by ERVING GOFFMAN f CONTENTS 65 Editor's Introduction / Sol Worth 69' Gender Display 79 Picture Frames 92 Gender Commercials PUBUCATiON OF THE SOCIETY fOR THE AWTHROPQLOGY OF VISUAL COIVIIUUi&UCATipW I Reproduced in this paper are some commercial still I photographs—ads—featuring human subjects. In addition, ' some use is made of news shots of "actual" persons, that is; of models who are being pictured in their own capacity. My assumption is that anyone whose picture appears in media print .figs almost certainly cooperated in the process and therefore—like a professional .modd—has placed this appearance in the public domain, foregoing the protection from social analysis that persons, at least living ones, can strongly claim regarding pictures taken for home consumption. The pictures reproduced were selected at will from newspapers and current popular magazines easy to hand—at least to my hand.' They were chosen to fit into sets, each set-to allow .the displaying, delineating, or mocking up of a discrete theme bearing on gender, especially female gender, and arranged with malice within each'set to the same end. Each set of pictures is accompanied informally by some verbal text. SI Some comments first concerning how pictures can and I can't be used in social analysis. My^arm-is'thSt'the themes that can- be. delineated through- pictures have a very mixed ontologieal status and -that any attempt tolegisiate as to the order of fact represented'in these themes is likely to be optimistic: ' ' (1) The student of commercial pictures can draw a ^ random .sample from a magazine's particular issue, or from a defined period of issue, or from a specified list of magazines, and disclaim characterizing other issues, periods, or magazines, even more so other sources of pictures, such as newsprint, postcards, and the like, not even to mention actual life itself. Specifiable representativeness, then, is a way that a collection of pictures could .qualify2—and a way the pictures about to' be analyzed do not. (Of course, findings based on ,a systematic sample very often get their weight-from the fact that the reader can be trusted to generalize the findings beyond their stated universe, statistical warrant for which.would require another study, which, if done, would induce a stili broader overgeneraiization, and so on, but that is another matter.) Observe--that this .sort of representativeness pertains to picture's as s'Uch and'doesn't'teAWu.5. what-we very often want to know, namely-}what-aspects-'of 1 And to that of a fellow student, Michi is'hida. 2For a recent exampie, see Robinson (1976). reaMife pictures provide us a fair image of, and what social effect-veowtrrtereial picturing -has-jupon tbe-.'lJfa . tftat Ay puKpopletA^piaarred—a limitation also of the purposely selected pictures displayed here. (2) Since there is little constraint on what I elect to identify as a theme {a^'gerrderism"}, or which pictures I bring together in order to display what is thus identified, or on the way! order the stills within a given series, it could be taken tharahything could be depicted that I can manage to suggest through what appears to be common to a few pictures. Success here requires nothing more than a small amount of perversity and wit.and a large batch of pictures to choose from. The larger the initial collection, the more surely the analyst can find.cjanfirrning examples of what he thinks he has found in one picture or would in any case like to depict-a case of representativeness declining as the data base increases. So effective depiction of a theme cannot in itself prove anything about what is found in pictures or, of course, in the world. Indeed, something like the method I use is employed by artful compilers of photegr-aptric funny books, -. camera pranksters who match" gesticulatory pictures of famous citizens against animals and plants apparently engaged in similarly characterizable posjures, or who superimpose ^ballooned though^^awer^ltatements, these formulated toMJefine the situation as it never was in actual life, committing the protagonists to respons^s~oT"a wildly scurifous. kind. So, too, the texts accompanying the-p.ictures, are cast in the style of generalization-by-pronouncement found in the writings of fv^m&&^bo4y>:Airi§ms^ strayed ethologists, and lesser journalists. (3)'The particular matters I want to consider raise three distinct and general methodological questions that should the first two will here be at issue, these two allowing me to » exploit without a major research investment the very special advantages of working with photographs, which advantages are as follows: (i) There is a class of behavioral practices—what'might be ' called "sjm&™b&hwmm$''—whose physical forms are fairly-well codified even though the social implications or meaning of the acts may have vague elements, and which are realized in their entirety, from beginning to end, in a brief period of-time and a small space. These behavioral events can be, recorded and their image made retrievable by means of audio; and video tapes and camera. (Tape and film, unlike a stilly provide not only a recoverable image of an actuai instance o(f the activity in question, but aiso an appreciable collection of. these records. More important, audio and video recordings off very small behaviors facilitate micro-functional study, that is,| an examination of the role of a bit of behavior in the stream! which precedes, co-occurs, and follows.) The coincidence of I a subject matter and a recording technology places the] student in an entirely novel relation to his data, forming th'el practical basis for microanalysis. This special research! situation should not be confused with the use of recording! technology to document a news story, provide a feef for a! community, limn in the contours of a relationship, depict the! history of a nation, or any Other matter whose meaning is\ not linked to a fixed physical, form which can be realized in ■■ the round in a recordable space and time. . \ (IT) Pictures from any source are now cheap and easy-'-t© 92 SIUDi£S»¥H£^WR©lp^ reproduce in uniform slide form. A collection allows for easy arranging and rearranging, a search and rnock-up, trial and error juggling, something between cryptography and doing jigsaw puzzles, a remarkable aid both to uncovering patterns and finding examples, whether mere illustrations or actual instance records. (iii) The student can exploit the vast social competency of the eye and the impressive consensus sustained by viewers. Behavioral configurations which he has insufficient literary skill to summon up through words alone, he can yet unambiguously introduce into consideration. His verbal glosses can serve as a means to direct the eye to what is to be seen, instead of having to serve as a full rendition of what is at issue. The notion of a "merely subjective response" can then be academically upgraded; for clearly part of what one _ refrains from studying because the only approach is through verbal vagaries has a specific nature and is precisely perceived, the vagary being a characteristic of one's literary ^incapacity, not one's data.3 (iv) A set of pictorial examples (whether illustrations or instance records) of a common theme provides more than a device for making sure that the pattern in question wilt be clear to the viewer. Often one or two examples would suffice for that. Nor does the size of the set relate to the traditional sampling notion of showing how prevalent were cases of a particular kind in the sample and (by extension) in. the sampled universe. Something else is involved, different pi^Q^laKgXra^pj^ textual baekg^ ■dispaBiities^veto It is the \depth and breadth of these contextual differences which somehow provide a sense of structure, a sense of a single organization underlying mere surface differences, which sense is. not generated simply by reference to the numerical size of the-set relative to the size of the sample. Whereas in traditional methods the differences between items that are to be counted as instances of the same thing are an embarrassment, and are so in the degree of their difference, in pictorial pattern analysis the opposite is the case, the casting together of these apparent differences being what the analysis is all about Indeed, something is to be learned even when an advertiser in effect performs analysis backwards, that is, starts with the same models and the same sales pitch and. then searches out different possible scenes as vehicles for them and it-all this in the hope of building product interest through a mixture of repetition and novelty. For in The ear as well as the eye provides an impressive competency, and here phoneticians (and lately those interested in conversational^ analysis} have made an exemplary effort to formuiate notation systems that can be printed on paper yet avoid the* limitations of ordinary orthography, thus providing a bridge between sounds and publications. The problem is that although trained students can produce the same transcription of a given spate of sound, the formulation they produce will equally apply to expressions which they would hear as significantly different. Given a recording to listen to, a linguist's transcription can serve as a very adequate means of directing the ear's attention to a particular sound and with that the full competency of the ear can be academically exploited. But written transcriptions without recordings do not solve the problem. (Nor, ! believe, does it help much to package a tape in the jacket of a book, along with encouragement of do-it-yourself analysis.) The printing of the analysis of videotape records presents still-greater problems. purposely setting out to ring changes on a set theme, the advertiser must nonetheless satisfy scene-production requirements such as propriety, understandability, and so forth, thereby necessarily demonstrating that, and how, different ingredients can be choreographed to "express" the same theme. Here, certainty, it is entirely an artifact of how advertisements are assembled that a set of them will exhibit a common underlying pattern, and here the student is only uncovering what was purposely implanted to this end in the first place. But how the advertiser succeeds in finding different guises for his stereotypes still instructs in the matter of how the materials of real scenes can be selected and shaped to provide a desired reading. —-x (4) The pictures I have un-randomly collected of gender-. relevant behavior can be used to jog one's consideration of three matters: the- gender::behavioral styjes: found: in actual life, the ways in' yjyhich.rad.yertiserncnts .might present. a> \ t&l!&^W&^^HWfrim^.^ithou^ri my primary interest,is .actual j&]M§&d$]^t!&> the pictures are accompanied by "textual glosses that raise questions of any order that might be stimulated by the'pictures. In any case, what will mostly be shown and discussed is advertisers-' views of hov/;. woroeq can., be;■ pE#itabjM;>fflcJUKsd My unsubstantiated generalizations have the slight saving grace that they mostly refer to the way gender is pictured, not the way it is actually performed. (5) By and large, I did not look for pictures that exhibited' what seemed to me to be- common to the two -■ sexes, whether just in pictures or in reality as well. Nor for pictures that dealt with sex differences which I assumed were widely and well-understood. The vast amount of what is—at ..'least to me—unremarkable in advertisements is thus vastly underrepresented. (Something of the same bias actually informs every ethnography; it is differences from one's own world and unexpected similarities that get recorded.) But * given these limitations, once a genderism was identified as ^ one worth mocking-up, almost all sex role exceptions and ^ reversals I came across were selected. It is to be added that -* although the advertising business is focused (in the U.S.A.) in J} New York, and although models and photographers' are "«* drawn from a very special population indeed, their productjs U3 trejJed^as_QMbing-out-of-the-or^dinary byviewers, somethinjpj-j* '"ofajy naturaJ-^ntTrief.' altfTough~ThTrjTctures shown here" J^, . cannot be taken as representative of gender behavior in real_ p life or even representative of advertisements in general or H I particular publication sources in particular, one can probably make, .a...significant negative statement about them, namely, that -ja-> p-leffires they a(% not be$b&¥$ How can stills present the world when in the world persons are engaged in1 courses of action, in doings through time (not frozen posturings), where sound is almost as important as sight, and smell .and touch figure as well? Moreover, in the world, we can know the individuals before f us personally, something unlikely of pictures used in/ advertising. Some of the solutions to this problem are obvious. A scene can be simulated in which figures are captured in those acts which stejrgolypkall^^jjtorni^e.. the sequence from which Lhgy^-are. taken--presurnab'y because these acts are identified as happening only in the course of, and momentarily during, an extended action. Thus viewers are /led to read backward and forward in sequence time from the moment of vision.5 Another solution-is to/draw: on^sc-enes that-are- themselves- silent- and static in- real life:-.steeping* pensive- posesy-window; •shopping,- - and, importantly, the off-angle fixed looks through which we are taken to convey our overall alignment to what another person—one not looking at us directly—is saying or doing. Another solution is to'position4he characters in thepicture microecologically-so that-theirr placement relative to one another will provide an indsxj::ei.-!aik tjitcTVset in cin't finJ in/ col™ :roioire myotic :v- Uong:witlvsfoM-lut-vvfitingdesk- - He said Tint bigth ic> We satd Think We thought syW.drather not '■■■have thal-bigsTV :.tve staring ^c-yotc-1 ill the tmte 1-1 c ÜÜil Äs r-t d 1-1 a \'2 And here exceptions seem to prove' the rule. For on the very few occasions when women- are pictured taller than men. the men ■ seem almas. ayxto_be :ocJai~xiass, not' only subordjnati status; _blfi[aßoTho^ "crafEbound servitors who—it might^a; plSäT^cl^ circumscribed terms of their modest trade! ~~~ ~ ' / 1-2 Q ^i I Arr I cb ■ In 1-2 c 96 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION 1-3 The theme of relative size is sometimes employed as a basis for symboliza-tion, that is, designing a picture whose every detail speaks to a single thematic issue: ■ ■■■ 1-3 c 1-3 d II THE FEMININE TOUCH* Or 11-1 Women,,more than" men, are pictured using: their, fingers and hands to trace the outlines of an object or to cradle it or to caress'its surface (the latter sometimes under the guise of guiding it), or to effect a "just barely touching" of the kind that might be significant between two electrically charged bodies. This ritualistic touching is to be distinguished from the utilitarian kind that grasps, manipulates, or holds: Mm; H-1 b (continued) Here and elsewhere in connection with the role of fingers {see Vl-2 and'VI3, pp. 125-126), I draw directly on observations made by Michi ishida, to whom I give thanks. GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 97 ■ l1i^mwu'ro£h*-'nnhliigdw(shiHi[>cii<:ii>i lll-5o Admittedly there is the popular notion that members of the aristocratically inclined classes traditionally engaged personal servants to obtain body-connected care that members of the middle classes would want to provide for themselves, ashamedness here being a support of democracy. Of course, correlated with personal servicing was the non-person treatment of those who provided it. It appears.thatwome^arftiQ^^ •"fr^"men than glvingjLioJtheB^'rtd-arfe-..... ' not depicted "markedly Buying, their response:* II[-5 d III-5 f GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 103 /IJt*6 Which raises the questions of how / males-are-pTctured' when' hi ~th"e" domains of -the -tradffion^vaaSpi^y^rwJ--^m.-.petenee......'erf "TwSiies^-thej; kitchen, • .the nursery and the living" room, when..it >is being cleaned: One answer, borrowed from"ttf^arRf^oTs1t^ is to picture the male en.pged 'in no ..c^ntributfag"^.......this-, way avoiding either subordination or... con-Y tam mation^ith'a ^Fecaale?"'..tasik: 111-6 c m-7<7 111-7 6 i \ l!i-7c (continued) 104 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION lll-7s 111-7/ '"'8 ^subtfer^techniq^Jsjtojdjmyjtfxe... male' to pursue the alien activitygmder _the_d irecJLap^ajsjr^ can do^hjjjteW doing were itse)gb^iJy.3.y.--otl>eing-a-lark "ofXaare.. a smiliLOP^e-face- of the., doe r or the mtch^^ii^tlng^thS'Ssenthl\y uriserious_eisay.ed..Gh-ai=acter'B'f the undertaking?2' / lll-8o (continued) Correspondingly, when females are pictured engaged in a traditionally male task, a male may (as it were) parenthesize the activity, looking on appraisingly, condescendingly, or with wonder: III-8 fn a GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 105 Iii Iii 11 ru-s 6 IV THE FAMILY The nuclear family as a basic unit of social organization is well adapted to the requirements of pictorial representation. All of the members of almost any actual family can be contained easily within the same close picture, and, properly positioned, a visual representation of the members can nicely serve as asymboliza-tion of the family's social structure. IV-1 Turning to mocked-up families in advertisements, one finds that the allocation of.;at least one-girl and at least one boy ensures that a symbolization of the full set of intrafamily:relations-can be effected. For example, devices are employed to exhibit the presumed special bond between' the girl and the mother and the- boy and the father; sometimes in the same picture: 1 K*^ft-i^fv^ (continued) 106 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION IV'2 Although in commercial scenes a unity- is symbolized between fathers and sons and between mothers and daughters, there is a suggestion that different types of unity might be involved. In a word, there is a tendency for women to be pictured- as. more akin to their daughters (and to themselves in younger years) than is the case with men. Boys, as it were, have, to. push their way into manhood, and problematic effort is involved: / \ sir up m n H '11 U lV-2d war Mr^Yr? 'Xlg^ls-Canieia-ActioiT IV-2 b 1V'2 c Girls merely have to unfold: JMtntHL mart camfarthMc thnn WMrine nffHilrtf! IV-2 e / 1 •a r4 (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 107 II m fV-2 5 IV-2 i IV-2 ft 1V*3 Often the father (orin his absence, a son) stands a little outside the,physical c ircl e of the o ther mem bets of the fam il y, as 'if ■ tOMexpress-v:;a«irelatf,Qnship..whose protectiveness is linked with, perhaps even requires, distance: IV-3 a IV-3 6 IV-2 ; IV-3c (continued) 108 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION An interesting contrast is to be found in turn-of-the-century portrait poses of couples, wherein the effect was often achieved of displaying the man as the centra! figure and the woman as backup support, somewhat in,the manner of a chief lieutenant. I cite from Lesy (1973): ^If— ti* A IV-3 to a IV-3 f/7 c IV-3 fn d 5Perhaps the contrast be-I tween past I and current portraits less betokens a change in underlying social organization than in conventions of expression within the I picture for- lv-3 fn e GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 109 is «di«; ci^iu «v runes o stop going up?" IV-3 / V THE RITUALIZATION ORDINATION OF SUB- V'1 A classic stereotype of deference is that of lowering oneself physically in some form or other of prostration. Correspondingly, holding the body:.ereC;|,:arid/> the head high is stereo typically a mark of unashamedness, superiority, and, .disdain. Advertisers draw on (and endorse) the claimed universality of the theme: 110 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION V-1 h v-iy V»2 Beds and floors provide places in social situations where incumbent persons will be lower than .anyone sitting on a chair or standing; Floors also are associated with the. less-clean, lesspure,iess-exalted parts of a room—for example, the place to keep dogs, baskets of soiled clothes, street footwear, and the like. And a recumbent position is one from which physical defense of oneself can least well be initiated and therefore one which renders one very dependent on the benignness of the surround. (Of course,^ lying on the floor or on a sofa or bed J seems also to be a conventionalized ex- i pression of sexual availability;) The poinf ; here is that it appears that children and I women are pictured on floors and beds j more and appeasement: V«5 i Body cant: V'5 i b 15 From Darwin (1872:53, Fig. 6). V-5 i f V-5 ii c (continued) 116 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION 3 , 1 O V-5 ii r" t 1 *\ Nv-v; fir*1 ' V-5 ii /j V°6 Smiles, it can be argued, often function as ritualistic mollifiers, signaling thatK:-,riothiriig'-"agOri!strc is intended., or invited, that the meaning of the other's act" has'- been/understood and found acceptable, that, indeed; the other is approved and -appreciated. Those who warily keep an eye on the movements of a potential aggressor may find themselves automatically smiling should their gaze be "caught" by its object, who in turn may find little cause to smile back. In addition, a responding smile (even more so an appreciative laugh) following very rapidly on the heels of a speaker's sally can imply that the respondent belongs, by knowl-edgeability, at least, to ■ the speaker's circle. All of these smiles, then, seem more the offering of an inferior than a superior.. In any case,-it appears that in cross-sexed encounters in American society, women smile more, and more expansively, than men,16 which arrangement appears to be carried over into advertisements, perhaps with little conscious intent. IS ■Hi V-6o (continued) V-5 ii 3 16 See the (1973:49). comments in Weisstein GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 117 V-6 e V«7 Given the subordinated and indulged position of children in regard to adults, it would appear that to present oneself in packish'-styling-is toencourage the corresponding treatment. How much of this guise is found in real life, is an open question; but found it is in advertisements. 118 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION V-7 k \A8 The note of uiiserioiisness struck by a childlike guise is struck by another styling of the self, this one perhaps entirely restricted to advertisements, namely, the use of the entire body as a playful'' gesticulative device; a sort of body .clowning-; V-8c (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 119 V-S f \S A ssssm HB HC V-9 The special unseriousness involved in childlike guises and clowning suggests a readiness to be present in a social situation garbed and styled in a manner to which one isn't deeply or irrevocably committed. Perhaps reflected here is a readiness to try out various guises and to appear at various times in different ones, Tn~any case, in advertfsementsTat least, there seems to be an unanticipated difference between men and women«jMen_are~ displayed in forma!, business, and in-. foWaT^f, and alth^ V-S g Ttood thai the ■ same..; indiyidual^-wiH-iit different times appear ini'-al^-rfiieisfe-' ipuisesf _ ""each guise seems to afford him something " he is totally serious _ab_au.t,_and-deeply"" id en tiffed with,' as thou gh jvearing a s kin, no'ra"Costume:^ve'n'"!ri' the case of the cowboy garb that urban males affect recreationaily, little sense that one's whole appearance is aJark_,would seem to be present Women in ads seem to have a different relationship "to their clothing " "arrdto the gestures worn with it. Within ~ea"cfr~6road category (formal, busmess~ informal) there are choices which are considerably different one from another, and the sense is that one may as well try out various possibilities to see what comes of it.^As though life were j series of costume ban^TTTTuvoTie^caFoccasion-Ifliy modon?5 own appearance, for identification is not deep.^Jt m|gjr^jbe_. argued, Jh^,_^M--^&^^ums^Lks^. ■^h-araEE^ Trtents !ocates-"women-as'-st|ess':*seriously. ■ presentTff "social' sftuattoris':than- meri, the ' seff-prcsCTrtt!ththrotrgh'get-ups~bein If / in' a way ah" unserijKis Ihmg^Observe t.haK T!Te~extensionofthis argument to real life need not involve a paradox. It is a j common view that women spe.nd much 1 more of. their....time and., concern in , shopping for clothes and.preparing for 1 appearances than do men, and that women . set considerable store on the appreciative or depreciative-.response they , produce thereby. But, of course, so does an actor in a part he will never play again. A concern over carrying an appearance off does not necessarily imply a deep and abiding identification with that appearance. (This argument fits with the fact that women's styles change much more rapidly than do men's,) (continued) V STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION UtlfN CASUALS, IliiBllii^iil V-9( V-9f tenu too mum lor «om< »1_ *. ^*srf - »1 ft > fi V-9 (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 121 V-10/ V-10/J (continued) 122 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION V>10o V-10 r VTJ\ A male pictured- with a female .•sometimes appears to employ an. ex-■ tended arm;, iri effect- marking■ the boundary • of his social • property and guardthfit&against encroachment. A suggestion is that this miniature border patrol is especially found when the female at the same time is engaged in a ursjxHVwiich accords her authority. GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 123 V*12 There seem to be four main behavioral arrangments of pairs of persons which provide what is taken to be a physical expression that the. two are a "with," that is, together as a social unit with respect to the social situation in which they are located. (In all four cases, note, the work these dyadic tie-signs do in defining the relationship between figures in a picture would seem to be much the same as the work they do in real social situations.) V*12 i First, a matter of fniiroecology: sitting or standing close and alongside, with . or withoyt ,■ touching. This arrangement is symmetrical in physical character and social implication, no differentiation of role or rank being in itself conveyed: 124 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION V*12 ii The "arm lock" is the basic tie-sign in Western societies for marking that a woman is under the protective custody of the accompanying man. Although most commonly sustained between husband and wife, no sexual or legal link is necessarily advertised through it; father and grown daughter, man and best friend's wife may also employ it. The sign is asymmetric both in terms of its physical configuration and what it indicates. However nominally, the woman shows herself-to be receiving support, and both the man's hands are free for whatever, instrumental.tasks may arise: V-12 ii c (continued) V-12 iij The "shoulder hold" is an asymmetrical configuration' more or less requiring that the person,holding be taller tha^-'the-vpersoa^heldi^and^that-ifee^held person accept direction and constraint. Typically the arrangement seems to be dyadically irreversible. When employed by a cross-sexed adult pair, the sign seems to be takenvt6'')ridicatesexualiy-pbfential proprietary ship; ■BHHBSEBI V-12 iii d V-12iiio V-12HU t Was r f&fvVr* *t jr*V GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 12S V»12 iv Finally, hand-holding. When employed between adult male and female, hand-holding appears to be taken to indicate a sexually potential/exclusive relationship.17 A relatively symmetrical tie-sign presumably expressing relative, equality. Physical asymmetry Is to be detected in the tendency for the male to hold the female hand, this allowing the indication that he is presumably free to let go quickly should an emergency arise and free to guide and direct. The physical fact that the back of his hand is likely to be facing what is upcoming can faintly symbolize protectiveness: V-12 iv b The directing potential of hand-holding can be made apparent in ads: V-12 iv d particular are considered in Goffman (1971:188-237). V-12 ivf So also another theme, that of the male providing a safe tether: 126 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION VI LICENSED WITHDRAWAL Women, more than men, it seems, are pictured engaged in involvements which remove, them?1 psychologically from, the social situation at large, leaving them unortented in it and to it, and presumably, therefore, dependent on the protectiveness.-andv-goodwill of others who. are (or might come to be)..present.,. VM When emotional response causes an individual to lose control of his facial posture, that is, to "flood out," he can partly conceal the lapse by turning away from the others present or by covering his face/especially his mouth, with his hands. Realization of the kind associated with the young is involved, for the act cannot conceal that something is being concealed, and furthermore requires momentary blindness to everything around oneself—this being a particularly empty and maladaptive response when the withdrawal is itself a response to a real threat. VIA i Remorse: VH i * 1 VM i e VM i f VM ii Fear: vH ii a (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 127 VI-'. . 128 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION Vhi iv Laughter:... EÁDY! AIM! TEE HEE! VH iva VI-1 iv e Vl*2 just as covering the mouth with the hand can be an attenuation of covering the face; so a finger brought to the mouth" cSníbTran "'a'ttih'U'ation'-of "covering it with the hand. But here another ritual-ization seems more common: ■ the attenuation of sucking or biting the finger. The impression is given that somehow a stream of anxiety, rumination, or whatever, has been split off from the main course of attention and is being sustained in a dissociated, unthinking fashion. In any case, the face is partly covered as though one could see but not be seen and were therefore free to engage hand and face outside the stream of face-to-face address: Vl-2t (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 129 VI-2 f "3«. VI-2 / (continued) VI-2/I 130 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION V!-2o (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 131 I VI -5 o v>6 In real social situations and in pictured ones, the--'individuaf.:;ga|i^tb' draw his gaze "from: the scene at large (with the dependency and trust that this implies) and lock it in such a way as to give the impress!on of having only minor dissociated .= concern with what is thus seen, even as his mindhas wandered from ■■evfir^^ng^^^ituatiort^psyeh'olygi^I-Iy,:he is(^aw'ay.'(Doodling and middle distance loofe are examples, although it should be kept in mind that these two practices can also figure in another arrangement,, the one in which the individual aurally attends to what is being said by another while making it apparent that nothing he can see is competing for attention.) Vl-6 b ---- (continued) 134 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION An interesting object on which to lock an away look is the hands, for this focus not only can convey some sort of self-enclosure; but also can require a downward turning of the head, submissiveness being.. ■a*vposs"ibler,"-eo'nsequerit' interpretation: Vi-6g JH5BM lita™ Vl*7 In advertisements women are shown mentally drifting from the physicalscene around them (that is, going "away") while in close physical touch with a.male, as though his aliveness to the surround and his readiness to cope with anything.-vthat;.míght^presentBÍtself - were enough for both of them. (At the same time, the male may well wear a wary, monitoring look.) Thus, "anchored drifts." Various points of visual focus are found. Vl«7 i Middle distance: „^^^^ KS. Vl-7iŕ> i^filiiliSiPSiiSiillii »»■■^■■09 VI-7U VI-7 i rf VI-7 i ŕ Vl-7\f f. í' - • - f, í * i 1 * Vi-7 i ) from Love and Hate. 144 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION VM2iic (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 145 VM2iii Sitting: VM2iiirf ' VM2iii/r (continued) 146 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION VI-12 iii / V I -1 2 iv c (continued) ' GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 147 Vi-12 v c 148 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION vi-i: blue .shirt isiilt wh*jHHittei1 *obt\ VI -1 2 v / VI-13 Nuzzling—apparently an attenuated form of snuggling—involves employment of the face and especially the nose as a sort of surrogate or substitute for tucking in the. whole body. Nuzzling, then, would seem to constitute a form of partial withdrawal from full availability to the situation at large. What one finds, in pictures at least, is that women nuzzle children but men apparently do not. Indeed, women are sometimes pictured nuzzling objects.^. And, of course, women are pictured nuzzling men. 1 "S» ■■■■ 13 a VI-13 c 7 VI-13 d VI-13 ft I 111 11 »11 iy k* V ^**a^ v VI-13? (continued) GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 149 i ifie L-amnen look The lumber* Loakw-jnqM-^ThrV'.'inirl rfiOlfl VllftifMg lii'i ■>■■■: vi-i 3u VM4 The process whereby an individual snuggles into another seems anything but.impersonal, and yet is (I feel}'related ' to sbmething that has an impersonal cast, namely, the-ase-of ■another's'body as if it were something that could be used at will,- without'apparent -reference to its possessor, as an object to lean on or rest one's-limbs-*0nj^in--shorty as a physical resource, not a socially responsive one. In many cases, note/such leaning use of another seems to be an attenuated, very ritualized, form of snuggling. Note also that a non-sexual implication is present in the contact,1 and that, in advertisements at *least«'"Women (much as do children with respect to adults) apparently have Iicense"to use more of a -"man's.,,body in this u til itar4an»way^ than - the. reverse. The assumption seems to be that a woman is lessKlikelyKtosffihavefsexual in ten t ■ than > a mahj'-and that- her use of his body.-.is ther©fore4ess^uspect-than- h isuse of hers. (Of course, an added, factor is the understanding that he will be able to bear her weight much easier than she his.). Note, the configurations here considered involve individuals in a personal relationship;-typically a sexually potential/one. Among the less close, the license to touch follows a different pattern. Men can punctuate their, verbal, "interaction with, women by showing support, protective-ness, good will, and parent-like affection,-through the laying on of the hand, a license apparently less available to women (and other subordinates) in their dealings with men (see Henley 1973). VI-14 a ISO STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION VI-14 e (continued) Vl-H f V !-1 4 i v>15 A very standardized two-person asymmetrical configuration observable in real life and often in pictures is the "grief embrace." Ail combinations of sex are found in the two roles, except, apparently, that women are not pictured providing this sort of comfort to men.24 Whether in life or in pictures, one is provided here with a nice example of formalization-the reduction of multiple configurations to a rather set ritualistic maneuver: ML1 *e *SKS&>4 VI-15 a (continued) VI-15 A 24This distribution is not, I think, the basic one in our society. For there are many ritual practices of a supportive, bonding kind that women can extend to women or men, that men can extend to women, but that men can't extend to men. Kissing and terms of endearment such as "honey," "dear," "love," are examples, indeed, a wide range of supportive practices may have a common, natural social ihistory, beginning as something adults extend to children and then moving on through the following sequence of accretions; women-to-women, women-to-men, men-to-women, rnen-to-men. GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 151 Vf-T5 c VI-15 d VM6 The grief embrace appears to manifest itself in an attenuated, hyper-ritualized form, namely, arm support given as evidence of sonie soft' of commendation or' mora! approval. Again, in commercial pictures, women do not seem to be shown giving this support to men. mi /Arn. i&£38?'!fö "'1 fr « IBB M ft . »ES s s fa It ■ VI-T6 rf But you've told him plenty. Vl-16 e Fi- * T?^4sfe^ VI-16 f VI-16 5 VI-16 c (continued) 152 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION j ■1 i ,1 GENDER ADVERTISEMENTS 153 /\F Under display have been "natural" expressions of / V gender insofar as these can be represented in com-( mercial advertising .through, .visually....accessible...behavioral style. I believe that upon examination these expressions turn out to be illustrations of ntual-hke bits of behavior which portray an- ideali qonce-ptjori of the two sexes and their' structural relationship to each other, accomplishing this in' part by indicating, again ideally, the^aJigmrient of me-actor in' l\the soiiaUrujatipc, Commercial photographs, of course, involve carefully performed poses presented in .the style of being "only natural." But it is argued that actual gender expressions are artful poses, too. From the perspective of ritual, then, what is the difference between the scenes depicted in advertisements and scenes from actual life? One answer might be "hyper-ritual-PTzatiort." The standardization, exagge rati on, and si mplifica-tion that characterize:rituals; in^ne^^^ posings found to an extended degrce'xSflelT.l^i^edTas jiabyjshness, mockery, a*nd" other forms of unseriousness. Another answer is found in the process of editing. A commercial photograph is a ritualization of social ideals with all the occasions and senses in which the ideal is not exhibited having been cut away, edited out of what is made available. In ordinary life we conspire to provide the same kind of "natural" expressions, but-we can only do this by means of behavioral style or at particular junctures in our course of activity—moments of ceremony, occasions for giving sympathy, sudden access to friends, and similar junctures in the daily round, as determined by a schedule we know little about as yet. So both in advertisements and life we are interested in colorful poses, in externalization; but in life we are, in addition, stuck with a considerable amount of dull footage. Nonetheless, whether we pose for a picture or execute an actual ritual action, What we are- presenting is- a commercial,-an ideal repie^entatioh'imo^r the auspices of.its characterizing the~way things really are, When^Tlnarnrrreat life'"ttg}rte~a~CTgarette forlTwornan, the. presupposition is that females are worthy objects, physically limited in some way, and that they should be helped out in all their transitions. But this "natural" expression of the relation between the sexes, this little interpersonal ritual, may be no more an actual reflection of the relationship between the sexes than is the couple pictured in the cigarette ad a representative couple. Natural expressions are commercials perforjned to sell avejikyLflfthe world under conditions no less questionable and treacherous than the ones that advertisers' faceT ~ By and large, advertisers do not create the ritualized /'expressions they employ; they seem to draw upon'the same corpus of displays, the same ritual idiom, that is the resource of all of us who participate in social situations, and to the same end: the rendering of glimpsed: action readable. If anything, advertisers.conventionalize our conventions, stylize what is already- a stylization, make frivolous use of what is already something considerably cut off from contextual ..controls-.- Their hype is hyper-ritualization. REFERENCES CITED Bateson, Gregory, and Margaret Mead 1942 The Balinese Character. New York: New York Academy of Science. Chalfen, Richard 1975 Cinema Naivete: A Study of Home Moviemaking as Visual Communication. Studies in the Anthropology of Visual Communication 2:87jäi03. Chance, M. R. A. P< 1962 An Interpretation of Some Agonistic Postures: The Role of "Cut-Off" Acts and Postures. Symposium of the Zoological Society of London 8:71-89. Darwin, Charles 1872 On the Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Irenaus 1972 Love and Hate. Geoffrey Strachan, trans. New York: iHolt, Rinehart and Winston. Goffman, Erving 1971 Relations in Public. New York: Basic Books. 1974 Frame Analysis. New York: Harperand Row. ■ Henley, Nancy 1973 The Politics of Touch. In Radical Psychology. Phi! Brown, ed. Pp. 421-433. New York: Harper and Row. Komisar, Lucy 1972 The Image of Woman in Advertising. In Woman in Sexist Society. Vivian Gprnick and Barbara K. Moran, eds. New York: New American Library. Lesy, Michael 1973 Wisconsin Death Trip. New York: Pantheon. Robinson, Dwight E. 1976 Fashions in Shaving and Trimming of the Beard: The Men of the Illustrated London News, 1842-1972. American Journal of Sociology 81{5):113M141. Sudnow, David 1972 Temporal Parameters of Interpersonal Observation. In Studies in Social Interaction. David Sudnow, ed. Pp. 259-279. New York: The Free Press. Weisstein, Naomi 1973 Why We Aren't Laughing Any More, MS 2:49-90. Weitzman, Lenore j., Deborah Eifler, Elizabeth Hokada, and Catherine Ross 1972 Sex-Role Socialization in Picture Books for Preschool Children. American Journal of Sociology 77(6):1125-1150. 154 STUDIES IN THE ANTHROPOLOGY OF VISUAL COMMUNICATION