Review Psychological benefits of believing conspiracy theories Jan-Willem van Prooijen1,2,3 Abstract Many people believe conspiracy theories, even though such beliefs are harmful to themselves and their social environment. What is the appeal of conspiracy theories? In this contribution, I propose that conspiracy theories have psychological benefits by imbuing perceiver’s worldview with meaning and purpose in a rewarding manner. Conspiracy theories enable an alternative reality in which perceivers (a) can defend a fragile ego by perceiving themselves and their groups as important, (b) can rationalize any of their beliefs and actions as legitimate, and (c) are entertained through the opportunity to uncover a mystery in an exciting tale. These are short-term benefits, however, suggesting that conspiracy theories provide people with a form of instant gratification. Addresses 1 Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Netherlands 2 The Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement (NSCR), Netherlands 3 Maastricht University, Netherlands Corresponding author: Prooijen, Jan-Willem van (j.w.van.prooijen@ vu.nl) Current Opinion in Psychology 2022, 47:101352 This review comes from a themed issue on Conspiracy Theories (2023) Edited by Jan-Willem van Prooijen and Roland Imhoff For complete overview about the section, refer Conspiracy Theories (2023) Available online 5 May 2022 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101352 2352-250X/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons. org/licenses/by/4.0/). Keywords Conspiracy theories, Psychological benefits, Alternative reality, Instant gratification. Although conspiracy theories are omnipresent, empirical research has mainly emphasized their negative effects. Believing conspiracy theories is associated with poor health choices [1e5], decreased well-being [6,7], and deteriorated social relationships [8]. Conspiracy beliefs also harm society by decreasing public support for policy aimed at addressing genuine problems such as climate change [9; but see 10] and the Covid-19 pandemic [11,12]. Moreover, they are associated with support for populist and politically extreme movements [13e15], and violent activism [16e19]. These findings suggest a paradox: If conspiracy theories are largely harmful for perceivers and their environment, then why do so many people endorse them? The present contribution will examine how perceivers benefit psychologically from conspiracy beliefs. While current theoretical models may imply that conspiracy theories satisfy basic psychological needs or help alleviate threats, empirical evidence does not support that people actually benefit from conspiracy beliefs in this manner. For instance, an evolutionary perspective suggests that susceptibility to conspiracy theories has been adaptive to ancient hunteregatherers, to cope with the realistic threat of hostile coalitions. This reasoning, however, only emphasizes how conspiracy theories helped ancestors survive in a Pleistocene environment, and does not hold implications for possible psychological benefits in present-day society [20]. If anything, the evolutionary perspective implies macro-level societal benefits, by explaining why people possess mental systems that make them sensitive to signals suggesting possible collusion. Consistent with this perspective, citizens display stronger conspiracy beliefs in highcorruption than low-corruption countries [21,22]. Motivational perspectives also fall short of clarifying the psychological benefits of conspiracy beliefs. Extensive research has revealed that conspiracy beliefs increase following deprivation of existential, epistemic, and social needs [23]. These findings do not show, however, that conspiracy beliefs subsequently are successful in satisfying these needs. While conspiracy theories may help people understand their social environment when faced with existential threats, the epistemic assumption that hostile conspiracies operate in the shadows is unlikely to make them feel safe or certain. Conspiracy beliefs indeed do not reduce, and sometimes even increase, anxiety, uncertainty aversion, and existential threat [24]. Furthermore, although experiences of social exclusion increase conspiracy beliefs [25], believing conspiracy theories does not restore belongingness needs. Instead, conspiracy beliefs make people vulnerable to stigmatization [26], and predict job loss and social rejection [7]. One can even question how Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2022, 47:101352 comforting “denialist” conspiracy theories are: These theories may reduce feelings of existential threat in one domain (e.g., by denying the reality of climate change) yet reinforce other existential threats (e.g., the belief that authorities are deceptive). These issues raise the question what the psychological payoff of conspiracy theories is for believers. The core of my argument is that although conspiracy beliefs may not reduce anxiety or help them maintain social relationships, they do stimulate a sense of meaning and purpose in a specific way that is psychologically rewarding. Through conspiracy theories perceivers can construct an alternative reality in which they are important and legitimate actors, participating in a spectacular narrative. In what follows I more specifically propose that conspiracy theories (1) have ego-defensive benefits by making people feel important, (2) help people rationalize their behavior and therefore make them feel legitimate, and (3) have entertainment value by stimulating feelings of excitement. Conspiracy theories are ego-defensive Many conspiracy theories provide alternative explanations for impactful crisis events in the world, such as pandemics, wars, or natural disasters [27]. Believing conspiracy theories therefore can install a sense of meaning and purpose by raising the impression that one is discovering something truly important. This reasoning is compatible with theoretical insights into closely related phenomena: For instance, radicalization theories stress that extreme ideological beliefs satisfy a need for significance, by making people feel important and special. People therefore are more likely to radicalize following experiences of significance loss (e.g., injustice; humiliation [28]). Likewise, conspiracy beliefs are associated with feeling unique and special, and can hence reflect positively on people’s self-perception [29,30]. It should be specified, however, that these self-related benefits of conspiracy theories do not necessarily pertain to maintenance of a positive self-concept; instead, conspiracy theories help people defend relatively fragile forms of self-perception [31]. For example, conspiracy beliefs predict instability of self-esteem (i.e., the extent to which self-esteem fluctuates over time) more strongly than level of self-esteem [32]. Moreover, conspiracy beliefs are positively associated only with an inflated sense of self, as conspiracy beliefs predict lower self-esteem but also higher narcissism [6]. A similar process can be observed in the way people derive self-worth from their group memberships, as conspiracy beliefs predict collective narcissism (i.e., an inflated perception of one’s ingroup [33,34]). Through conspiracy theories people can establish the superiority of their ingroup as compared with competing outgroups. In sum, conspiracy beliefs can make people feel important and superior, and thus are beneficial by helping them defend a fragile sense of self-worth. Conspiracy theories as rationalization tool An additional way in which conspiracy theories contribute to meaning and purpose is by legitimizing people’s role in the world. Recall that conspiracy beliefs are associated with counter-normative behaviors including rejection of vaccines, reduced containmentrelated behavior during the Covid-19 pandemic, and antisocial behavior [3,4,18]. It is important to keep in mind that much of the evidence supporting this link is correlational. Even research showing causal effects of conspiracy theories on counter-normative behavior [11,35] does not exclude the possibility of bidirectionality: Counter-normative behavior may causally increase conspiracy beliefs. Here, I propose that the link between counter-normative behavior and conspiracy beliefs partially reflects a psychological benefit for perceivers: Conspiracy theories may be used as rationalization tool because they are flexible narratives that people can endorse, sometimes even with limited or no evidence [36]. Conspiracy theories therefore facilitate a motivated reasoning process that helps people justify (to themselves and others) their beliefs and behaviors [37]. The notion that people seek to rationalize their behavior, and therefore easily embrace conspiracy beliefs, is consistent with classic theories of cognitive dissonance [38]. The flexible nature of conspiracy theories implies that people can conveniently form them about any societal institution or group, and as such, conspiracy theories help perceivers mentally reconstrue unhealthy behaviors as healthy (e.g., justifying vaccine refusal by believing that pharmaceutical companies are dishonest), and anti-government violence as legitimate (e.g., justifying violent protests as legitimate resistance against oppressors). While these actions themselves are not beneficialdto neither believers nor societydconspiracy beliefs can imbue people’s worldview, and their actions associated with it, with a sense of legitimacy. Various findings support the notion that people use conspiracy theories as a rationalization tool. During the Covid-19 pandemic, conspiracy beliefs not only predict decreased Covid-19 preventive behavior over time, but also, decreased preventive behavior predicts increased conspiracy belief over time [39]. Also, at the start of the pandemic the link between conspiracy beliefs and detrimental health behaviors was mediated by a belief to have experienced a Covid-19 infection, without evidence of a positive medical test. Apparently, conspiracy beliefs predict a motivated tendency to interpret one’s own physical discomfort as evidence of a Covid-19 2 Conspiracy Theories (2023) Current Opinion in Psychology 2022, 47:101352 www.sciencedirect.com infection, presumably to rationalize a worldview that downplays the dangers of the coronavirus [12]. Conspiracy beliefs also help to rationalize people’s perceptions of society. In politics, people endorse conspiracy theories as a form of motivated reasoning to support their ideological beliefs and degrade competing ideologies [40,41]. Moreover, conspiracy theories help people justify the societal system that they live indthus raising their satisfaction with itdby blaming its problems on outgroup conspiracies instead of inherent flaws in the system [42,43]. Relatedly, outgroup conspiracy theories are associated with collectivist cultural values [44e46]. In sum, conspiracy theories make perceivers feel like legitimate actors by helping them rationalize their beliefs and behaviors. Conspiracy theories as entertainment A third way in which conspiracy theories contributes to meaning and purpose is by creating an alternative reality that is exciting, attention-grabbing, and spectacular. Conspiracy theories typically portray an archetypical struggle between good and evil, and introduce mystery about the potentially dubious role of powerful and important societal actors (e.g., politicians; celebrities). It is therefore not surprising that the plotlines of many works of fictiondincluding novels, theater plays, and moviesdcenter around conspiracies [47]. Believing conspiracy theories turns perceivers into active players in such spectacular narratives, and gives them the opportunitydmuch like lay detectivesdto uncover a mystery. Believing conspiracy theories hence offers people entertainment. At first blush, this psychological benefit might seem discrepant with the notion that conspiracy theories can increase negative emotions such as anxiety [24]. Note, however, that many popular sources of entertainment are likely to increase anxiety (e.g., scary movies; detective novels; gambling; bungee jumping). People often do not avoid such negative emotions; instead, people are drawn to events that provide intense emotional experiences, which may include emotions that are negative, positive, or both [48]. Such intense emotional experiences are exciting, and make people feel alive. Research supports the notion that people experience conspiracy beliefs as entertaining. Conspiracy beliefs are associated with dispositional aversion to boredom [49], and with the more general trait sensation-seeking, reflecting people’s desire for intense sensations and experiences [50]. Sensation-seeking also predicts a range of phenomena closely related with conspiracy beliefs, including radicalization and participation in violent extremist groups [51,52], and supernatural beliefs [53,54]. Moreover, conspiracy beliefs are associated with not only negative but also positive emotions [55]. Experimental studies underscore the entertainment value of conspiracy theories. Participants rated a conspiratorial text (about the Notre Dame fire or the death of Jeffrey Epstein) as more entertaining than a text describing the official account of these events. These entertainment appraisals subsequently predicted increased belief in these conspiracy theories. An additional study showed that people more strongly believed that an election event was rigged if it was described in an entertaining rather than a boring manner [50]. These findings illuminate that people find conspiracy theories entertaining, which motivates increased belief in them. Conclusions Why are so many people drawn to conspiracy theories? The present contribution has highlighted three complementary ways in which believing conspiracy theories may bring psychological benefits. Conspiracy theories help people defend a fragile ego by exaggerating the importance of themselves and their groups; conspiracy theories make people feel like legitimate actors by rationalizing their beliefs and behaviors; and, believing conspiracy theories entertains people by making them active participants of an exciting tale. The general process underlying these psychological benefits is the potential of conspiracy theories to imbue perceivers’ worldview with meaning and purpose in a rewarding manner. Two qualifications of these propositions are in order. First, the described processes are likely to be psychologically beneficial only in the short run. While constructing an alternative reality may be appealing as described here, a risk is that reality catches up with perceivers eventually. Many of the harmful effects of conspiracy theories emerge by promoting choices that may have negative real-life consequences for themselves and others [3,7,11]. Second, the current propositions only focus on the question how perceivers themselves benefit from conspiracy theories. How perceivers’ social environment, or society at large, might benefit from conspiracy theories is a different issue. Although conspiracy theories also may have certain benefits for societydin that they can increase transparency among decision-makers, and can keep the public attentive to the possibility of actual corruption [56]dmost social consequences are likely to be negative [1e5,7e12,16e20]. These considerations suggest that conspiracy theories provide a form of instant gratification to people. Somewhat analogous to smoking or gambling, conspiracy beliefs may be psychologically rewarding in the short run; but perceivers and society pay a price for them in the long run. It therefore should be emphasized that the main goal of the present contribution was to understand what makes conspiracy theories appealing despite all of Benefits of conspiracy theories Prooijen 3 www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2022, 47:101352 their negative effects. Recognizing that there are psychological benefits to believing conspiracy theories does not imply a recommendation to endorse them, nor is it an argument against interventions to reduce conspiracy beliefs (e.g., debunking and prebunking; [57]). The appeal of conspiracy theories notwithstanding, in the end perceivers and society benefit most from reason and truth. Conflict of interest statement Nothing declared. References Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as: * of special interest * * of outstanding interest 1. Bierwiaczonek K, Kunst JR, Pich O: Belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories reduces social distancing over time. Appl Psychol: Health Well-Being 2020, 12:1270–1285. 2. Grebe E, Nattrass N: AIDS conspiracy beliefs and unsafe sex in Cape Town. AIDS Behav 2012, 16:761–773. 3. Hornsey MJ, Finlayson M, Chatwood G, Begeny CT: Donald Trump and vaccination, 103947 vaccination: the effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy. J Exp Soc Psychol 2020, 88: 103947. 4. Imhoff R, Lamberty P: A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 2020, 11:1110–1118. 5. Jolley D, Douglas K: The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One 2014, 9:e89177. 6. Cichocka A, Marchlewska M, Golec de Zavala A: Doe self-love or self- hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, selfesteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 2016, 7:157–166. 7 * . Van Prooijen J-W, Etienne T, Kutiyski T, Krouwel APM: Conspiracy beliefs prospectively predict health behavior and well-being during a pandemic. Psychol Med 2021, https:// doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004438. This article illuminates that conspiracy theories are harmful in the long run for perceivers’ health and well-being. 8. Van Prooijen J-W, Spadaro G, Wang H: Suspicion of institutions: how distrust and conspiracy theories deteriorate social relationships. Curr Opin Psychol 2022, 43:65–69. 9. Van der Linden S: The conspiracy-effect: exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Pers Indiv Differ 2015, 87: 171–173. 10. Hornsey MJ, Harris EA, Fielding KS: Relationships among conspiratorial beliefs, conservatism, and climate scepticism across nations. Nat Clim Change 2018, 8:614–620. 11. Pummerer L, Böhm R, Lilleholt L, Winter K, Zettler I, Sassenberg K: Conspiracy theories and their societal effects during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 2021, https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506211000217. 12. Van Prooijen J-W, Etienne T, Kutiyski T, Krouwel APM: Just a flu? Self-perceived infection mediates the link between conspiracy beliefs and Covid-19 health beliefs and behaviors. J Health Psychol 2021, https://doi.org/10.1177/ 13591053211051816. 13. Erisen C, Guidi M, Martini M, Toprakkiran S, Isernia P, Littvay L: Psychological correlates of populist attitudes. Adv Polit Psychol 2021, 42:149–172. 14. Imhoff R, Zimmer F, Klein O, António JHC, Babinska M, Bangerter A, Van Prooijen J-W: Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nat Human Behav 2022, 6:392–403. 15. Van Prooijen J-W, Cohen Rodrigues T, Bunzel C, Georgescu O, Komáromy D, Krouwel APM: Populist gullibility: conspiracy theories, news credibility, bullshit receptivity, and paranormal belief. Polit Psychol 2022, https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12802. 16. Imhoff R, Dieterle L, Lamberty P: Resolving the puzzle of conspiracy worldview and political activism: belief in secret plots decreases normative but increases nonnormative political engagement. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 2021, 12:71–79. 17. Jolley D, Paterson JL: Pylons ablaze: examining the role of 5G COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence. Br J Soc Psychol 2020, 59:628–640. 18. Rottweiler B, Gill P: Conspiracy beliefs and violent extremist intentions: the contingent effects of self-efficacy, self-control and law-related morality. Terrorism Polit Violence 2020, https:// doi.org/10.1080/09546553.2020.1803288. 19. Sternisko A, Cichocka A, Van Bavel JJ: The dark side of social movements, movements: social identity, non-conformity, and the lure of conspiracy. Curr Opin Psychol 2020, 35:1–6. 20. Van Prooijen J-W, Van Vugt M: Conspiracy theories: evolved functions and psychological mechanisms. Perspect Psychol Sci 2018, 13:770–788. 21. Imhoff R: Conspiracy theories through a cross-cultural lens. Online Readings, Readings Psychol Cult 2022, 5, https://doi.org/ 10.9707/2307-0919.1175. 22. Alper S: There are higher levels of conspiracy beliefs in more corrupt countries. PsyArXiv 2021, https://doi.org/10.31234/ osf.io/2umfe. 23. Douglas KM, Uscinski JE, Sutton RM, Cichocka A, Nefes T, Ang CS, Deravi F: Understanding conspiracy theories. Adv Polit Psychol 2019, 40:3–35. 24 * . Liekefett L, Christ O, Becker JC: Can conspiracy beliefs be beneficial?Longitudinal linkages between conspiracy beliefs, anxiety, uncertainty aversion, and existential threat. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2021, https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672211060965. Research showing that conspiracy theories do not reduce, and may even increase, anxiety, uncertainty aversion, and feelings of existential threat. 25. Poon K-T, Chen Z, Wong W-Y: Beliefs in conspiracy theories following ostracism. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2020, 46:1234–1246. 26. Lantian A, Muller D, Nurra C, Klein O, Berjot S, Pantazi M: Stigmatized beliefs: conspiracy theories, anticipated negative evaluation of the self, and fear of social exclusion. Eur J Soc Psychol 2018, 48:939–954. 27. Van Prooijen J-W, Douglas KM: Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Mem Stud 2017, 10:323–333. 28. Kruglanski AW, Gelfand MJ, Bélanger JJ, Sheveland A, Hetiarachchi M, Gunaratra R: The psychology of radicalization and deradicalization: how significance quest impacts violent extremism. Adv Polit Psychol 2014, 35:69–93, https://doi.org/ 10.1111/pops.12163. 29. Imhoff R, Lamberty P: Too special to be duped: need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. Eur J Soc Psychol 2017, 47:724–734. 30. Lantian A, Muller D, Nurra C, Douglas KM: “I know things they don’t know!”: the role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Soc Psychol 2017, 48:160–173. 31 * * . Biddlestone M, Green R, Cichocka A, Sutton R, Douglas K: Conspiracy beliefs and the individual, relational, and collective selves. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 2021, 15:e12639. Review paper illuminating how conspiracy theories can help people defend their self-concept at various levels. 32. Van Prooijen J-W: Sometimes inclusion breeds suspicion: self-uncertainty and belongingness predict belief in conspiracy theories. Eur J Soc Psychol 2016, 46:267–279. 4 Conspiracy Theories (2023) Current Opinion in Psychology 2022, 47:101352 www.sciencedirect.com 33. Golec de Zavala A, Federico CM: Collective narcissism and the growth of conspiracy thinking over the course of the 2016 United States presidential election: a longitudinal analysis. Eur J Soc Psychol 2018, 48:1011–1018. 34. Sternisko A, Cichocka A, Cislak A, Van Bavel JJ: National narcissism predicts the belief and dissemination of conspiracy theories during the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from 56 countries. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2021, https://doi.org/ 10.1177/01461672211054947. 35. Jolley D, Douglas KM, Leite AC, Schrader T: Belief in conspiracy theories and intentions to engage in everyday crime. Br J Soc Psychol 2019, 58:534–549. 36. Van Prooijen J-W: Injustice without evidence: the unique role of conspiracy theories in social justice research. Soc Justice Res 2022, 35:88–106. 37. Mercier H: Not born yesterday: the science of who we trust and what we believe. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2020. 38. Festinger F: A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press; 1957. 39 * * . Bierwiaczonek K, Gundersen AB, Kunst JR: The role ofconspiracy beliefs for COVID-19 health responses: A meta-analysis. Curr Opin Psychol 2022, 46:101346, https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.copsyc.2022.101346. A meta-analysis showing that conspiracy theories not only predict decreased Covid-19 preventive behaviors over time, but also that decreased Covid-19 preventive behaviors predict increased conspiracy beliefs over time. 40. Enders AM, Smallpage SM: Informational cues, partisanmotivated reasoning, and the manipulation of conspiracy beliefs. Polit Commun 2019, 36:83–102. 41. Miller JM, Saunders KL, Farhart CE: Conspiracy endorsement as motivated reasoning: the moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. Am J Polit Sci 2016, 60:824–844. 42. Jolley D, Douglas KM, Sutton RM: Blaming a few bad apples to save a threatened barrel: the system-justifying function of conspiracy theories. Polit Psychol 2018, 39:465–478. 43. Mao J-Y, Van Prooijen J-W, Yang S-L, Guo Y-Y: System threat during a pandemic: how conspiracy theories help to justify the system. J Pac Rim Psychol 2021, 15:1–11. 44. Adam-Troïan J, Wagner-Egger P, Motyl M, Arciszewski T, Imhoff R, Zimmer F, … van Prooijen JW: Investigating the links between cultural variables and belief in conspiracy theories: the key roles of collectivism and masculinity. Polit Psychol 2021, 42:597–618. 45. Lin Y, Zhang YC, Oyserman D: Seeing meaning even when none may exist: collectivism increases belief in empty claims. J Pers Soc Psychol 2022, 122:351–366. 46. Van Prooijen J-W, Song M: The cultural dimension of intergroup conspiracy theories. Br J Psychol 2021, 112: 455–473. 47. Butter M, Knight P: Routledge handbook of conspiracy theories. Oxon, UK: Routledge; 2020. 48. Van Boven L, Kane J, McGraw AP, Dale J: Feeling close: emotional intensity reduces perceived psychological distance. J Pers Soc Psychol 2010, 98:872–885. 49. Brotherton R, Eser S: Bored to fears: boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Pers Indiv Differ 2015, 80: 1–5. 50 * * . Van Prooijen J-W, Ligthart J, Rosema S, Xu Y: The entertainment value of conspiracy theories. Br J Psychol 2022, 113: 25–48. Research showing how belief in conspiracy theories can be psychologically beneficial by entertaining people. 51. Nussio E: The role of sensation seeking in violent armed group participation. Terrorism Polit Violence 2020, 32:1–19, https://doi.org/10.1080/09546553.2017.1342633. 52. Schumpe B, Bélanger JJ, Moyano M, Nisa CF: The role of sensation seeking in political violence: an extension of the significance quest theory. J Pers Soc Psychol 2020, 118: 743–761. 53. Smith CL, Johnson JL, Hathaway W: Personality contributions to belief in paranormal phenomena. Indiv Differ Res 2009, 7: 85–96. 54. Zuckerman M, Eysenck S, Eysenck HJ: Sensation seeking in England and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. J Consult Clin Psychol 1978, 46:139–149. 55. Whitson JA, Galinsky AD, Kay A: The emotional roots of conspiratorial perceptions, system justification, and belief in the paranormal. J Exp Soc Psychol 2015, 56:89–95. 56. Clarke S: Conspiracy theories and conspiracy theorizing. Philos Soc Sci 2002, 32:131–150. 57 * . Van der Linden S: Misinformation: susceptibility, spread, and interventions to immunize the public. Nat Med 2022, 28: 460–467. Paper reviewing interventions to reduce conspiracy theories and other forms of misinformation among the public. Benefits of conspiracy theories Prooijen 5 www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2022, 47:101352