Chapter 2.3 Abstracts The abstract, although it heads the article, is often written last, together with the title. This is partly because writers know what they have achieved, and parrly because it is not easy to write an abstract. Abstracts have ro summarise whar has been done, sometimes in as few as 150 words. It is easier to write an abstract if you remember that all abstracts have a basic structure. Indeed, the phrase 'structured abstracts' says it all. This kind of abstract, common in medical research journals and now appearing in many social science articles, can be adapted for most normal purposes. STRUCTURED ABSTRACTS Structured abstracts are typically written using five sub-headings — 'background', 'aim', 'method', 'results' and 'conclusions'. Sometimes the wording of these sub-headings varies a little - 'objectives' for 'aim', for example, but the meaning is much the same. Structured abstracts were introduced into medical research journals in the 1980s. Since then they have been widely used in medicine and other areas of research (Nakayama a al,, 2005). In 2004, I published a narrative review of their effectiveness based upon thirty-one research papers available at that time (Hartley, 2004). I concluded that, compared with traditional abstracts, structured abstracts: • contained more information • were easier to read • were easier to search • facilitated peer review for conferences • were generally welcomed by readers and by authors. Figure 2.3.1a below shows a typical structured abstract. Figure 2.3.1b shows the same abstract written with the sub-headings removed. It can be seen that both abstracts are clear, and so it is useful to write an abstract in 32 The academic article Abstracts 33 Background. In 1997 four journals published by the British Psychological Society began publishing structured abstracts. Aims. The aim of the studies reported here was to assess the effects of these structured abstracts by comparing them with original versions written in a traditional, unstructured format. Method. The authors of the articles accepted for publication in the four journals were asked to supply copies of their traditional abstracts (written when the paper was submitted for publication) together with copies of their structured abstracts requested by the editor when their paper was accepted. Forty-eight such requests were made, and thirty pairs of abstracts were obtained. The abstracts were then compared on a number of measures. Results. Analysis showed that the structured abstracts were significantly more readable, significantly longer and significantly more informative than the traditional ones. Judges assessed the contents of the structured abstracts more quickly and with significantly less difficulty than they did the traditional ones. Almost every respondent expressed positive attitudes to structured abstracts. Conclusions. The structured abstracts fared significantly better than the traditional ones on every measure used in this enquiry. We recommend, therefore, that editors of other journals in the social sciences consider adopting structured abstracts. figure 2.3. la An original abstract in structured form. Adapted from Hartley and Benjamin (1998), and reproduced with permission of the British Journal of Educational Psychology. © the British Psychological Society. a structured form first, and then to adjust it for the journal you are writing for if this journal does not use them. Figures 2.3.1a and b illustrate some of the virtues of structured abstracts. Using the sub-headings and the appropriately spaced typographical layout makes the content clearer (Hartley and Betrs, 2007). Furthermore, structured abstracts are easier for readers to scan, as every absrract follows the same format. The sub-headings thus allow the readers to go to the same place each time in an abstract to find out what it says. Furthermore, as the information required has to be provided by the author under each sub-heading, nothing gets missed out. With traditional abstracts, it is all too common to find that some elements are missing — the background, the method or the results, for example. Often one is left saying, 'So, what happened?' or So what?'. In 1997 four journals published by the British Psychological Society began publishing structured abstracts. The aim of the studies reported here was to assess the effects of these structured abstracts by comparing them with original versions written in a traditional, unstructured format. The authors of the articles accepted for publication in the four journals were asked to supply copies of their traditional abstracts (written when the paper was submitted for publication) together with copies of their structured abstracts requested by the editor when their paper was accepted. Forty-eight such requests were made and thirty pairs of abstracts were obtained. The abstracts were then compared on a number of measures. Analysis showed that the structured abstracts were significantly more readable, significantly longer and significantly more informative than the traditional ones. Judges assessed the contents of the structured abstracts more quickly and with significantly less difficulty than they did the traditional ones. Almost every respondent expressed positive attitudes to structured abstracts. In short, the structured abstracts fared significantly better than the traditional ones on every measure used in this enquiry. We recommend, therefore, that editors of other journals in the social sciences consider adopting structured abstracts. figure 2.3, / b The same abstract in unstructured form. Many people think that structured abstracts are only suitable for empirical papers - those with 'methods' and results'. As one of my correspondents put it: It seems to me that the format you have chosen imposes a unitary conception of research, at a time when educational research in particular, and social science more widely, has at last broken away from narrow strictures of method and procedure. However, I believe that the underlying characteristics of a structured abstract can apply to many other forms of enquiry. Figure 2.3.2a, for example, shows an original abstract written to accompany a review paper. Figure 2.3.2b shows a revision of it that, in my view, makes the background, aims and conclusions of the study more explicit. Bayley and Eldredge (2003) provide references to a variety of papers in the health sciences that have structured abstracts. These include qualitative studies, narrative reviews, systematic reviews, meta-analyses and randomised controlled trials. Table 2.3.1 similarly lists some more recent papers in the 34 The academic article Abstracts 35 There is something of a controversy taking place over how best to theorise human learning. In this article we join the debate over the relationships between sociocultural and constructive perspectives on learning. These two perspectives differ in not just their conceptions of knowledge (epistemological assumptions) but also in their assumptions about the known world and the knowing human (ontological assumptions). We articulate in this article six themes of a nondualist ontology seen at work in the sociocultural perspective, and suggest a reconciliation of the two. We propose that learning involves becoming a member of a community, constructing knowledge of various levels of expertise as a participant, but also taking a stand on the culture of one's community in an effort to take up and overcome the estrangement and division that are consequences of participation. Learning entails transformation of both the person and the social world. We explore the implications of this view for thinking about schooling and for the conduct of educational research. Figure 2.3.2a An original abstract for a review paper. Reproduced with permission from Packer and Goicoechea (2000) and Taylor & Francis, www. informaworld.com. health and social sciences chat have used structured abstracts with a variety of research methods. After the title, the abstract is the most frequently read part of any paper. Writing it in a structured format (with or without the headings) ensures that it is informative and complete- Table 2.3.1 Examples of studies with structured abstracts published in the health and social sciences Method Example Literature review Mayhew and Simpson (2002) Observational study Lauth et at. (2006) Survey Wilding and Andrews (2006) Longitudinal study Flouri (2006) Statistical paper Prosser and Trigwell (2006) Simulation Wright (2006) Experimental study Clariana and Koul (2006) Epidemiological study Evans (2000) Meta-analysis Bunn et al. (2006) Systematic review Duperrex et al. (2006) Qualitative study Maliski et al. (2002) Background. An interesting debate is currently taking place among proponents of different ways of thinking about human learning. In this article we focus on that portion of the debate that addresses sociological and constructive perspectives on learning. These two perspectives differ in not just their conceptions of knowledge (epistemological assumptions) but also in their assumptions about the known world and the knowing human (ontological assumptions). Aims and approach. We wish to try and reconcile these two different approaches first by examining the ontological assumptions of them both. We then consider six key themes of a nondualist ontology seen at work in the sociocultural perspective. Finally we propose that the constructive perspective attends to epistemological structures and processes which the sociological perspective must place in a broader historical and cultural context. Conclusions. We conclude that learning involves becoming a member of a community, constructing knowledge of various levels of expertise as a participant, and taking a stand on the culture of one's community in an effort to take up and overcome the estrangement and division that are consequences of participation. Learning entails transformation of both the personal and the social world. We explore the implications of this view for thinking about schooling and the conduct of educational research. Figure 2.3.2b The same abstract in structured form. REFERENCES Bayley, L. & Eldred^e, J. E. (2003). The structured abstract: An essential tool for researchers. Hypothesis, 17(1), 1 and 11-15. Or: http://research.mhlnet.org/structurcd_ abstract.html. (Retrieved 1 August 2006.) Bunn, F„ Collier, T., Frost, C„ Ker, K., Roberts, I. & Wentz, R. (2003). Traffic calming for the prevention of road traffic injuries: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Injury Prevention, 9(3), 200-4. Clariana, R. B. & Koul, R. (2006). The effects of different forms of feedback on ftuzy and verbatim memory of science ptinciples. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 259-70. Duperrex, O., Bunn, F. & Roberts, I. (2002). Safety education of pedestrians for injury prevention: A systematic review of randomised controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 324(7348), 1129-34. Evans, L. (2000). Risks older drivers face themselves and threats they pose to other toad users. International Journal oj Epidemiology, 29(2), 315—22. 36 The academic article Flouri, E. (2006). Parental interest in children's education, children's self-esteem and locus of control, and larer educational attainment: Twenty-six year follow-up of the 1970 British Birth Cohort. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(1), 41-56. Hartley, J. (2004). Current findings from research on structured abstracts. Journal of the Medical Library Association, 92(3), 368-71. Hartley, J. & Benjamin, M. (1998). An evaluation of structured abstracts in journals published by the British Psychological Society. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68(3), 443-56. Hartley, J. & Belts, L. (2007). The effects of spacing and titles on judgments of the effectiveness of structured abstracts. Journal of the American Society for Information Science & Technology, 38(14), 2335—40. Lauth, G. W., Hcubeck, B. G. & Mackowiak, K. (2006). Observation of children with attention-deficit hyperactivity (ADHD) problems in three natural classroom contexts. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 385-404. Maliski, S. L., Heilemann, M. V. Sc. McCorkle, R. (2002). From 'death sentence' to 'good cancer': Couples' transformation of a prostate cancer diagnosis. Nursing Research, 5(6), 391-7. Mayhcw, D. R. & Simpson, H. M. (2002). The safety value of driver education and training. Injury Prevention, 8 (Suppl. II): ii3-ii8. Nakayama, T., Hirai, N., Yamazaki, S. & Naito, M. (2005). Adoption of structured absttacts by general medical journals and format for a structured abstract. Journal of the Medical Library Association, 95(2), 237-42. Packer, M. J. & Goicoechea, J. (2000). Sociocultural and constructivist theories of learning: Ontological, not just epistemology. Educational Psychologist, 35(4), 227-41. Prosser, M. & Trigwell, K. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Approaches to Teaching Inventory. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 405-19. Wilding, J. & Andrews, B. (2006). Life goals, approaches to study and performance in an undergraduate cohort. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(1), 171-82. Wtight, D. B. (2006). Comparing groups in a before-after design: When t test and ANCOVA produce different results. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(3), 663-75. FURTHER READING Hartley, J., Rock, J. & Fox, C. (2005). Teaching psychology students to write structured abstracts: An evaluation study. Psychology Teaching Review, 1(1), 2-11. Kamler, B. & Thomson, P. (2004). Driven to abstraction: Doctoral supervision and writing pedagogies. Teaching in Higher Education, 9(2), 195-209. Kelly, A. E. & Yin, R. K. (2007). Strengthening structured abstracts for education research: The need for claim-based structured abstracts. Educational Researcher, 36(3), 133-8. Chapter 2.4 Key words Key words typically: 1 allow readers to judge whether or not an article contains marerial relevant to their interests; 2 provide readers with suitable terms to use in web-based searches to locate other materials on the same or similar topics; 3 help indexers/editors group together related materials in, say, the end-of-year issues of a particular journal or a set of conference proceedings; 4 allow editors/researchers to document changes in a subject discipline (over time); and 5 link the specific issues of concern to issues at a higher level of abstraction. WHO USES KEY WORDS? There appear to be no formal requirements for key words, no rules for formulating them, little guidance on how to write them, and no instructions for reviewers on how to assess them. This is surprising in view of the fact that, presumably, a wise choice of key words increases the probability that a paper will be retrieved and read, thereby potentially improving citation counts and journal impact factors. Table 2.4.1 shows, however, that there are typical disciplinary differences in the percentage of journals using key words. Table 2.4.1 The approximate percentages of research journals in different areas and disciplines supplying key words Arts Education Psychology Science Medicine Statistics 5 20 30 SO 50 75 Hartley and Kostoff (2003). 38 The academic article Key words 39 WHO CHOOSES THE KEY WORDS? Table 2.4.2 shows that there are several different ways of choosing key words. The most common method (used by over fifty per cent of avithors) is for them to supply as many words as they choose (within bounds), but sometimes a specified number of words is required (often about six). The next main method (used by about twenty per cent of authors) is for them to choose key words that fit into categories already prescribed by the journal's 'instructions to authors'. Thus, for example, authors generating key words for medical articles often have to select only words from the medical subject headings (MeSH) taxonomy - a structured taxonomy used by MEDLINE. In situations like this the number of words allowed and the number of categories to choose from can vary. Many psychology journals, for example, ask authors to list key words from any of the 5,000 terms that appeat in the American Psychological Society's Thesaurus of Psychological Index Term. Finally, key words are sometimes generated automatically at proof stage (as is the case for the Journal of Information Science, where the key words are derived from Library and Information Science Abstracts). HOW TO SELECT KEY WORDS Gbur and Trumbo (1995) published a list of ways of producing effective key words and phrases. Table 2.4.3 provides an abbreviated version. It is possible that, with future developments, all of these problems will actually disappear. As one colleague has put it, 'Inverted-full-text-Boolean indexing and online seatching (with similarity algorithms and citation-Table 2.4.2 Different methods for supplying key words Authors supply them with no restrictions on the numbers allowed. Authors supply up to a fixed number (e.g. six). Authors supply key words as appropriate from a specified list. Editors supplement/amend authors' key words. Editors supply key words. Editors supply key words from a specified list. Referees supply key words from a specified list. Key words are allocated according to the 'house-rules' applied to all journals distributed by a specific publisher. Key words are determined by computer program at proof stage. Hartley and Kostoff (2003). Table 2.4.3 Ten ways to produce effective key words and phrases 1 Use simple, specific noun clauses. For example, use variance estimation, not estimate of variance. 2 Avoid terms that are too common. Otherwise the number of 'hits' will be too large to manage. 3 Do not repeat key words from the title. These will be picked up anyway. 4 Avoid unnecessary prepositions, especially in and of. For example, use data quality rather than