ASCH'S LINE JUDGMENT STUDIES 5 Conformity Revisiting Asch's line-judgment studies Jolanda Jetten and Matthew J. Hornsey 7 + 2 = 5' George Orwell, Nineteen Eighty Four BACKGROUND Obviously, 2+2 does not equal S. However, as Orwell suggests, if you find yourself in a world where people believe this to be true, you might start to doubt your judgment. Perhaps you even start to think that this must be true; 'they are right and so I must be wrong'. For some decades now social psychologists have had a key interest in the factors that lead us to go along with others' views rather than stand strong and resist the pressure to conform. Of particular importance in advancing our knowledge about conformity and resistance were the line-judgment studies by Solomon Asch [1951,1955). Asch asked why we sometimes abandon our firmly held convictions and bring our attitudes and judgments in line with those of other people, even if we know that they are wrong and we are right. That is, why do we at times yield to conformity pressures and seem to uncritically adopt the majority point of view? As Asch (1955) writes, his interest in conformity started when he read about classic research by, among others, the psychologist Edward L. Thorndike. In these studies, people are asked to give their opinion on a topic and after doing so they are confronted with an authority figure or group of peers saying the opposite. What typically happens in these studies is that when people are asked again for their opinion, they shift their views and attitudes towards those expressed by the peers or authority. Like many others, Asch was puzzled by this finding. Why would people change their mind when the authority or majority did not even present any arguments to support their views? Why would the sheer number of opponents or the fact that an authority provides another attitude lead to conformity? THE LINE-JUDGMENT STUDIES To address such questions, Asch devised an experimental paradigm that was modelled closely on the experimental set-up in these suggestion studies. However, there was one important difference: in his experiments he created a situation in which various features of the task and the social context made it extremely difficult to resist conformity pressure despite it being very clear that conforming would mean giving an incorrect response. Perhaps the best way to explain the experiment is to invite you to imagine that you were a participant in one of these studies. After arriving at the laboratory, you find yourself in the company of between seven and nine other participants and you are told that you are all taking part in a psychological study of visual judgment. The experimenter informs you that you will be comparing the length of various lines. You are shown two large white cards (see Figure 5.1). One has a reference line and on the other card you see three comparison lines (labeled A, B and C). Your task is to say out loud which of the three comparison lines is similar in length to the reference line. One of the three lines is clearly the same length as the reference line and the other two are obviously shorter or longer. You think this will be easy. Target line (8 inches) Comparison lines (6, 8, 7 inches) Figure 5.1 The stimuli in Asch's standard line-judgment studies.The participant's (seemingly very easy) task is to say which of the comparison lines (A, B, or C) matches the target line on the left jztten & hornsey Participants are seated and they are presented with several trials where they have to call out their answers in the order in which they are seated. You are seated in a position where almost all the other participants have to call out their response before you. The study starts quite uneventfully; everyone agrees which comparison line is similar in length to the reference line. This is getting boring. But then, suddenly, on the third trial, the first participant calls out what is obviously the wrong answer. For example, given lines like those in Figure 5.1, they say 'A' rather than 'B'. They must have made a mistake and you reassure yourself that they cannot be correct by looking again closely at the lines. But then, the second and third person give the same answer as the first person. Number four and five also call out a letter associated with a line that appears to be clearly much longer (or shorter) than the reference line. Are they all blind? Then it is your turn. What do you answer? Should you just go along with their response (even though, privately, you are pretty sure they are wrong), or should you stick to your own judgment even though you are the only one giving this answer? Unbeknownst to you, the other participants are not actually real participants at all, but are assistants of the experimenter ('confederates') who have been instructed to call out wrong answers on 12 critical trials. The study was not about-visual perception, but an investigation into conformity. The results show that it is fairly difficult to withstand conforming in such contexts, even though it is clear that the majority is wrong. There are a number of ways in which the key findings have been reported and, for now, we focus on the way the results are typically reported in the majority of social psychology textbooks. The responses on the critical trials (where the majority clearly gave the wrong response) are presented in Figure 5.2. One popular way of summarizing these findings is to say that 76% of participants conformed at least once to an incorrect answer given by the majority. 40-, B 30 c is ä o 20 's!