!3 Routledge j!^^ Taylor &. Francis Gro Group Political Communication ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: www.tandfonline.com/journals/upcp20 How Propaganda Techniques Leverage Their Advantages: A Cross-national Study of the Effects of Chinese International Propaganda on the U.S. and South Korean Audiences Bumgi Min & Luwei Rose Luqiu To cite this article: Bumgi Min & Luwei Rose Luqiu (2021) How Propaganda Techniques Leverage Their Advantages: A Cross-national Study of the Effects of Chinese International Propaganda on the U.S. and South Korean Audiences, Political Communication, 38:3, 305-325, DPI: 10.1080/10584609.2020.1763524 To link to this article: https://doi.Org/10.1080/10584609.2020.1763524 ^ Published online: 23 May 2020. Submit your article to this journal C? Article views: 2917 Pi View related articles G? O Cross Mark View Crossmark data .30 <.001 Model R2 .07 <.001 .09 <.001 POLITICAL COMMUNICATION @ 317 Table 5. Ordinary least squares regression model coefficient for models 2, 3, and 4. Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Outcome Persuasive effect Perceived credibility Persuasive effect Predictor Coeff. P Coeff. P Coeff. P Di -.15 .29 a1- -.61 <.001 .02 .87 D2 C2-* .13 .38 o2- -.73 <.001 4~* .34 <05 Nation .29 <.01 a3- -.09 .28 4~* .32 <001 D^Nation .83 <.001 a4- .91 <.001 4~* .57 <.001 D2xNation -.35 .11 a5- .54 <.01 -.50 <.05 Perceived credibility .28 <.001 Model R2 .11 <.001 .15 <.001 .17 <.001 Interaction AR2 .05 <.001 .08 <.001 .04 <.001 the old propaganda, and this perception was associated with a greater persuasive effect. These results support Hypothesis 1. Table 5 summarizes the coefficients of the ordinary least squares regression model (Models 2 to 4). With regard to Model 2, the data indicate that nationality (American and Korean) significantly moderated the relationship between the type of propaganda and its persuasive effect, R2 = .11, F(5, 335) = 7.92, p < .001. In addition to these main effects, there was a significant relationship between the interaction term (Di xnationality) and the persuasive effect (c4: B =.83, p < .001). That is, for the Korean participants, the persuasive effect of propaganda relative to nonpropaganda increased. These results support Hypothesis 2a. However, in terms of the moderating effect of nationality, there was no significant difference between new and old propaganda (c5: B =-.35, p = .11). These results do not support Hypothesis 2b. Hypothesis 3 (Model 3) predicted a similar effect of the type of propaganda on its perceived credibility, with nationality (American and Korean) serving as the moderator. The results demonstrated that the main effects are significant, R2 = .15, F(5, 335) = 11.60, p < .001. With respect to Hypothesis 3a, there was a significant relationship between the interaction term (Di xnationality) and the perceived credibility (a4: B =.91, p < .001), indicating that the perceived credibility of propaganda relative to nonpropaganda increased for those who are Koreans. These results support Hypothesis 3a. In terms of Hypothesis 3b, there was a significant relationship between the interaction term (D2 xnationality) and its perceived credibility (a5: B =.54, p < .01), indicating that the effect of new propaganda relative to old propaganda on the participants' perception of its credibility increased among the American audience. These results support Hypothesis 3b. Model 4 predicted that the effect of the type of propaganda on its persuasiveness would be mediated in part indirectly through its perceived credibility, with this process being likewise moderated by nationality. The prior analysis verifies that the path from the type of propaganda to its perceived credibility depends on nationality. If one of these paths is moderated, then so too is the indirect effect (Pollack et al, 2012). It is noteworthy that, holding the type of propaganda and nationality constant, participants reporting greater perceived credibility effects also reported greater persuasive effects (b: B =.28, p < .001). The model assumes multicategorical independent variables, which means that there were two relatively indirect effects, each of which was a function of the moderator W. Hayes (2018) argues that "as relative conditional indirect effects are products of regression 318 @ B. MIN AND R. LUQIU Table 6. The bootstrap confidence intervals of moderated mediation. Index SE BootLLCI BootULCI Di D2 .26 .15 .07 .07 .12 .03 .41 .30 coefficients, a bootstrap confidence interval for each relative conditional indirect effect is a sensible inferential strategy" (p. 493). Following this reasoning, we used a percentile bootstrap estimation approach as before with 5,000 samples to test each relative conditional indirect effect (Table 6). In terms of these effects for propaganda versus nonpropa-ganda, the bootstrap confidence interval was entirely above 0 (0.12-0.41). These results indicate a moderation of the indirect effect of propaganda relative to nonpropaganda on the persuasiveness of propaganda through perceived credibility that is further moderated by nationality. Based on this analysis, we conclude that the persuasiveness of the propaganda, when compared with the nonpropaganda, was greater for the research participants because of the perception that it was of high credibility and that this result was particularly strong for the Korean participants. These results support Hypothesis 4a. In terms of the relative conditional indirect effects of new compared with old propaganda, the bootstrap confidence intervals were entirely above 0 (0.03-0.30). These results indicated the moderation of the indirect effect of new relative to old propaganda on the persuasive effect by perceived credibility and nationality. Based on this analysis, we conclude that new propaganda seems to have enhanced the persuasive effect owing to the perception that it was of high credibility and that this enhancement was particularly pronounced for the U.S. participants. These results support Hypothesis 4b. Discussion With the advent of the Internet, almost anyone can distribute literature, images, videos, and software to an international audience easily and inexpensively. The messages contained therein, political and otherwise, can be manipulated by the distributors and can bypass national laws (Aly et al, 2017; Crilley, 2001). In China, the CCP is accordingly concentrating its international propaganda efforts online. While the party's domestic propaganda products and their effects have been well documented (e.g., Wasserman, 2016; Wu, 2016; Zhang & Mwangi, 2016), its international propaganda efforts, especially those delivered anonymously over the Internet, have not been extensively studied. To help fill this gap, the present study compared cross-national persuasive effects by analyzing the effects of different types of propaganda created by the CCP on audiences in the U.S. and Korea. Our research yielded three significant findings. First, the persuasive effect of the CCP's new propaganda was mediated by its perceived credibility. Second, the nationality of the audience (i.e., American or Korean) moderated both the perceived credibility of Chinese international propaganda and its persuasive effect. Third, the indirect effect of various propaganda products (operationalized as new propaganda, old propaganda, and nonpropaganda) on their persuasiveness as mediated through their perceived credibility was contingent on the nationality of the audience (again, American or Korean). POLITICAL COMMUNICATION @ 319 With respect to perceived credibility as a mediator, our results demonstrated that perceived credibility is a significant mediator between the CCP's new propaganda and its persuasive effect. In particular, the participants perceived new propaganda to be of higher credibility than old propaganda and therefore more persuasive. Prior studies have concentrated on perceived credibility as a key factor in the persuasiveness of messages (Jones et al, 2003; Mun et al, 2013; Pornpitakpan, 2004), and the significant role of perceived credibility on its persuasive effect was demonstrated even in the online space (Hsieh et al, 2012). Interestingly, a recent study has demonstrated that when an online video is credited to a non-CCP brand, its perceived credibility among U.S. audiences is significantly higher than that of the same video branded as a CCP product (Fang & Mutz, 2016). In addition to existing studies, our finding provides considerable insight into how the CCP's new propaganda increases its persuasive effect through its perceived credibility. According to Wong (2016), the CCP has used private entities to create and disseminate propaganda to disguise its source and increase its credibility. By doing so, the CCP may increase the perceived credibility of the material in the eyes of international audiences, removing their hostility toward its propaganda. This was the case with the 2013 video titled "How Leaders Were Tempered" that compared Chinese and Western leadership produced by a supposedly independent Chinese production company called Studio on Fuxin Road, which was considered the new propaganda stimulus in our study. This video was posted on YouTube and then promoted on state media Twitter accounts, and it received coverage by many foreign media outlets, which gave it a mixed reception. The Chinese government and state media, however, labeled it a success. Wong (2016) indicates that the studio was in fact established by the CCP's International Department, while a British public relations consulting company and several other outside contractors helped produce the video. Fuxin Road has also created another similar online propaganda, such as the 2015 animated video titled "13 What" explaining China's thirteenth five-year economic plan. Geographical and cultural differences play important roles in the persuasive effect of messages (Aladwani, 2013; Laroche et al, 2001; Yu & Shen, 2013). Thus, nationality has been found to moderate the persuasive effect of fear appeal advertisement (Laroche et al, 2001) and the message framing effect (Yu & Shen, 2013). In our study, nationality (American and Korean) moderated the persuasive effect of propaganda relative to non-propaganda and that of new relative to old propaganda with regard to the participants' perception of its credibility. Interestingly, we were able to demonstrate moderation of the indirect effect of propaganda relative to nonpropaganda and of new propaganda relative to old propaganda on persuasiveness through perceived credibility and nationality (American versus Korean). Our study demonstrates that the persuasiveness of the propaganda, when compared with the nonpropaganda, was greater for the research participants because of the perception that it was of high credibility and that this result was particularly strong for the Korean participants. The cultural proximity thesis gives a plausible explanation for why the audience in Korea is more persuaded by propaganda, including both old and new forms, based on its perceived credibility. As Straubhaar (1991) argued, the audience tends to consume regionally produced media due to cultural relevance or proximity. A recent study found that cultural proximity is significantly related to a positive attitude toward regional media products (Yoo et al, 2014). Because cultural proximity is a constructed process related to the interpretation of content 320 @ B. MIN AND R. LUQIU (Chen, 2017), culturally proximate content is better comprehended by the audience. In the same context, Koreans could have a better understanding of the CCP's propaganda compared with nonpropaganda because of close cultural and historical relations with China. Better comprehension is significantly related to higher perceived degree of credibility (Sparks & Rapp, 2011), and this could increase the persuasive effect by increasing the positive image of China among the Koreans. These causal relationships among cultural proximity, perceived credibility, and the persuasive effect should be analyzed in future studies to provide convincing explanations of this phenomenon. By narrowing the persuasive effect to a comparison between new and old propaganda, the present study showed that American respondents seem to find the new propaganda more persuasive than the old propaganda when they perceived its credibility to be high. To analyze the effects of the new propaganda product, it is necessary to identify its target audience, media utilization techniques, and any special methods designed to enhance its power. First, we should pay attention to the metaphorical nature of the messages conveyed by the new propaganda, the processing of which can facilitate persuasion by disrupting counter argumentation (Guthrie, 1972; Sopory & Dillard, 2002). A critique of the efficiency of Western elections may resonate with the prevailing perceptions of certain audiences regarding the political systems in the United States or United Kingdom. Thus, earlier research has shown that, although the core ideology of the CCP's propaganda is Marxist-Leninist, the organization has been able to function in the West through the coordinated use of sophisticated argumentation and disinformation (Kampf, 1987). When discussing the truth and the actual course of events can serve its aims, states have no reason to turn to disinformation (Martin, 1982), and the CCP's narrative propaganda strategy emphasizes indisputable weaknesses in Western political systems. This rhetoric strategy may increase perceived credibility among Americans, and as a result, it has highly persuasive effects. Second, the CCP's latest propaganda uses sophisticated techniques to communicate with its intended audience in a targeted way. Thus, as mentioned, "How Leaders Were Tempered" incorporates animation, music, and informal speech in an obvious effort to appeal to Western and young audiences accustomed to Internet videos. This approach seems more effective in a low-context society (the U.S. in our study) than in a high-context society (Korea in our study) because the CCP's new propaganda provides more verbal cues and less social pressure compared with the old propaganda. Moreover, the representation of value in animation in this manner can be an effective form of propaganda (Merkt & Sochatzy, 2015), and music can be likewise frequently used to stir emotion, while lyrics reinforce the underlying ideology (Jowett & O'Donnell, 2014). The symbolic use of language can also create a sense of power and contribute to opinion formation (Edelman, 1988; Gamson & Modigliani, 1987), and content and format framing influence an individual's political perception (Terkildsen et al., 1998). To develop a better understanding of the effect of new propaganda, future studies are needed to explain which factors increase the CCP's new propaganda source credibility, thereby increasing its persuasive effects. Conclusion The findings presented here, then, offer insights into how a fairly new form of propaganda differentially affects cross-national audiences in the U.S. and Korea. There are, however, POLITICAL COMMUNICATION @ 321 certain limitations to this study. First, the effect of propaganda on viewers was measured in the short term, with the respondents answering the survey questions immediately after watching the stimulus videos. The long-term or cultivation effects of propaganda disseminated via the Internet are thus an obvious subject for future research. Second, our study tested one example of each type of propaganda. However, alternating additional versions of each type of video under each treatment condition would demonstrate more deep and comparable propaganda effects. Therefore, future studies should be performed in this line of research. Last, although MTurk has the advantage of reaching diverse participants at relatively low cost, this online survey system also has potential limitations in that previous studies have demonstrated the tendency of MTurk samples to skew toward a younger, more female (Berinsky et al, 2012), and more educated (Paolacci et al, 2010) population than the general U.S. population. In this case, the demographic composition of the MTurk samples, like that of most samples, likely does not mirror the larger population exactly. Despite these limitations, however, this study has demonstrated both the power of new propaganda and its distinct effects on audiences in the U. S. and Korea by adopting moderated mediation models. It is intended as a contribution to the growing body of literature on cross-national and international propaganda, media effects, perceived credibility, and persuasive effects. Our work here is particularly timely because governments today are investing ever more resources in diverse media in efforts to influence foreign and domestic audiences, to export their ideologies and to improve their public images. Disclosure Statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. Notes on contributors Bumgi Min is a Ph.D. candidate in Donald P. Bellisario College of Communications at Pennsylvania State University. His research interests include intercultural communication, political communication, digital media, and media effect. Luwei Rose Luqiu is an assistant professor at the School of Communication at Hong Kong Baptist University. She researches censorship, propaganda and social movements in authoritarian regimes. She has been a journalist for 20 years and was a 2007 Nieman Fellow at Harvard University. ORCID Bumgi Min © http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2972-8973 Luwei Rose Luqiu © http://orcid.org/0000-0003-3772-9148 References Aaker, J. L., & Maheswaran, D. (1997). The effect of cultural orientation on persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(3), 315-328. https://doi.org/10.1086/209513 Aladwani, A. M. (2013). A cross-cultural comparison of Kuwaiti and British citizens' views of e-government interface quality. Government Information Quarterly, 30(1), 74-86. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.giq.2012.08.003 322 @ B. MIN AND R. LUQIU Aly, A., Macdonald, S., Jarvis, L., & Chen, T. M. (2017). Introduction to the special issue: Terrorist online propaganda and radicalization. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 40(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/1057610X.2016.1157402 Bailard, C. S. (2016). China in africa: An analysis of the effect of Chinese media expansion on african public opinion. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 21(4), 446-471. https://doi.org/10. 1177/1940161216646733 Bandurski, D. (2016, September 2). Innovation, so the party can shine. China Media Project, http:// chinamediaproject.org/2016/09/02/innovation-so-the-party-can-shine/ Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351-368. https://doi.org/10. 1093/pan/mpr057 Brady, A. M. (2006). Guiding hand: The role of the ccp central propaganda department in the current era. Westminster Papers in Communication and Culture, 3(1), 58-77. https://doi.org/10. 16997/wpcc.l5 Brady, A. M. (2015, October 26). China's foreign propaganda machine. Kissinger Institute on China and the United States, https://www.wilsoncenter.org/article/chinas-foreign-propaganda-machine Bullock, J., & Ha, S. (2011). Mediation analysis is harder than it looks. In J. Druckman, D. Green, J. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science (pp. 508-522). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511921452.035 Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Quintanar, L. R. (1982). Individual differences in relative hemispheric alpha abundance and cognitive responses to persuasive communications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(3), 623-636. https://doi.Org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.3.623 Carpenter, C. J. (2015). A meta-analysis of the ELM's argument qualityx processing type predictions. Human Communication Research, 41(4), 501-534. https://doi.org/10.llll/hcre.12054 Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(3), 460-473. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.663.460 Chang, T. K., & Lin, F. (2014). From propaganda to public diplomacy: Assessing China's international practice and its image, 1950-2009. Public Relations Review, 40(3), 450-458. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.pubrev.2014.04.008 Chen, L. (2017). The emergence of the anti-hallyu movement in china. Media, Culture & Society, 39 (3) , 374-390. https://doi.org/10.1177/0163443716646176 Creemers, R. (2015). Never the twain shall meet? Rethinking China's public diplomacy policy. Chinese Journal of Communication, 8(3), 306-322. https://doi.org/10.1080/17544750.2015. 1046187 Creemers, R. (2017). Cyber china: Upgrading propaganda, public opinion work and social management for the twenty-first century. Journal of Contemporary China, 26(103), 85-100. https://doi. org/10.1080/10670564.2016.1206281 Crilley, K. (2001). Information warfare: New battle fields Terrorists, propaganda and the Internet. Aslib Proceedings, 53(7), 250-264. https://doi.org/10.1108/EUM0000000007059 Cull, N. (2008). The cold war and the United States information agency: American propaganda and public diplomacy, 1945-1989. Cambridge University Press, https://doi.org/10.1017/ CBO9780511817151 Cull, N. J. (2013). Roof for a house divided: How U.S. propaganda evolved into public diplomacy. In J. Auerbach & R. Castronovo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of propaganda studies (pp. 131-146). Oxford University Press. D'Hooghe, I. (2005). Public diplomacy in the People's Republic of China. In J. Melissen (Ed.), The new public diplomacy (pp. 88-105). Palgrave Macmillan. DeBono, K. G., & Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source expertise, source attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A functional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55 (4) , 541-546. https://doi.Org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.541 POLITICAL COMMUNICATION @ 323 Dou, X., Walden, J. A., Lee, S., & Lee, J. Y. (2012). Does source matter? Examining source effects in online product reviews. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5), 1555-1563. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.chb.2012.03.015 Edelman, M. (1988). Constructing the political spectacle. University of Chicago Press. Edney, K. (2012). Soft power and the Chinese propaganda system. Journal of Contemporary China, 21(78), 899-914. https://doi.org/10.1080/10670564.2012.701031 Edney, K. (2014). The globalization of Chinese propaganda: International power and domestic political cohesion. Palgrave Macmillan. Fang, K., & Mutz, D. C. (2016, September). Communist party's soft power in cross-national persuasion videos: Shaping China's image among overseas audiences [Paper Presentation]. APSA Preconference, Philadelphia, PA. Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1987). The changing culture of affirmative action. In R. G. Braungart & M. M. Braungart (Eds.), Research in Political Sociology (Vol. 3, pp. 137-177). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Giebels, E., & Taylor, P. J. (2009). Interaction patterns in crisis negotiations: Persuasive arguments and cultural differences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(1), 5. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012953 Gilboa, E. (2008). Searching for a theory of public diplomacy. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 616(1), 55-77. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716207312142 Greenwood, D. N. (2013). Fame, Facebook, and Twitter. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2(4), 222-236. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000013 Guthrie, M. (1972). Effects of credibility, metaphor, and intensity on comprehension, credibility, and attitude change [Unpublished master's thesis], Illinois State University Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press. Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press. Hong, J. (2011). From the world's largest propaganda machine to a multipurposed global news agency: Factors in and implications of Xinhua's transformation since 1978. Political Communication, 28(3), 377-393. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2011.572487 Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), 635-650. https://doi.org/10.1086/266350 Hsieh, J. K., Hsieh, Y. C, & Tang, Y. C. (2012). Exploring the disseminating behaviors of eWOM marketing: Persuasion in online video. Electronic Commerce Research, 12(2), 201-224. https://doi. org/10.1007/sl0660-012-9091-y Jones, L. W., Sinclair, R. C, & Courneya, K. S. (2003). The effects of source credibility and message framing on exercise intentions, behaviors, and attitudes: An integration of the elaboration likelihood model and prospect theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(1), 179-196. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.l559-1816.2003.tb02078.x Jowett, G. S., & O'Donnell, V. (2014). Propaganda and persuasion. Sage Publications. Kampf, H. A. (1987). The challenge of Marxist-Leninist propaganda. Political Communication, 4(2), 103-122. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.1987.9962813 Kecskemeti, P. (1956). The Soviet approach to international political communication. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 20(1), 299-308. https://doi.org/10.1086/266618 Kenton, S. B. (1989). Speaker credibility in persuasive business communication: A model which explains gender differences 1. The Journal of Business Communication (1973), 26(2), 143-157. https://doi.org/10.1177/002194368902600204 LaMarre, H. (2013). When parody and reality collide: Examining the effects of Colbert's Super PAC Satire on issue knowledge and policy engagement across media formats. International Journal of Communication, 7(1), 394-413. https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/1939 Laroche, M., Toffoli, R., Zhang, Q., & Pons, F. (2001). A cross-cultural study of the persuasive effect of fear appeal messages in cigarette advertising: China and Canada. International Journal of Advertising, 20(3), 297-317. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2001.11104895 Li, M. J. (2008). China debates soft power. The Chinese Journal of International Politics, 2(2), 287-308. https://doi.org/10.1093/cjip/pon011 324 @ B. MIN AND R. LUQIU Lu, X., & Simons, H. W. (2006). Transitional rhetoric of Chinese communist party leaders in the post-Mao reform period: Dilemmas and strategies. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 92(3), 262-286. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335630600938674 Martin, R. C. (1982). The pseudohomophone effect: The role of visual similarity in non-word decisions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34(3), 395-409. https://doi.org/10. 1080/14640748208400851 Melissen, J. (2005). The new public diplomacy: Between theory and practice. In J. Melissen (Ed.), The new public diplomacy (pp. 3-27). Palgrave Macmillan. Merkt, M., & Sochatzy, F. (2015). Becoming aware of cinematic techniques in propaganda: Instructional support by cueing and training. Learning and Instruction, 39, 55-71. https://doi. org/10.1016/j .learninstruc.2015.05.006 Meyer, P. (1988). Defining and measuring credibility of newspapers: Developing an index. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 65(3), 567-574. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 107769908806500301 Mun, Y. Y., Yoon, J. J., Davis, J. M., & Lee, T. (2013). Untangling the antecedents of initial trust in Web-based health information: The roles of argument quality, source expertise, and user perceptions of information quality and risk. Decision Support Systems, 55(1), 284-295. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.dss.2013.01.029 Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population-based survey experiments. Princeton University Press. Nelson, T. E., & Garst, J. (2005). Values-based political messages and persuasion: Relationships among speaker, recipient, and evoked values. Political Psychology, 26(4), 489-516. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/j. 1467-9221.2005.00428.x Nye, J. S. (2003, fanuary 10). Propaganda isn't the way: Soft power. The International Herald Tribune, https://www.belfercenter.org/publication/propaganda-isnt-way-soft-power Nye, J. S. (2004). Soft power: The means to success in world politics. Public Affairs. Nye, J. S. (2008). Public diplomacy and soft power. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 616(1), 94-109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716207311699 Nye, J. S. (2011, April 12). The war on soft power. Foreign Policy, https://foreignpolicy.com/2011/04/ 12/the-war-on-soft-power/ Okabe, R. (1983). Cultural assumptions of east and west: fapan and the United States. In W. Gudykunst (Ed.), Intercultural communication theory: Current perspectives (pp. 21-44). Sage. Orth, U. R., Koenig, H. F., & Firbasova, Z. (2007). Cross-national differences in consumer response to the framing of advertising messages: An exploratory comparison from Central Europe. European Journal of Marketing, 41(3/4), 327-348. https://doi.org/10.1108/03090560710728363 Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments on Amazon mechanical turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(5), 411-419. http://www.sjdm.org/journal/10/10630a/ jdml0630a.pdf Park, H. S., Levine, T. R., Kingsley Westerman, C. Y., Orfgen, T., & Foregger, S. (2007). The effects of argument quality and involvement type on attitude formation and attitude change: A test of dual-process and social judgment predictions. Human Communication Research, 33(1), 81-102. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1468-2958.2007.00290.X Park, S. J., & Lim, Y. S. (2014). Information networks and social media use in public diplomacy: A comparative analysis of South Korea and fapan. Asian Journal of Communication, 24(1), 79-98. https://doi.org/10.1080/01292986.2013.851724 Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Message elaboration versus peripheral cues. In Communication and persuasion (pp. 141-172). New York, NY: Springer. Pollack, J. M., Vanepps, E. M., & Hayes, A. F. (2012). The moderating role of social ties on entrepreneurs' depressed affect and withdrawal intentions in response to economic stress. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33(6), 789-810. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1794 Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades' evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 243-281. https://doi.0rg/lO.llll/j.1559- 1816.2004.tb02547.x POLITICAL COMMUNICATION @ 325 Repnikova, M., & Fang, K. (2019). Digital media experiments in China: "Revolutionizing" persuasion under Xi Jinping. The China Quarterly, 239, 679-701. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0305741019000316 Shen, F., & Guo, Z. S. (2013). The last refuge of media persuasion: News use, national pride and political trust in China. Asian Journal of Communication, 23(2), 135-151. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 01292986.2012.725173 Shi, T., Lu, J., & Aldrich, J. (2011). Bifurcated images of the US in urban China and the impact of media environment. Political Communication, 28(3), 357-376. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609. 2011.572479 Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. (1996). How message evaluation and source attributes may influence credibility assessment and belief change. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 73(4), 974-991. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769909607300415 Sopory, P., & Dillard, J. P. (2002). The persuasive effects of metaphor: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research, 28(3), 382-419. https://doi.Org/10.llll/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00813.x Sparks, J. R., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Readers' reliance on source credibility in the service of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 230-247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021331 Straubhaar, J. D. (1991). Beyond media imperialism: Asymmetrical interdependence and cultural proximity. Critical Studies in Media Communication, 8(1), 39-59. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 15295039109366779 Taylor, L. D. (2015). Investigating fans of fictional texts: Fan identity salience, empathy, and transportation. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 172-187. https://doi.org/10.1037/ ppm0000028 Terkildsen, N., Schnell, F. I., & Ling, C. (1998). Interest groups, the media, and policy debate formation: An analysis of message structure, rhetoric, and source cues. Political Communication, 15(1), 45-61. https://doi.org/10.1080/105846098199127 Turner, I. (2007). The messenger overwhelming the message: Ideological cues and perceptions of bias in television news. Political Behavior, 29(4), 441-464. https://doi.org/10.1007/slll09-007- 9031-z Wasserman, H. (2016). China's "soft power" and its influence on editorial agendas in South Africa. Chinese Journal of Communication, 9(1), 8-20. https://doi.org/10.1080/17544750.2015.1049953 Wasserman, H., & Madrid-Morales, D. (2018). How influential are Chinese media in Africa? An audience analysis in Kenya and south Africa. International Journal of Communication, 12, 2212- 2231. https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/7809 Wong, C. H. (2016, May 12). Chinese president Xi Tinping's extreme makeover. The Wall Street Journal, https://www.wsj.com/articles/xi-jinpings-extreme-makeover-1463069291 Wu, Y. S. (2016). China's media and public diplomacy approach in Africa: Illustrations from South Africa. Chinese Journal of Communication, 9(1), 81-97. https://doi.org/10.1080/17544750.2016. 1139606 Yoo, I., Io, S., & lung, I. (2014). The effects of television viewing, cultural proximity, and ethno- centrism on country image. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 42(1), 89-96. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.L89 Yu, N., & Shen, F. (2013). Benefits for me or risks for others: A cross-culture investigation of the effects of message frames and cultural appeals. Health Communication, 28(2), 133-145. https:// doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2012.662147 Zhang, L, & Han, Y. (2013). Testing the rhetoric of China's soft power campaign: A case analysis of its strategic ambiguity in the Six Party Talks over North Korea's nuclear program. Asian Journal of Communication, 23(2), 191-208. https://doi.org/10.1080/01292986.2012.725180 Zhang, Y., & Mwangi, J. M. (2016). A perception study on China's media engagement in Kenya: From media presence to power influence? Chinese Journal of Communication, 9(1), 71-80. https://doi.org/10.1080/17544750.2015.1111246 Zhao, Y. (2016, February 22). Enhance the four dimension of socialism ideology discourse. People's Daily, http://opinion.people.com.cn/nl/2016/0222/cl003-28137730.html