, such as the observed pattern of significant P-values only in the apraxic group. L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1105 Table 7 Distribution of responses in four hand posture categories Group Subject Real conflict Real no-conflict Novel Poke Pinch Palm Clench Poke Pinch Palm Clench Poke Pinch Palm Clench Pantomime conditions Control Mean 2 7 1 2 4 7 4 8 6 7 2 5 Apraxie Al 0 10 1 1 0 12 2 9 8 9 0 3 A2 1 7 1 3 1 12 0 10 1 6 10 3 A3 0 7 0 5 3 4 5 11 9 10 1 0 A4 0 6 0 6 0 8 0 14 2 5 0 13 A5 0 1 6 5 2 4 6 11 1 14 5 0 A6 0 5 1 6 1 12 3 7 11 4 2 3 A7 0 4 4 4 0 3 5 15 5 9 0 6 A8 0 0 6 6 0 0 11 11 3 2 3 12 A9 0 4 0 8 0 9 0 14 0 6 3 11 Total 1 44 19 44 7 64 32 102 40 65 24 51 Point conditions Control Mean 2 7 1 2 4 7 4 8 6 7 2 5 Apraxie Al 0 9 1 2 5 7 3 8 7 6 1 6 A2 0 8 0 4 0 13 0 10 0 14 0 6 A3 3 4 4 1 5 4 8 6 3 7 3 7 A4 0 7 2 3 4 5 4 10 0 10 0 10 A5 3 2 5 2 4 4 6 9 0 13 1 6 A6 3 4 1 4 4 6 8 5 7 7 6 0 A7 0 6 5 1 0 9 6 8 9 5 0 6 A8 3 5 1 3 1 8 3 11 9 5 4 2 A9 2 4 1 5 4 6 5 8 8 4 0 8 Total 14 49 20 25 27 62 43 75 43 71 15 51 Values in bold are significantly different than control distribution by / (P < 0.05). to Pantomime condition (j2 > 7.0, P < 0.05 in both cases). Conversely, apraxic subject A3 produced more correct responses in the Pantomime condition (j2 — 10.5, P < 0.01). The other apraxics performed equally in both response conditions. None of the RCVA or LCVA subjects exhibited significant differences between conditions. 3.2.5. Error rate analyses: structural responses For each object, there was a correct response (e.g. pinch) and three possible error responses (e.g. palm, clench, and poke). There was a one in three (33%) chance that an observed error would be consistent with the structural response. In the healthy controls, the rates of structural response errors were 5/15 (33%) and 2/13 (15%) in Pantomime and Point conditions. These rates were not significantly different than chance (P > 0.1 by Binomial Test). The rates of structural response errors observed in the apraxic subjects, in Pantomime and Point conditions, respectively, were 22/58 (38%) and 15/55 (27%) (neither significantly different than chance, P > 0.2 for both). Thus, neither apraxic nor control subjects were more likely to make structural response errors than would be expected by chance. 3.2.6. Item analyses As Table 8 shows, 13 items (recall that "item" refers to an object in a specific condition, e.g. apple/Pantomime) evoked a relatively great number (>6) correct responses by apraxics in Pantomime and/or Point conditions. All 13 of these items had pinch or clench as the controls' modal response. Thirteen items evoked a relatively small number (<3) correct responses in Pantomime and/or Point conditions. Twelve of these 13 items had poke as the controls' modal response. 3.2.7. Regression analyses of gesture task and experimental tasks We performed two regression analyses with the data from all 17 brain-lesioned subjects in which hand posture, arm posture, amplitude, and timing component scores from the gesture to sight of objects task were used to predict scores in Pantomime and Point conditions of the experimental tasks. Gesture scores strongly predicted performance of the experimental tasks. The multiple adjusted r2 for the model in which gesture was used to predict the experimental Pantomime condition was 0.71 (F = 11.0, P < 0.0005). The strongest independent predictor of experimental performance was the hand posture gesture component (standard coefficient — 0.93, P — 0.01), followed by timing (standard coefficient — 0.66, P — 0.05). The other gesture components did not contribute to the model (P > 0.2). Similarly, the multiple adjusted r2 for the model in which gesture scores were used to predict the experimental Point condition was 0.72 (F — 11.1, P — 0.0005). The only gesture component to independently predict experimental performance was the 1106 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 Table 8 Item analysis: numbers of correct responses to real objects Control modal response No. correct Pantomime Conflict objects Glass Clench 8 Rolling pin Clench 7 Zipper Pinch 7 Hammer Clench 5 Leaf Pinch 5 Pencil Pinch 5 Nail Pinch 4 Envelope Pinch 3 Shoelaces Pinch 3 Keyboard Poke 1 Typewriter Poke 0 Wall switch Poke 0 No-conflict objects Apple Clench 8 Paperclip Pinch 7 Clothespin Pinch 7 Saltshaker Clench 7 Plug Pinch 6 Guitar pick Pinch 6 Lemon Clench 6 Sandpaper Palm 6 Hairbrush Clench 6 Phone receiver Clench 6 Doorknob Clench 5 Fur Palm 5 Key Pinch 5 Pliers Clench 5 Basketball Palm 4 Cushion Palm 4 Bongo drums Palm 3 Thimble Poke 3 Chalk Pinch 2 Doorbell Poke 2 Calculator Poke 1 Deadbolt Pinch 1 Push button phone Poke 0 Point Conflict objects Pencil Pinch 7 Chalk Pinch 6 Leaf Pinch 6 Shoelaces Pinch 6 Hammer Clench 5 Nail Pinch 5 Glass Clench 5 Rolling pin Clench 5 Zipper Pinch 4 Envelope Pinch 3 Typewriter Poke 2 Wall switch Poke 2 Keyboard Poke 0 No-conflict objects Paperclip Pinch 8 Apple Clench 8 Clothes spin Pinch 8 Guitar pick Pinch 7 Phone receiver Clench 7 Bongo drums Palm 6 Cushion Palm 6 Table 8 (Continued) Control modal response No. correct Basketball Palm 5 Fur Palm 5 Plug Pinch 5 Key Pinch 5 Sandpaper Palm 5 Saltshaker Clench 5 Hairbrush Clench 4 Deadbolt Pinch 4 Doorknob Clench 4 Lemon Clench 4 Pliers Clench 4 Thimble Poke 4 Doorbell Poke 3 Push button phone Poke 2 Calculator key Poke 1 hand posture component (standard coefficient — 0.99, P — 0.007). 3.2.8. Discussion On tests assessing production and recognition of hand postures in response to real and novel objects, patterns of performance differing from healthy controls were prevalent in the apraxic group, but not observed in LCVA or RCVA patients without apraxia. Apraxics' performance differed, in particular, from the performance of LCVA non-apraxics with moderately large cortical lesions and comprehension impairments. We also observed a highly specific predictive relationship between performance on the hand posture component of the gesture to sight of objects pantomime task and the experimental tasks. Recall that apraxics' errors in the gesture pantomime tasks were spatiotemporal, rather than content-related, in nature. The strong association of spatiotemporal hand posture errors on the praxis tasks and deficits on the experimental tasks suggests that the latter performance is unlikely to be an artifact of overall cognitive severity or comprehension difficulties. Instead, it suggests that apraxic subjects suffer a particular deficit in cognitive representations of hand posture. Apraxics were impaired in numerous aspects of experimental task performance. Most were impaired in terms of their agreement with control subjects' modal responses, and produced many atypical responses. Several were impaired as well in terms of the distribution of responses in each of the four response categories. Examination of the pattern of responses suggested that subjects were making fewer pokes, and more clench and pinch responses than expected. Additionally, apraxics' hand postures were judged subjectively to be more difficult to assign to a posture category than the brain-lesioned control subjects. The majority of apraxic subjects were impaired with real objects in both Pantomime and Point conditions. Even with the opportunity to select an appropriate hand posture from a choice of four photographs, most subjects performed L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1107 quite poorly. This suggests that for most of the apraxics, knowledge of the hand postures associated with objects is degraded, and not merely inaccessible to motor output mechanisms. Eight of the apraxic subjects who performed abnormally with real objects performed normally with novel objects in the Point condition. Consistent with our predictions, this indicates that for the majority of apraxic subjects, knowledge of the hand configuration appropriate to object structure is intact. The specificity of this pattern is additional evidence that neither overall severity nor deficient comprehension is likely to explain the present data. To our knowledge, this is the first empirical demonstration of this pattern in IM. In this context, only three of these eight apraxic subjects (and an additional subject) performed normally with novel objects in the Pantomime condition. For most subjects, the impairment in pantomiming thus occurs irrespective of object type, suggesting that deficits in spatiomotor production are a factor additional to the deficit in evoking stored gesture representations. In other words, the pattern of performance in the majority of apraxics appears to result from the interaction of two factors: a degradation of gesture representations affecting both production and recognition for responses to real objects, and additional production deficits affecting pantomime for all objects, whether familiar or novel. We will discuss this pattern further in the General Discussion. While we had expected that most patients would exhibit relatively good performance with real objects for which structural and functional influences were in agreement, this was not the case. One possibility is that stored hand-posture representations may be activated automatically by objects, even when damaged. Reliance upon the damaged information may over-ride the relative integrity of procedures enabling responses to structural factors. Although apraxics' unimpaired performance with novel objects indicates relative integrity of response to object structure, their errors in the real conflict condition did not follow the predicted pattern, i.e. they were not largely consistent with structural responses. The determinants of apraxics' errors with real objects for which structure and function conflict thus remain unclear. The observation that they made more clench and pinch responses than expected (see above) suggests the possibility that objects might be differentially "easy" or "difficult" for the apraxic subjects as a function of the response associated with them. That is, it is possible that subjects performed correctly on "pinch" and "clench" objects (i.e. objects whose modal response was prehensile) but erred on "palm" and "poke" objects (i.e. non-prehensile objects). We performed several post hoc analyses to pursue this possibility. As the study was not originally designed to assess the prehensile/non-prehensile distinction, the number of real and novel objects in each of these groups was not balanced, and the analyses to follow should be viewed as points of interest for future investigation. 3.2.8.1. Data analysis, part 2: prehensile and non-prehensile posture analyses. 3.2.8.1.1. Prehensile versus non-prehensile analyses. Each brain-lesioned subject's "correct" responses were tallied for objects for which the controls' modal response was prehensile (pinch, clench) versus those for which the modal response was non-prehensile (palm, poke), and the conditions compared by x2 analysis. 3.2.8.1.2. Error rate analyses: prehensile responses. As we had done with structural response errors, we determined whether errors that were prehensile responses (clench and pinch) occurred over chance rates by comparing the expected rate of prehensile errors (number of possible prehensile errors over number of all possible errors) to the observed rate of prehensile errors. We then subjected the data to binomial tests. This procedure was performed separately for the apraxic and healthy control groups using data from the real object condition. There were too few errors in the Brain-lesioned control groups (eight and five in Pantomime and Point conditions, respectively) to perform this analysis. 3.3. Results 3.3.1. Prehensile versus non-prehensile analyses As Table 9 shows, in Pantomiming with real objects, five apraxics (Al, A2, A4, A6, A9) were more often correct with prehensile (modal response clench or pinch) than non-prehensile objects (modal response palm or poke), and three of the same, and an additional subject (Al, A2, A4, and A7) showed the same pattern in the Point condition. One of the eight brain-lesioned control subjects (L5) showed this pattern in the Point condition as well; however, both prehensile and non-prehensile scores (100 and 75% correct, respectively) were above the range of apraxics. With novel objects, two apraxics (A4 and A6) more often pantomimed correctly with prehensile than non-prehensile objects, and the same two subjects and two additional subjects (A2, A4, A5, and A6) showed the same pattern in the Point condition. None of the brain-lesioned control subjects showed significant differences between prehensile and non-prehensile objects. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests performed on the group data from the apraxic subjects confirmed that overall, responses were significantly more often correct for prehensile as compared to non-prehensile objects (Pantomime: z — —2.3, P — 0.02; Point: z = -2.5, P = 0.01). Given the finding that apraxics were relatively impaired with non-prehensile hand postures, we next explored whether they were equally impaired with poke and palm configurations. We tallied "correct" responses for poke versus palm trials (i.e. trials for which poke or palm were the modal control responses), collapsing across real conflict, real no-conflict, and novel objects, and across Pantomime and Point conditions, to gain additional statistical power. 1108 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1091-1113 Table 9 Comparison of responses to prehensile and non-prehensile objects Group Subject Real prehensile Real non-prehensile Significance of Novel prehensile Novel non-prehensile Significance of in = 23) no. in = 12) no. correct comparison in = 12) no. in = 8) no. correct comparison correct (%) (%) correct (%) (%) Pantomime condition Apraxic Al 16 (70) 3 (25) * 8 (67) 7 (88) A2 15 (65) 2 (17) ** 4 (33) 1 (13) A3 19 (83) 8 (67) 4 (33) 5 (63) A4 17 (74) 0 (0) *** 7 (58) 2 (25) *** A5 7 (30) 5 (42) 6 (50) 1 (13) A6 15 (65) 3 (25) * 8 (67) 1 (13) * A7 15 (65) 4 (33) 10 (83) 5 (63) A8 5 (22) 3 (25) 1 (8) 2 (25) A9 15 (65) 1 (8) ** 5 (42) 0 (0) RCVA Rl 23 (100) 12 (100) 9 (75) 8 (100) R2 23 (100) 10 (83) 12 (100) 7 (88) R3 23 (100) 11 (91) 10 (83) 7 (88) LCVA LI 23 (100) 11 (92) 10 (83) 4 (50) L2 23 (100) 10 (83) 7 (58) 8 (100) L3 23 (100) 11 (92) 12 (100) 7 (88) L4 22 (96) 10 (83) 10 (83) 7 (88) L5 23 (100) 10 (83) 12 (100) 6 (75) Point condition Apraxic Al 22 (96) 7 (58) ** 11 (92) 6 (75) A2 17 (74) 1 (8) *** 11 (92) 0 (0) *** A3 7 (30) 7 (58) 5 (42) 0 (0) * A4 20 (87) 4 (33) ** 9 (75) 0 (0) *** A5 9 (39) 4 (33) 12 (100) 0 (0) *** A6 11 (48) 8 (66) 11 (92) 0 (0) *** A7 18 (78) 4 (33) * 11 (92) 8 (100) A8 6 (26) 2 (17) 5 (42) 5 (63) A9 15 (65) 4 (33) * 10 (83) 7 (88) RCVA Rl 23 (100) 12 (100) 9 (75) 8 (100) R2 22 (96) 11 (92) 12 (100) 8 (100) R3 23 (100) 12 (100) 10 (83) 7 (88) LCVA LI 22 (96) 10 (83) 12 (100) 7 (88) L2 23 (100) 10 (83) 9 (75) 8 (100) L3 21 (91) 9 (75) 12 (100) 6 (75) L4 22 (96) 11 (92) 10 (83) 8 (100) L5 23 (100) 9 (75) * 10 (83) 8 (100) * P < 0.05. '* P< 0.01. '* P< 0.001 Subjects were clearly impaired in the production of both palm (mean 54.4% correct) and poke (mean 25.4% correct) postures, but were significantly more impaired with the latter (Z = -2.25, P = 0.02 by Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test). Analyses of individual subject data showed that four apraxics (A3, A4, A5, and A6) were significantly more impaired on poke than palm trials (j2 > 5.9, P < 0.05 for all comparisons). No subjects showed significant differences in the other direction. To assess whether subjects had equal difficulty with palm and poke trials in Pantomime and Point conditions, we examined these separately. There were five trials each in Pantomime and Point conditions for which palm was the modal response, and 15 trials each in Pantomime and Point con- ditions for which poke was the modal response. Apraxics showed the same pattern (few pokes or palms) in both Pantomime (poke mean — 3.2, palm mean — 2.4) and Point (poke mean — 4.4, palm mean — 3.0) conditions (t < 1.5, P > 0.14 for all comparisons). 3.3.1.1. Error rate analyses. Of the 105 possible errors with the real objects, there were 47 opportunities (45%) for prehensile errors (i.e. errors that were pinches or clenches). The observed rate of prehensile errors in the Pantomime and Point conditions, respectively, were as follows: healthy controls: 26/54 (48%) (non significant by Binomial Test) and 20/51 (39%) (non significant); apraxics: 128/164 (78%) (P < 0.0001) and 82/151 (54%) (P < 0.05). L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1109 3.3.2. Discussion Apraxics are more likely to produce responses like those of control subjects for objects that require a prehensile response than for those that do not. Six of nine apraxics performed better with real prehensile than non-prehensile objects in Pantomime, Point, or both conditions. Four apraxics were similarly better with novel prehensile than novel non-prehensile objects. Additionally, errors produced by apraxics were prehensile (clenches or pinches) significantly more often than would be expected by chance. Finally, analyses of non-prehensile trials showed that while apraxic subjects were impaired in producing both palm and poke postures, they were particularly deficient in the latter. One possibility is that the requirement to isolate a single digit from the others renders poke configurations particularly difficult to represent in a computational sense. Because the pattern of poor performance with non-prehensile hand postures was evident in the Point condition, in which subjects did not have to program a hand configuration response, this finding can not be attributed to difficulty with motor programming of pokes or palms. Apraxics' failure to recognize the correct hand posture in a forced-choice context suggests that they are deficient in representations of hand posture configurations for non-prehensile responses to objects. However, another possibility that must be considered is that apraxic subjects, due to aphasia or general cognitive impairment, may have understood the task instructions to mean "show how you would take (grasp) the object". If so, they would have produced many inappropriate prehensile responses. There are several lines of evidence suggesting this is unlikely to have been the case. First, as noted, several of the non-apraxic LCVA patients had moderately large cortical lesions and comprehension impairments, yet performed normally on the experimental tasks. Second, as is evident in Table 7, all of the apraxics produced non-prehensile responses—albeit not as many as the control subjects— indicating that no patient exhibited an overall misunderstanding of the task. Third, the apraxics performed poorly even with non-prehensile objects for which there was unanimity or near unanimity (agreement 90% or better) among control subjects. For example, doorbell was associated with a poke response with 100% agreement among controls, but only two of the eight apraxics produced a poke (see Table 8). Push-button telephone had a 90% agreement rate among controls, but none of the apraxics produced a poke. Bongo drums had 100% agreement for a palm response, and only three apraxic subjects produced a palm. The numbers were similar for the Point condition. Thus, even when non-prehensile functional responses are very strongly associated with objects, the apraxics still frequently failed to produce or recognize them. Finally, as noted earlier, the hand posture component of the background transitive gesture testing was a strong independent predictor of experimental task performance, consistent with the possibility that the same representations mediate performance on both tasks. 4. General discussion In this study, we explored whether deficits in the production and recognition of hand postures associated with objects are prevalent in ideomotor apraxia (IM), and if so, whether they are specific to the apraxia syndrome. The experimental task required responses to real objects for which function and structure agreed or conflicted, as well as novel objects for which structure was the sole informant of hand posture. On the assumption that IM most frequently results from deficient access to or integrity of stored gesture representations, we expected the majority of apraxics to exhibit a pattern of performance consistent with impairment of hand posture representations, but integrity of responses to structure. Our predictions were largely confirmed. All of the apraxic subjects, but none of the non-apraxic brain-lesioned subjects, produced abnormal hand posture responses to objects. In the case of real objects, most apraxics were impaired in recognition as well as in production, indicating damage to the representations underlying knowledge of appropriate hand postures for functional object interactions. In contrast, nearly all of the apraxic subjects performed normally in recognizing the hand postures appropriate for interacting with novel objects, suggesting relative integrity of response to object structure. Several investigators have proposed a distinction between object-oriented actions programmed and adjusted "on line" in response to object properties, and actions based upon memorial representations of objects. These different types of action have been proposed to be mediated by the dorsal and ventral visual processing systems, respectively. The dorsal stream projects from striate to posterior parietal cortex and premotor regions of the frontal lobe [13]. It was originally characterized as a spatial system coding object location ("where" information), in contrast to the "what" representations of the ventral system. More recent physiological evidence has indicated, however, that the dorsal system is largely dedicated to computation of movements of the effectors required to bring objects into proximity (e.g. foveation, head turning, reaching with the limbs), leading Milner and Goodale [14] to characterize it as the "how" system. A large neuronal population in the dorsal stream is involved in the coding of hand grasping movements. Neurons in the premotor area F5 in the monkey are selective for different types of grip responsive to the structural properties of objects: precision grip, finger prehension, and whole-hand prehension [15]. Neurons coding different types of grip in response to object structure are also present in a part of the intraparietal sulcus (area AIP) which is connected closely with area F5 [16] According to Gallese et al. [17], both F5 and AIP may participate in the transformation of visual information about object structure into information about "graspability" of objects in terms of affordances. The ventral system's representation of objects is proposed to differ from this in two ways. Consistent with the 1110 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 "how" versus "what" distinction, Jeannerod [18] has proposed that the dorsal system generates a "pragmatic" representation of object attributes relevant to action, as contrasted with a "semantic" mode important for object recognition and categorization, coded by the ventral system. According to Goodale et al. [19], the primary distinction concerns the type of representation processed by each system. Unlike the target-directed actions computed by the dorsal system, which operate in real time, ventral structures are specialized for computing and storing memorial information about objects over longer time intervals. For example, the requirement to pantomime a gesture near an object, rather than acting directly upon it, requires buffering and spatiomotor transformation of information that places relatively strong demands on representational motor memory. Thus, the dorsal system in isolation is not equipped to act in locations other than those containing target objects. These characterizations of the dorsal/ventral division of labor (which, incidentally, are not mutually exclusive) suggest a potential explanation for our apraxic subjects' pattern of performance. Apraxics' relatively unimpaired structural responses to novel objects may be mediated by intact dorsal stream processes, whereas their impaired responses to familiar objects and deficient pantomimes are attributable to damage to structures in the inferior parietal lobe aligned relatively more closely to the ventral system. Although concerned with actions upon objects (like the dorsal system), the inferior parietal lobe appears to mediate stored representations, as opposed to "on line" computations (for review see [20]). Lesion data suggest all 9 apraxics have lesions including Brodmann areas 39 or 40 (angular and supramarginal gyri of the inferior parietal lobule), and 8 of these subjects have involvement of both of these regions. Conversely, inferior parietal regions were involved in only one of the eight non-apraxic controls. This difference is significant (x2 — 3.9, P < 0.05). The control subject with inferior parietal involvement on acute neuroimaging scans had been apraxic acutely, as evidenced by hospital records, suggesting the possibility that the inferior parietal damage may have resolved to some degree. The neuroanatomic data suggest that poor performance on the experimental task may be associated with inferior parietal damage. This is consistent with a recent lesion study with 41 left hemisphere stroke patients demonstrating that ideomotor apraxia may be associated with lesions of the left inferior parietal lobe and intraparietal sulcus [21]. Given that none of the apraxic subjects have inferior parietal sparing, it remains possible that apraxia resulting from lesions that spare the inferior parietal lobe would also be associated with deficits in hand posture representations. Additional support for the present account would be obtained if it could be shown that apraxic patients with inferior parietal sparing are unimpaired in recognizing learned hand postures. Data we are currently collecting on a gesture recognition task with large numbers of apraxic patients should be helpful in this regard. The neuroanatomic basis for the relative preservation of subjects' performance with novel objects is at least potentially consistent with the possibility of dorsal system mediation. The superior parietal lobe (areas 5 and/or 7) is spared in five of the eight apraxics. None of the apraxics had lesions involving both areas five and seven. It is thus the case that all of the apraxics had sparing of at least some of the superior parietal architecture. Larger group studies are required to confirm these trends. Given that several apraxics did have superior parietal involvement, what is the behavioral evidence that the dorsal stream is in fact functioning relatively normally in these subjects? The first line of evidence is that in contrast to their difficulties with evocation of stored representations and with performance of pantomime, the apraxics performed relatively normally in reaching and grasping tasks, in which relevant information is delivered "on line" by visual perception. This pattern of performance doubly dissociates from patients with lesions to dorsal structures (most frequently superior parietal lobule) who exhibit optic ataxia. Optic ataxics are impaired in visually-guided reaching and object-oriented grasping, but are frequently unimpaired in gesture production [22]. The fact that the apraxics did not exhibit disturbances of goal-directed reaching is consistent with integrity of the dorsal system (see [14,16,17,19,22]). Although admittedly more speculative, an additional line of evidence potentially consistent with apraxics' reliance upon dorsal stream processes are the data from the analyses suggesting relative integrity of production and recognition of prehensile postures. Recent work by Sirigu et al. [1] (in preparation) using 3D kinematic analyses of hand posture supports the contention that non-prehensile hand postures are performed relatively poorly by patients with IM. One possibility, not inconsistent with the "pragmatic" versus "semantic" distinction proposed by Jeannerod [18], is that prehensile hand posture representations are mediated by the dorsal system, which is relatively intact in IM. In contrast, non-prehensile configurations such as poke and palm are exploratory hand postures that are important in object identification. Consequently, they may be relatively closely aligned with ventral systems coding object identity information, and thus localized to neuroanatomic structures inferior to the system representing prehensile postures. A possible locus for the computation of non-prehensile hand posture representations is the inferior parietal lobule.5 Non-prehensile, exploratory representations like poke and palm may not be the only postures mediated by the gesture representation system. This system may contain 5 Milner and Goodale [14] have demonstrated that prehensile postures such as clench and pinch may be used for exploration as well, but that applied force and skin contact area differ when the postures are used for the purpose of grasping versus exploration. Unlike non-prehensile postures, prehensile postures may be redundantly represented in both the dorsal and gesture engram systems and activated differently in different contexts. If so, these two types of prehensile representations could theoretically be differentially damaged by brain lesions. L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1111 the features of even prehensile hand postures when these are object-specific, responsive to function, and critical in distinguishing a given posture from others. To return to the example from the apraxic patient of Sirigu et al. [1], cited in Section 1, although a spoon can be grasped with a whole-hand power grip, the precise learned hand posture appropriate to eating with a spoon entails holding the spoon's handle between the forefinger and thumb while it rests along the third knuckle of the middle finger. These attributes of a spoon-hold are likely to be represented in the gesture engram as a range of values across various parameters such as "thumb-forefinger aperture", "finger flexion/extension", etc. Such parameters for finger position and hand configuration are normally activated when the specific "spoon" posture is evoked by the intention to eat with a spoon, and precise values along each parameter are subsequently specified in response to the spatial features of the particular spoon to be used. Our data suggest that in apraxic patients, the damaged memorial parameters for specific finger and hand configurations may be over-ridden by more generic representations of precision and power grip computed in response to object structure. Acknowledgements Supported by NIH Grant R29NS36387 to the first author. We gratefully acknowledge the collaboration of Pas-cale Pradat-Diehl, who referred patients and assisted in running subjects, H. Branch Coslett, who assisted with analysis of lesion location, and Megan Bartlett-Williams, Tania Gio-vannetti, Catherine Lai, Ivan Kautter, Perdita Permaul, and Adrian Wilson, who ran subjects and performed data analyses. We are grateful also to an anonymous reviewer for his or her careful reading, which resulted in important improvements to this manuscript. Appendix A. Background tests A.l. Praxis testing Subjects performed a battery of gestural praxis tasks which included transitive gesture to sight of objects, transitive gesture upon imitation of the examiner (no object present), and gesture with object in hand (Object Use condition). The same 10 gestures (pantomimed use of scissors, watch, razor, fork, comb, toothbrush, bottle opener, eraser, and cigarette lighter) were assessed in each condition. Subjects were asked to "pretend you are actually holding and using (the object)". Subjects also performed five intransitive gestures (wave goodbye, salute, hitchhike, come here, and stop) to command and imitation. Gestures were scored with the system reported in Buxbaum et al. [4] by an experienced coder naive to subjects' identity and background. First, each gesture received a score of 1 or 0 for content. Substitutions of an incorrect (e.g. semantically related) but recognizable gesture were scored 0. Gestures receiving a 1 on the Content scale were scored 0 or 1 on four Spatiotem-poral scales: hand Posture, Arm Posture, Amplitude, and Timing. To ascertain that subjects understood the requirement to pantomime the hand posture associated with using the object, the first instance of body part as object (BPO) error (e.g. use of finger as a toothbrush) was corrected, and subjects were permitted a second attempt for that trial. Any subsequent BPO errors resulted in a "0" score for hand Posture. Detailed scoring guidelines are provided in Buxbaum et al. [4]. Due to scheduling constraints, French subjects performed only the gesture to sight of object and intransitive gesture to command tasks. A.2. Sensory-motor battery: grip strength, dexterity, tactile sensation, proprioception Manual strength of each hand was assessed by subjective grip test and characterized as either normal or paretic. Manual dexterity was measured with three digit movement tasks: alternating extension and flexion of the first and last digits, crossing and uncrossing of the second and first digits, and thumb-finger sequential touch. The number of repetitions in 10 s was tallied. Tactile sensation was tested with three trials in which a light pinprick was applied to the dorsum of each hand and three trials in which the pinprick was withheld (in randomized order); subjects indicated by yes/no response whether they had felt a touch. There were three measures of proprioception, performed with the subjects' eyes closed: two tasks requiring reproduction with one hand of a position formed by the experimenter with the subject's other hand, and a third task requiring judgments of finger movements in the vertical plane (up or down). A.3. Grasping objects Subjects' ability to form an appropriate hand aperture for object grasping was also assessed. Ten of the objects from Study 2 (doorknob, light switch, clothespin, drinking glass, pliers, saltshaker, calculator, apple, lemon, and crayon) were placed on a table top at midline 40 cm from subjects' body wall. Subjects were asked to reach out and grasp each object, once with each hand (when contralesional hand use was possible). Performance was videotaped and each grasp was coded on a 3 point scale by a single coder who was naive as to subjects' identities (2: normal hand aperture for object, 1: clumsy or mildly imprecise, 0: grossly abnormal; includes ineffective grasp, aperture much too narrow or wide). Maximum score was 20 points. A.4. Gesture recognition Subjects completed a subtest of the Florida Apraxia Battery [23] which assesses recognition of 30 videotaped 1112 L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 transitive gestures. After viewing a gesture performed twice in succession, subjects were shown a choice of three words, one of which denotes the gesture (e.g. "hammering a nail"), one of which is a semantically-related gesture (e.g. "screwing in a screw") and one of which is unrelated. The words were read aloud by the examiner, and the subject was required to select the word corresponding to the seen gesture. Fifteen older control subjects (mean age 54.5, range 40-74) obtained a mean score of 29.3 (range 28-30; S.D. 0.8). A.5. Static posture imitation In a task modeled closely after one reported by Gold-enberg [24], subjects viewed a videotape of the head and shoulders of an actor, posing on each trial with her left hand in 10 different positions relative to her head (e.g. hand under chin, palm forward; hand beside left ear, etc.). There were 2 trials with each posture, presented in randomized order. Each posture was displayed for 10s, with a 7s inter-stimulus interval. Subjects were required to imitate the posture as precisely as possible and were permitted to begin while watching the model. Goldenberg [24] reported data from 60 healthy control subjects (mean age 54.9, S.D. 14.0) who performed a version of the task with the same static postures we used, presented once to each subject, but scored 2 points for correct performance on the first try and 1 point for correct performance on the second try. Mean Score was 19.7 (S.D. 0.6, range 18-20). A.6. Moss Object Knowledge Probes Test Black-and-white photographs of each of the 35 real objects of the experimental tasks of Study 2 were presented singly to brain-lesioned subjects. Subjects were asked to name each object; failing this, they received a single phonemic cue (e.g. hammer -> ha, etc.). Objects that were not named correctly even with phonemic cueing were not further assessed. For named objects, three aspects of semantic object knowledge were assessed with a forced-choice format in which two printed words or phrases were displayed below each photograph. In the "Function" condition, the two choices referred to possible functions of the stimulus (e.g. tightening, pounding). In the "Manipulation" condition, the forced-choice concerned how the object is held and used (e.g. swung up and down, moved in a circle). In the "Physical" condition, the forced-choice concerned the object's visual or tactile characteristics (e.g. soft or hard); efforts were made to avoid characteristics that could be gleaned directly from the photographs. Conditions were blocked. The printed items were read aloud by the examiner (e.g. "is this object for tightening, or for pounding?") while the subjects examined them, and subjects indicated their choice by pointing response. Ten healthy older adult control subjects (mean age 64.9 years, range 51-76; mean education 13.6 years, range 11-18) obtained the following scores: function mean 100% correct, S.D. 0%; manipulation mean 100% correct, S.D. 1%, range 97-100%; physical mean 97% correct, S.D. 2%, range 94-100%. References [1] Sirigu A, et al. A selective impairment of hand posture for object utilization in apraxia. Cortex 1995;31:41-55. [2] Heilman KM, Rothi LGJ. Apraxia. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical neuropsychology. 2nd ed. Oxford University Press: New York; 1985. p. 131-50. [3] Heilman KM, Gonzales Rothi L, Mack L, Feinberg T, Watson RT. Apraxia after a superior parietal lesion. Cortex 1986;22:141- 50. [4] Buxbaum LJ, Giovanetti T, Libon D. The role of the dynamic body schema in praxis: evidence from primary progressive apraxia. Brain and Cognition 2000;44:166-91. [5] Heilman KM, Rothi LJ, Valenstein E. Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology 1982;32:342-6. [6] Klatzky RL, McCloskey B, Doherty S, Pellegrino JEA. Knowledge about hand shaping and knowledge about objects. Journal of Motor Behavior 1987;19(2):187-213. [7] Heilman KM, Gonzalez Rothi LJ. Apraxia, In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. p. 141-50. [8] Damasio H, Damasio AR. Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1989. [9] Kertesz A. The western aphasia battery. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1982. [10] Roach A, Schwartz MF, Martin N, Grewal RS, Brecher A. The Philadelphia naming test: scoring and rationale. Clinical Aphasiology 1996;24:121-33. [11] Buxbaum LJ, Saffran EM. Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and non-apraxic subjects. Brain and Cognition 2002;82:179-99. [12] Buxbaum LJ, Veramonti T, Schwartz MF. Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: knowing 'what for' but not 'how'. Neurocase 2000;6:83-97. [13] Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Manfield RJW, editors. Analysis of visual behavior. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press; 1982. [14] Milner AD, Goodale MA. The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1995. [15] Rizzolatti G, et al. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. Experimental Brain Research 1988;71:491— 507. [16] Taira M, Mine S, Georgopoulos AP, Murata A, Sakata H. Parietal cortex neurons of the monkey related to the visual guidance of hand movement. Experimental Brain Research 1990;83:29-36. [17] Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogasi L, Luppino G, Murata A. A parietal-frontal circuit for hand grasping movements in the monkey: evidence from reversible inactivation experiments. In: Their P, Karnath H-O, editors. Parietal lobe contributions to orientation in 3-D space, Berlin: Springer; 1997. [18] Jeannerod M. Object oriented action. Insights into the reach grasp movement. In: Bennet KMB, Castiello U, editors. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1994. 3-15. [19] Goodale MA, Jakobson LS, Keillor JM. Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia 1994;32(10): 1159-78. L.J. Buxbaum et al. /Neuropsychologic! 41 (2003) 1091-1113 1113 [20] Buxbaum LJ. Ideomotor apraxia: a call to action. Neurocase 2001;7:445-58. [21] Haaland KY, Harrington DL, Knight RT. Neural representations of skilled movement. Brain 2000;123:2306-13. [22] Perenin MT, Vighetto A. Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. Brain 1988;111:643-74. [23] Gonzalez Rothi LJ, et al. Florida apraxia battery: experimental edition; 1991. [24] Goldenberg G. imitating getures and manipulating a mannikin-the representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia 1995;33(l):63-72.