Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 1 (+/-)-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, “Ecstasy”) Investigator’s Brochure Lisa Jerome Ph.D December 2007 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 2 Table of Contents Preface................................................................................................................................. 3 New Developments............................................................................................................. 3 MDMA research and Interspecies Scaling ..................................................................... 3 The Netherlands XTC Toxicity (NeXT) Studies............................................................ 4 No Interaction between MDMA and Ambient Temperature on Human Body Temperature.................................................................................................................... 5 1. Drug Substance and Formulation.................................................................................... 5 2. Pharmacological and toxicological effects ..................................................................... 6 Overview......................................................................................................................... 6 MDMA and Monoamines............................................................................................... 7 Receptor Affinity and Direct and Indirect Actions on Receptors............................... 9 MDMA and Gene Expression................................................................................... 10 Physiological Effects .................................................................................................... 10 Humans ..................................................................................................................... 11 MDMA and Brain Activity....................................................................................... 12 Neuroendocrine Effects of MDMA .......................................................................... 13 Immunological Effects of MDMA............................................................................ 13 Behavioral and psychological effects ........................................................................... 13 Humans ..................................................................................................................... 14 3. Pharmacokinetics and biological disposition................................................................ 16 4. Safety and effectiveness in humans obtained from prior clinical studies..................... 17 MDMA-assisted psychotherapy for PTSD................................................................... 18 MDMA for anxiety arising from diagnosis with a potentially life-threatening illness. 19 MDMA and Parkinson’s disease .................................................................................. 19 5. Possible Risks and Side Effects.................................................................................... 20 Overview....................................................................................................................... 20 Fatalities........................................................................................................................ 20 Common Adverse Effects and Side Effects.................................................................. 20 Medical Emergencies and Adverse Events in Ecstasy Users ....................................... 23 Long-Term Effects........................................................................................................ 24 Central Serotonin Function, Cognition and Affect: Retrospective Studies.............. 24 Changes in Serotonin Function and Indicators of Neuronal Injury or Repair.......... 24 Impaired Cognitive Function.................................................................................... 25 Psychological Well-being, Affect, Impulsivity, and Sleep....................................... 26 Prospective Studies................................................................................................... 27 Conclusions............................................................................................................... 28 Abuse Potential............................................................................................................. 28 Reproductive and Developmental Toxicity .................................................................. 29 6. Research trial data......................................................................................................... 30 7. Acknowledgements....................................................................................................... 30 References......................................................................................................................... 30 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 3 Preface In 2001, Baggott, Jerome and Stuart collaborated on a comprehensive review of the literature concerning 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, or MDMA (Baggott et al. 2001). After writing this monumental work, three additional reviews were created to assist and support the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies (MAPS), first by Jerome and Baggott in 2003, and then Jerome in 2004 and 2005 (Jerome 2004; 2005; Jerome and Baggott 2003). As the amount and pace of research accelerated, and after gaining more knowledge about investigator’s brochures, the present author (Jerome) concluded that information on MDMA should be presented in a more accessible and compact format. Fortuitously, a number of excellent reviews of nearly all aspects of MDMA have appeared in the past four years, making the task of summarizing and presenting this research easier. As in all previous documents, this brochure is based on literature located through the electronic database PubMed, examining relevant journals and communication with researchers. With the exception of some information on ongoing studies and conference presentations, most of the information has been drawn from reports appearing in print or electronic editions of relevant journals. This document is intended to encapsulate the current state of our knowledge concerning MDMA, especially as it pertains to humans. Rather than follow the format of its predecessors, this document will follow the outline used for recently created investigator’s brochures for LSD and psilocybin. There will be a greater focus on proposed mechanisms of action, cataloguing acute physiological, behavioral and therapeutic effects in humans, and information on planned or ongoing use in medical settings. Readers interested learning more about MDMA or specific areas are strongly encouraged to read reviews by Cole and Sumnall, Dumont and Verkes, Laws and Kokkalis, Zakzanis and others on the pharmacology, acute and potential long-term effects and risks of MDMA and ecstasy, with ecstasy defined here as material represented as containing MDMA (Baylen and Rosenberg 2006; Cole and Sumnall 2003a; b; Dumont and Verkes 2006; Green et al. 2003; Guillot 2007; Laws and Kokkalis 2007; Sumnall and Cole 2005; Zakzanis et al. 2007). New Developments Three major developments have transformed our understanding of previous and current findings in the area of MDMA research since the last update of the literature review. The first development affects interpretation of nonhuman animal studies, and the second development relates to our understanding of the presence and degree of long-term effects of ecstasy use. The third development relates to primate studies examining the impact of ambient temperature on the effects of MDMA on body temperature. MDMA research and Interspecies Scaling Up until as recently as 2006, investigations of the pharmacology, functional effects or toxicity of MDMA in nonhuman animals injected large and often repeated doses of MDMA (see Baggott et al. 2001). Some of the earliest research used high doses in order to study mechanisms of neurotoxicity. Most researchers continued to employ these doses, and some later explained these dosing regimens in terms of using interspecies scaling to Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 4 arrive at human-equivalent doses. Interspecies scaling is a pharmacological model intended to account for species-specific differences in metabolism (Mahmood and Balian 1996; Mordenti and Chappell 1989). However, interspecies scaling assumes that the target compound has linear pharmacokinetics, and requires data from at least two species to arrive at human-equivalent doses. Findings first reported in humans and later in monkeys (de la Torre et al. 2000; Mechan et al. 2006) demonstrate that MDMA has nonlinear pharmacokinetics, with larger doses producing greater increases in blood MDMA levels than would be expected from linear pharmacokinetics. Furthermore, contrary to earlier reports that found that route of administration had no impact on plasma MDMA levels (Finnegan et al. 1988; Ricaurte et al. 1988), a recent study of MDMA in monkeys found that sub-cutaneous doses produced higher peak plasma MDMA levels than intra-gastric administrations (Mechan et al. 2006). Some recent reports describe findings suggesting that even the doses of MDMA employed in rodent studies are inappropriately high (Baumann et al. 2006; Wang et al. 2005), though see work by Xie and colleagues (Xie et al. 2006). After examining these recent findings, it appears that much of the previous research in nonhuman animals has used inappropriately high doses of MDMA, and this occurred not only in studies of MDMA neurotoxicity, but also in studies of behavioral or pharmacological effects. Studies conducted subsequent to the discovery began employing mg/kg doses similar or identical to those used by humans (e.g.Taffe et al. 2006; Von Huben et al. 2006). Most drug-discrimination and selfadministration studies in rodents (De La Garza et al. 2007) have employed and continue to employ doses identical or similar to those used by humans. However, the discoveries concerning the inadequacy of interspecies scaling mean that findings from the majority of previous studies must be considered with caution. The Netherlands XTC Toxicity (NeXT) Studies A team of researchers in the Netherlands embarked upon an ambitious program of research that included prospective studies of people who planned on using ecstasy in the future (De Win et al. 2005). Participants expressing interest in using ecstasy were assessed prior to use. After some reported their first use of ecstasy or soon afterwards, the team assessed these participants as well as another group that had not yet used ecstasy. The researchers studied brain activity, estimated brain serotonin transporter (SERT) sites, neurochemical markers of brain injury, working memory, and cognitive function including verbal memory. These studies are unique in their examination of people before and after drug self-administration rather than making cross-group comparisons between ecstasy users and non-user controls. Hence findings from these prospective studies far more relevant to examining risks and benefits in human MDMA studies than the majority of previous work. In general, studies from this research program failed to find any longterm effects of low to moderate ecstasy use. They did not find any changes in serotonin uptake sites nor chemical markers of brain injury, and they found only minor regionspecific changes in cerebral blood volume (de Win et al. 2007; de Win et al. 2006; Jager et al. 2007b). While an examination of change scores before and after use indicated greater improvement in non-users than in ecstasy users, they failed to find impaired working memory after low ecstasy use (Jager et al. 2007b; Schilt et al. 2007). There are a number of problems with this research, including use of change scores and the inclusion of an individual whose ecstasy use was considerably greater than others in the cognitive Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 5 function assessment. However, despite these findings, the general picture from the NeXT studies is that low to moderate ecstasy use produces very few long-term effects and is not comparable to effects found after heavy use of ecstasy and other drugs. No Interaction between MDMA and Ambient Temperature on Human Body Temperature To date, the only novel and relevant finding from human MDMA studies is a study that sought to detect an interaction between MDMA and ambient temperature on human body temperature after 2 mg/kg MDMA (Freedman et al. 2005). Freedman and colleagues found that human body temperature was unaffected by the combination of MDMA and ambient temperature, and that the desirable effects of MDMA are not increased by warm ambient temperature (Freedman et al. 2005). One of two research teams reached similar conclusions in rhesus monkeys (Crean et al. 2006; Von Huben et al. 2006). 1. Drug Substance and Formulation (+/-) 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, 3,4-methylenedioxy-nmethylamphetamine, N-methyl-3,4-methylenedioxyamphetaine, ) has the chemical formula of C11H15NO2. It is a phenylisopopylamine derived from safrole, an aromatic oil found in sassafras, nutmeg, and other plants (Shulgin 1986). Merck patented MDMA in 1912 as an intermediate chemical involved in the production of the styptic hydrastinine (Freudenmann et al. 2006). No significant investigations examined the pharmacological, physiological or psychological effects of MDMA until the 1950s, when the US Army administered MDMA to guinea pigs, monkeys, mice, rats and dogs, but not humans, as part of a military research program, possibly intended to develop chemical incapacitants or means of enhancing interrogation (Hardman et al. 1973). While evidence exists for intentional use of MDMA as early as the late 1960s (see Shulgin and Shulgin 1991), and there are records of a police seizure of MDMA in the early 1970s that suggests either intentional or unintentional use (Gaston 1972), Shulgin and Nichols were the first to report on the effects MDMA in humans (Shulgin and Nichols 1978). Shulgin introduced MDMA to a psychotherapist he knew, and the psychotherapist went on to introduce MDMA as a psychotherapeutic adjunct to others, with MDMA-assisted psychotherapy first occurring during the mid to late 1970s. Some have estimated that up to 4000 people underwent MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in North America prior to its placement in Schedule 1. Psychotherapists used it to treat anxiety and depression, and posttraumatic stress disorder (Greer and Tolbert 1998; Metzner and Adamson 2001). However, none of the therapists conducted controlled studies or published any formal descriptions or analyses of MDMA-assisted therapy at this time. Therapeutic use continued up until its placement in US Schedule 1 in 1986 (Adamson 1985; Greer and Tolbert 1998; Stolaroff 1997). During the early 1980s, increasing numbers of people began using MDMA outside the therapeutic context (Beck and Rosenbaum 1994). The first wave of non-medical use occurred not only in dance clubs but in small groups of people, in a self-exploratory or spiritual context or while attending concerts. Non-medical use continues today in the same contexts (Carlson et al. 2004; Sumnall et al. 2006). A few uncontrolled human studies of MDMA occurred in the 1980s (Downing 1986; Greer and Tolbert 1986), including Greer and Tolbert’s study of MDMA in a Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 6 psychotherapeutic context. However, controlled human studies of MDMA did not commence until early to mid-1990s, with the publication of research conducted by Grob and colleagues (Grob et al. 1996). Currently, ongoing investigations in the US and Switzerland are examining the use of MDMA in psychotherapy (Halpern 2006; Mithoefer 2006; Oehen 2006). Many researchers categorize MDMA as belonging to a unique class of drugs, the entactogens (Nichols 1986; Vollenweider et al. 1998a), of which it is considered the first identified member, and perhaps a prototype of the class. Entactogens are reported to produce changes in mood, social interactions or feelings of interpersonal closeness and changes in perception. MDMA shares some of the pharmacological effects of stimulants and serotonergic hallucinogens (Cami et al. 2000; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al. 1999; Liechti et al. 2001b; Tancer and Johanson 2003), but it also appears to possess qualities it shares in common with a small number of related compounds, as methylenedioxyethylamphetamine (MDE) (Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al. 1999). Retrospective reports and surveys have assessed the social cognitive effects of MDMA or ecstasy (Liester et al. 1992; Peroutka et al. 1988; Solowij et al. 1992). To date only two controlled studies have sought to measure these effects (Farre et al. 2007; Harris et al. 2002). Although researchers have offered several models and explanations for the source of entactogenic effects, it appears that release of serotonin plays a significant role in producing at least some of these effects. Indirect action on 5HT1A or 5HT2A receptors, and neuroendocrine responses, as increases in the hormones oxytocin, vasopressin and cortisol may also play a role in producing the unique effects of MDMA. 2. Pharmacological and toxicological effects Overview MDMA possesses a complex pharmacological profile, but it is dominated by its effects on monoamine release and reuptake. MDMA prevents uptake of serotonin (5-HT), norepinephrine (NE) and dopamine (DA) and is involved in the release of these three transmitters, with the greatest effects on serotonin release. While MDMA also has some affinity for specific serotonin, norepinephrine, acetylcholine and histamine receptors, strength of activity on these receptors is low (Battaglia et al. 1988; Setola et al. 2003, see also values listed on NIMH Psychoactive Drug Screening Program). There are a few studies of changes in gene expression seen after MDMA, but given that these studies use high doses of MDMA and examination of gene expression occurred at times falling between acute and sub-acute effects, the significance of these findings are unclear. MDMA is chiral, possessing two enantiomers, S-(+)-MDMA and R-(-)-MDMA, with S(+)-MDMA is more potent than R-(-)-MDMA (Lyon et al. 1986; Shulgin 1986). Rodent drug-discrimination and behavioral studies (Fantegrossi et al. 2003; Yarosh et al. 2007) and self-administration studies in monkeys (Fantegrossi 2007), suggest that not only do the enantiomers produce different effects, but that there may be some synergy between the two. One microdialysis study suggests that S-(+)-MDMA is associated with greater dopamine release in specific brain areas (Acquas et al. 2007). However, most if not all street doses are racemic, meaning they contain roughly equal amounts of both enantiomers, and all controlled studies to date also employed a racemic mixture. The Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 7 nature of differential effects of the two enantiomers of MDMA remain unknown in humans. An early uncontrolled study suggests differential effects (Anderson et al. 1978), and an a controlled study comparing the enantiomers of the related compound MDE reported R-(-)-MDE to more strongly affect visual perception than the S-(+)-enantiomer (Spitzer et al. 2001). Intravenous MDMA has an LD50 of 97 mg/kg in mice and 49 mg/kg in rats, 14 to 18 mg/kg in dogs and 22 mg/kg in monkeys (Frith et al. 1987; Hardman et al. 1973). Estimating from this data, LD50 in humans is liable to fall between 10 and 20 mg/kg (Shulgin 1986). One team of researchers reported that in mice, aggregate LD50 was 20 mg/kg, considerably lower than values in isolated animals, and recent studies in mice confirm lower LD50 when mice are housed together (Davis et al. 1987; Fantegrossi et al. 2003). Typically, human trials have used doses between 1 and 2 mg/kg. MDMA and Monoamines While the pharmacology of MDMA is complex, its chief effect is the release of monoamines, particularly serotonin. MDMA also releases norepinephrine and dopamine (Cole and Sumnall 2003b) and prevents uptake of all three monoamines. Recent in vitro studies suggest that MDMA inhibits norepinephrine uptake more strongly than dopamine uptake (Mlinar and Corradetti 2003; Verrico et al. 2007) and that MDMA does not have as strong an affinity for the dopamine transporter as methamphetamine (Han and Gu 2006). MDMA appears to alter the configuration of the serotonin transporter so that it works in reverse of its usual mode, transporting serotonin outside of neurons rather than shuttling extracellular serotonin into these neurons (Cole and Sumnall 2003b; Johnson et al. 1986; see also Rudnick and Wall 1992). While MDMA has some affinity for some serotonin, norepinephrine, acetylcholine and histamine receptors, its direct activity at these receptors is weaker than its activity on monoamine transporters. Early studies of the pharmacological affinity of MDMA reported after detecting strong affinity for serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine transporters, binding at α1, 5HT2, α2 adrenergic receptors, H1 histamine receptors and acetylcholine muscarinic M1 receptors, β adrenergic receptors, dopamine D2 and D1 dopamine receptors, roughly in that order (Battaglia et al. 1988). This study failed to detect any direct pharmacological effects on GABA, glutamate or opioid receptors. Later reports and investigations have clarified and for the most part supported this data (e.g. Jones et al. 2004, see also NIMH PDSP database; Setola et al. 2003), providing more specific affinity assays and with only one significant disagreement, concerning the muscarinic M1 receptor. However, it appears that the effects of MDMA at all receptors investigated to date is considerably lower than its activity at any of the monoamine transporters, suggesting that any receptor-mediated effects reported in humans or nonhuman animals arise indirectly from monoamine release or inhibition of reuptake. For instance, MDMA may cause acetylcholine release and changes in the GABAergic systems through serotonin release activating 5HT4 receptors (Nair and Gudelsky 2005; 2006). Indirect effects of monoamine are likely involved in producing therapeutic effects, as facilitated recall and changed meaning of perceptions and events, increased positive mood and increased interpersonal closeness, empathy or compassion for the self and others. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 8 Table 1: Affinity assays from Battaglia et al. 1988: Affinities of MDMA at receptors wherein K1 is below 500 mcM. Male rat brain samples used in assays. Transporter/Receptor Ki (mcM) Hot Ligand Spe cies Source Reference Serotonin Transporter 0.61 +/- 0.05 3H-paroxetine Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) Norepinephrine Transporter 15.8 +/- 1.7 3H-Mazindol Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) Dopamine Transporter 24.4 +/- 1.9 3H-GBR-12935 Rat Striatum (Battaglia et al. 1988) α2 3.6 +/- 0.8 3H-aminoclonidine Rat Fr. cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) 5HT2 5.1 +/- 0.3 3H-ketanserin Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) H1 5.7 +/- 2.4 3H-mepyramine Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) M1 5.8 +/- 0.3 3H-QNB Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) M2 15.1 +/- 0.1 3H-QNB Rat Brainstem (Battaglia et al. 1988) α1 18.4 +/- 1.2 3H-prazosin Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) Β 19.2+/- 2.1 3H-dihydroalprenolol Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) 5HT1 23 +/- 1.5 3H-serotonin Rat Fr. Cortex (Battaglia et al. 1988) D2 95 +/- 15 3H-Spiperone Rat Striatum (Battaglia et al. 1988) D1 148 +/- 14 3H-SCH-23390 Rat Striatum (Battaglia et al. 1988) Fr. Cortex = Frontal cortex Table 2: Receptor binding profiles for MDMA recorded from the NIMH Psychoactive Drug Screening Program Database (PDSP) Receptor Ki (mcM) Hot Ligand Species Source Reference Serotonin transporter 0.072 or 0.102 Functional (1), 3Hcitalopram (2) Rat, Human Brain, Cloned (Jones et al. 2004; Setola et al. 2003) Norepinephrine Transporter 0.110 Functional Rat Brain (Setola et al. 2003) Dopamine transporter 0.278 Functional Rat Caudate (Setola et al. 2003) 5HT2B 0.5 or 0.7 3H-LSD Human Cloned (Setola et al. 2003), (PDSP 2007) α2C 1.12 3H-Clonidine Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) Calcium Channel 1.2 3H-Nitrendipine Rat Heart (PDSP 2007) α2B 1.8 3H-Clonidine Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) M3 1.8 3H-QNB Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) H1 2.1 3H-Pyrilamine Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) α2A 2.5 3H-Clonidine Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) M5 6.3 3H-QNB Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) M4 8.2 3H-QNB Human Cloned (PDSP 2007) 5HT2A 8.3 3H-ketanserin Rat Cortex (Lyon et al. 1986) Human MDMA studies suggest that serotonin release plays a prominent role in producing the effects of MDMA. Preventing serotonin release through administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) appears to attenuate or eliminate most subjective, physiological and immunological effects of MDMA (Farre et al. 2007; Liechti et al. 2000a; Liechti and Vollenweider 2000b; Pacifici et al. 2004; Tancer and Johanson 2007). Pre-treatment or coadministration with SSRIs attenuated the effects of MDMA on mood and perception, though leaving intact specific effects, as nervousness or excitability (Liechti et al. 2000a). Some researchers reported that SSRIs attenuated MDMA-induced increases in heart rate and blood pressure (Farre et al. 2007; Liechti and Vollenweider 2000b) while others only reported that SSRIs attenuated elevated heart rate only (Tancer and Johanson 2007). All three studies of SSRI pre-treatment suggest that coadministration of SSRIs with MDMA is safe, but that this combination prevents or Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 9 significantly reduces the appearance of the subjective effects of MDMA. The perceptual effects of MDMA appear to be the result of direct or indirect action on 5HT2A receptors, as coadministration of the 5HT2A antagonist ketanserin reduced reported perceptual alterations after 1.5 mg/kg MDMA and eliminated slight elevation in body temperature (Liechti et al. 2000b). At least some MDMA effects on mood and anxiety may result from dopamine release indirectly activating D2 receptors, as administering the D2 antagonist haloperidol diminished positive mood and increased anxiety in humans (Liechti and Vollenweider 2000a). There are no reports examining the contribution of norepinephrine release to MDMA effects in humans. Investigations in rodents suggest that norepinephrine plays a role in cardiovascular effects (Quinn et al. 2006). In rats, relatively large doses of MDMA (10 or 20 mg/kg) elevated serum corticosterone (a rodent cortisol analog) and prolactin (Connor et al. 2000; Nash et al. 1988), with elevation lasting up to four hours after dosing, and attenuated by a 5HT2 receptor antagonist. Given the large dosage used, it is unclear if this response is analogous to elevated cortisol in humans or whether it reflects a different process. A study of isolated rat hypothalamus reported arginine vasopressin and oxytocin release after MDMA and the MDMA metabolite HMMA (Fallon et al. 2002). Receptor Affinity and Direct and Indirect Actions on Receptors Most effects of MDMA on brain receptors likely arise indirectly from monoamine release. MDMA probably stimulates 5HT1A receptors indirectly through serotonin release, though it is possible that MDMA may also act as a partial 5HT1A antagonist in some brain areas (Giannaccini et al. 2007). Early studies in rodents suggest that 5HT1A receptors reduce anxiety and aggression (Brunner and Hen 1997; Graeff et al. 1996), and some drug discrimination studies suggest that the 5HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT partially or fully substitutes for MDMA (Glennon and Young 2000; Glennon et al. 2007; Schechter 1986). Administering a 5HT1A antagonist attenuates the prosocial behavior of adjacent lying in rats given MDMA, possibly because it prevents elevation in oxytocin (Morley et al. 2005; Thompson et al. 2007). To date there are no human studies investigating the contribution of 5HT1A receptors to MDMA effects. MDMA exhibits affinity for two of three 5HT2 family receptors, 5HT2B and 5HT2A, and for α2B, α2C, histamine H1 and muscarinic M3 receptors, with strongest affinity recorded for 5HT2B, α2B, α2C and muscarinic M3 receptors (Battaglia et al. 1988; Lavelle et al. 1999; Lyon et al. 1986; Setola et al. 2003), additional data from PDSP database. A pair of reports suggests MDMA may have some affinity for the trace amine receptor (TAR). In contrast with Battaglia’s work, later investigations did not find high affinity for M1 receptors. It is possible but not yet demonstrated that MDMA agonism at 5HT2B and α2 receptors might play a role in producing some of the subjective or psychological effects of MDMA. Most 5HT2B receptors are found on tissues outside the brain (Setola et al. 2003). However, 5HT2B receptors are also present within the amygdala (Duxon et al. 1997), and so it is possible that these receptors may be involved in regulating emotional reactivity or other amygdalar responses. For example, MDMA and fenfluramine can produce increased or decreased anxiety, or both at once, and feelings of dreaminess, suggesting a few commonalities between the two serotonin releasers and 5HT2B agonists Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 10 (Bond et al. 1995; Mortimore and Anderson 2000). Clonidine and other α2 agonists are associated with increased feelings of calmness and relaxation, and it is possible that, like these compounds, MDMA also induces a calm or relaxed state via α2 agonism. To date there are no behavioral studies examining the role of 5HT2B or α2 adrenergic receptors in the subjective or therapeutic effects of MDMA. Even less is known about the significance of MDMA activity at histamine and muscarinic receptors. At least some direct or indirect effects of MDMA on serotonin receptors may in turn produce additional effects, as changes in GABA uptake in the ventral tegmental area of rats (Bankson and Yamamoto 2004). However, very few publications investigate direct effects of MDMA upon receptors, and it is likely that most effects on serotonin or dopamine receptors result from monoamine release and not direct receptor activation or antagonism. MDMA and Gene Expression A number of research teams have studied the effects of MDMA on gene expression in rodents over the past seven years. However, many of these reports used 10 to 20 mg/kg MDMA, leaving it uncertain whether these changes can be generalized to humans given lower doses. These studies reported an increase in transcripts for genes that regulate the GABA transporter (Peng et al. 2002; Thiriet et al. 2002). Some of the increased gene transcriptions are associated with monoamine release (Peng et al. 2002). Investigations with serotonin transporter knockout mice suggest that at least some of these changes in gene transcription are related to serotonin release. A recent publication found that repeated administration with 1 or 5 mg/kg weekly for four weeks increased transcripts for 5HT1B receptors in various brain regions and 5HT2C receptors in the cortex and hypothalamus after MDMA (Kindlundh-Hogberg et al. 2006). However, because transcription was assessed ten hours after the last MDMA administration, it is not clear whether these changes reflect residual acute effects of MDMA or changes related to repeated administration. However, generally speaking it appears that serotonin plays more of a significant role than dopamine in changing gene transcription. Future studies will need to separate direct and indirect effects of MDMA on gene expression or transcription. Physiological Effects Moderate to high doses of MDMA elevate body temperature in rodents, often producing hyperthermia (Cole and Sumnall 2003b), and doses of MDMA closer to those used by humans still produce at least a slight increase in body temperature (Reveron et al. 2006), suggesting that this effect may occur only at higher doses. Ambient temperature and MDMA interact on body temperature in rodents, with high ambient temperature significantly increasing body temperature in mice and rats, and low ambient temperature and MDMA producing hypothermia rather than hyperthermia (Dafters 1994; Fantegrossi et al. 2003; Malberg and Seiden 1998). High doses of MDMA produce significant elevations in body temperature in primates (Bowyer et al. 2003; Mechan et al. 2006), but when they receive doses closer to those humans ingest, monkeys exhibit only slight to moderate elevation in body temperature Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 11 (Crean et al. 2007; Crean et al. 2006). In contrast to findings in rodents, human and nonhuman primates are not subject to interactions between ambient temperature and MDMA, exhibiting slight to moderate increases in body temperature in cool, room temperature and warm environments (Crean et al. 2007; Crean et al. 2006; Freedman et al. 2005), though one paper examining monkeys in restraining chairs detected an interaction between MDMA and ambient temperature on monkey body temperature (Banks et al. 2007). The difference between rodent and primate response to MDMA in different ambient temperatures may result from interspecies differences in means of body heat reduction, including differences relating to body mass (Gordon 2007). While there are few in vivo assessments of cardiovascular effects of MDMA in nonhuman animals, Cole and Sumnall noted in their review that previous findings detected increased sympathetic activity, as seen in humans (Cole and Sumnall 2003b). A team of researchers based in Dublin, Ireland conducted a number of in vitro studies with rat vascular tissue demonstrating that MDMA has both pressor and depressor effects, acting through adrenergic receptors. They reported elevations in blood pressure (Bexis and Docherty 2005; 2006). A large body of research has examined the neurotoxic potential of MDMA, with the general consensus being that MDMA may be neurotoxic to the axons of serotonergic cells at high or repeated doses (Baumann et al. 2007; Cole and Sumnall 2003b). Studies in rodents and nonhuman primates have found that high and repeated doses of MDMA reduce brain serotonin levels and may damage the axons of serotonergic neurons (Cole and Sumnall 2003b). However, lower doses do not appear to do this (Mechan et al. 2006; Wang et al. 2005), and it is not likely that the dosages and regimens found in controlled studies produce these effects. The issue of MDMA neurotoxicity is addressed further under “Possible Risks” below. Studies in mice found increased limbic excitability after repeated moderate or high doses of MDMA and increased activity after each dose without reducing brain serotonin or dopamine levels (Frenzilli et al. 2007; Giorgi et al. 2005). The significance of this finding for humans remains unclear, as animals received daily doses of MDMA. However, such changes might be related to increased tolerance to MDMA effects after frequent, repeated doses. Humans Onset of MDMA effects occurs 30 to 60 minutes after administration (Cami et al. 2000; Mas et al. 1999), peak effects appear 75 to 120 minutes post-drug (Liechti et al. 2001b; Tancer and Johanson 2003), and duration of effects lasts from three to six hours (Harris et al. 2002; Liechti et al. 2001a; Vollenweider et al. 1998a), with most effects returning to baseline or near-baseline levels six hours after drug administration. MDMA produces sympathomimetic effects that include elevation in blood pressure and heart rate, first recorded by Downing (Downing 1986) and replicated by other research teams in the US and Europe (Lester et al. 2000; Liechti et al. 2001a; Mas et al. 1999; Tancer and Johanson 2001). Elevation in blood pressure met diagnosis for hypertension in approximately 5% of research participants receiving at least 100 mg MDMA in research Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 12 studies (Mas et al. 1999; Vollenweider et al. 1998a), but none of these individuals needed clinical intervention and blood pressure returned to normal as drug effects waned. Most people do not experience elevations that are greater than seen after moderate exercise. Cardiovascular effects of MDMA first appear 30 to 45 minutes after administration (Downing 1986) and peak between 1 and 2 hours post-drug, with effects waning 3 to 5 hours after drug administration. Men given the same mg/kg dose of MDMA as women exhibited a significantly greater elevation in blood pressure, and they also exhibited a greater elevation in heart rate than women, as reported in a study summarizing and pooling data from a series of human MDMA studies (Liechti et al. 2001a). These studies did not report any discomfort or increased stress accompanying cardiovascular effects. As previously noted, MDMA produces only a slight elevation in body temperature (Liechti et al. 2001a) and this elevation is unaffected by ambient temperature (Freedman et al. 2005). Doses between 1.5 and 2 mg/kg MDMA (approximately 100-150 mg) fail to produce any clinically significant elevation in body temperature (Freedman et al. 2005; Liechti et al. 2001b). Men seem to exhibit a greater elevation in body temperature than women when given the dose of MDMA in milligrams per kilogram (Liechti et al. 2001a). However, it is notable that participants in controlled studies have not engaged in vigorous exercise and either remained sitting or lying down throughout most drug effects. It may be the case that ambient temperature and vigorous exercise contribute to the occurrence of hyperthermia in people ingesting ecstasy in uncontrolled settings. One of two naturalistic studies reported that ecstasy users had a slight but not statistically significant increase in body temperature, while the other failed to find any significant differences in ecstasy-user body temperature at a club (Cole et al. 2005; Irvine et al. 2006). In the study of Irvine and colleagues, average blood MDMA was 0.31 +/- 0.21 ng/L, though five participants had significantly higher blood MDMA levels. MDMA and Brain Activity Gamma and colleagues performed positron emission tomography (PET) brain scans 75 minutes after administering 1.7 mg/kg MDMA, finding increased regional blood flow (rCBF) in prefrontal, inferior temporal and cerebellar areas and decreased rCBF in the left amygdala (Gamma et al. 2000). Decreased amygdalar activity may be indicative of reduced reactivity to potential threats (Phelps et al. 2001). Ecstasy user participants receiving two doses of MDMA exhibited decreases in bilateral visual cortex, caudate, superior parietal, and dorsolateral frontal regions (Chang et al. 2000) ten to 21 days later, with increased rCBF measured in two participants at a later time point. However, a comparison between heavy ecstasy users and non-user controls failed to find differences in rCBF (Gamma et al. 2001), and a report assessing changes before and after initial use found only increase in one area of prefrontal cortex (de Win et al. 2007), suggesting that the changes seen by Chang and colleagues are a transient effect. Electroencephalography recorded after MDMA showed the following changes EEG activity; overall increase in beta activity, reduction in alpha activity, and specific decreases in alpha and delta in frontal areas and increased frontotemporal beta signal (Frei et al. 2001), EEG patterns the authors reported as being similar to those seen with serotonergic and noradrenergic drugs, and to a lesser extent, dopaminergic drugs. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 13 Neuroendocrine Effects of MDMA MDMA dose-dependently and acutely increases cortisol, prolactin, and adrenocorticotropic hormone concentrations (Farre et al. 2004; Grob 2001; Grob et al. 1996; Harris et al. 2002; Mas et al. 1999), while growth hormone was unchanged by up to 125 mg MDMA (Mas et al. 1999). Increases in cortisol and prolactin peaked at about 2 hours after MDMA administration. A second dose of 100 mg MDMA given four hours after an initial 100 mg produced a second increase in cortisol during an interval when cortisol levels were declining (Pacifici et al. 2001), and a dose of 100 mg MDMA given 24 hours after an initial dose stimulated a greater release of cortisol, but not prolactin (Farre et al. 2004). In a study of the effects of 0.5 and 1.5 mg/kg MDMA in eight people, there was a trend for increased levels of the hormone dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) after 0.5 mg/kg MDMA, and a significant increase after 1.5 mg/kg MDMA, with peak levels appearing 2 to 3 hours post-drug (Harris et al. 2002). Along with these findings from controlled studies, a naturalistic study reported elevated levels of the hormone oxytocin in clubgoers with detectable blood MDMA levels when compared with clubgoers without any detectable levels of MDMA. It is likely that all neuroendocrine changes result from monoamine release, and it is currently unknown what role, if any, they play in producing the effects of MDMA. Exogenous oxytocin increases trust and improves accuracy of emotion perception, and increased cortisol in some circumstances may serve as a signal to seek affiliation or to increase positive mood (Bartz and Hollander 2006; Domes et al. 2007; Kirsch et al. 2005; Wirth and Schultheiss 2006). Immunological Effects of MDMA Humans exhibit transient immunological changes after a dose of 100 mg (Pacifici et al. 2004; Pacifici et al. 2000; Pacifici et al. 1999; Pacifici et al. 2002), including reduced numbers of CD4 cells, increased numbers of NK cells, and an increase in levels of immunosuppressive and anti-inflammatory cytokines compared with levels of proinflammatory and immunostimulating cytokines. Pacifici and colleagues report that in several respects, these effects are similar to those that occur with other psychoactive substances and are not unique to MDMA. Immunological effects last for approximately 24 hours after administration, and most arise indirectly from serotonin release (Pacifici et al. 2004). The significance of these immunological effects remains unclear. However, we expect the impact of these effects to be modest in most cases. Behavioral and psychological effects Researchers investigating how MDMA produces its behavioral and psychological effects on humans and nonhuman animals are only just beginning to approach this topic, and the first controlled studies into the therapeutic potential of this compound are only just reaching completion now. In rodents, doses of MDMA equivalent or only slightly greater in size than human doses produce little to no behavioral effects. However, doses of 5 mg/kg or greater possess several specific behavioral effects, including increased locomotor activity, increased anxiety at moderately high doses and decreased anxiety at higher doses (Cole and Sumnall 2003b; Green et al. 2003). Rats given lower doses of MDMA exhibited increased anxiety in the elevated plus maze (Ho et al. 2004), while higher doses exhibited reduced anxiety on the plus maze. Rats given higher doses also reduced aggressive behavior, but they also reduced social investigation. Rodents Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 14 responded to high doses of MDMA by exhibiting flat body posture, forepaw treading and an erect tail (“Straub tail”) (Green et al. 2003), all signs of rodent “serotonin syndrome.” MDMA produces some repetitive behavior, but not to the same degree as do psychostimulants. MDMA increased locomotor activity in rats, leading them to walk around a cage perimeter, interpreted as an indicator of thigmotaxis, a sign of anxiety (Cole and Sumnall 2003b). However, it is notable that a recent in-press publication failed to find thigmotaxis in rats given 5 mg/kg MDMA (Selken and Nichols 2007). By contrast, rhesus monkeys do not exhibit increased locomotor activity after up to 2.4 mg/kg MDMA (Crean et al. 2006). To date, no one has performed empirical investigations of the effects of MDMA on social interaction in human or nonhuman primates. Morley and colleagues observed rat behavior after 5 mg/kg MDMA, noting that this dose increased likelihood of prosocial behavior, such as lying next to each other (Morley et al. 2005). Recent studies conducted by the same team of researchers suggest that MDMA increases prosocial behavior in rats by elevating oxytocin in the paraventricular nucleus through 5HT1A receptor agonism (Thompson et al. 2007), with the oxytocin increase arising from indirect effects of MDMA on 5HT1A receptors. To date, there have been no human pharmacological challenge studies combining MDMA with 5HT1A agonists or antagonists, and it is unclear whether the rat behavior is analogous to human reports of increased feelings of empathy or interpersonal closeness while under the influence of MDMA (Liester et al. 1992; Peroutka et al. 1988; Solowij et al. 1992; Vollenweider et al. 1998). Humans MDMA alters mood, perception and cognition. These effects peak 90 to 120 minutes after oral administration and they are near to or at pre-drug levels three to six hours later (Lamers et al. 2003; Tancer and Johanson 2001; Vollenweider et al. 1998a). Sub-acute effects may occur one to three days after drug administration, but are no longer apparent seven to 14 days later (Harris et al. 2002; see also Huxster et al. 2006). Most of the therapeutic effects of MDMA result from changes in affect, cognition and social interaction. When combined with psychotherapy that supports one or more of these effects, MDMA permits people to confront and consider emotionally intense memories, thoughts or feelings, and perhaps through changes in mood and perception, increases empathy and compassion for others and the self. MDMA may also increase accuracy of assessing at least some emotional expressions. Active doses of MDMA alter mood and cognition and produce slight alterations in perception (Dumont and Verkes 2006; Liechti et al. 2001a). Changes in mood include increased positive mood and anxiety (Cami et al. 2000; Harris et al. 2002; Liechti and Vollenweider 2001; Tancer and Johanson 2003). People reported feeling more talkative and friendly after receiving MDMA, and at least one research team informally reported increased feelings of closeness to others (Vollenweider et al. 1998). Researchers using two items within an instrument designed to assess drug effects and a visual analog scale rating closeness to others failed to detect increased feelings of empathy after 1.5 mg/kg MDMA (Harris et al. 2002), possibly due to the low sensitivity of this measure. However, Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 15 a recent investigation into the effects of pretreatment with the SSRI paroxetine on MDMA effects in humans reported MDMA increased feelings of sociality and closeness to others, and that the SSRI paroxetine reduced these effects (Farre et al. 2007). People reported feeling anxious and undergoing negatively experienced derealization, including increased anxiety in relation to loss of control, and experiences of racing or blocked thoughts after MDMA (Cami et al. 2000; Liechti et al. 2001; Vollenweider et al. 1998). Study participants experienced slight changes in visual or auditory perception, including changes in the brightness of the room or colors, sounds seeming closer or farther away, and simple visual distortions. Participants also experienced altered time perception and changed meaning or significance of perceptions after MDMA (Vollenweider et al. 1998). People maintained insight as to their experience, and there was little indication that MDMA produced any strong alterations to the sense of self or control over the experience (Harris et al. 2002; Tancer and Johanson 2003). Women reported experiencing all subjective effects of MDMA more intensely, but especially those related to perceptual changes (Liechti et al. 2001a). Though some researchers had hypothesized that MDMA would produce more positive or rewarding effects when taken in a warm environment (Parrott 2004), researchers comparing the effects of MDMA in a warm and a cool room failed to support this hypothesis (Freedman et al. 2005). People receiving active doses of MDMA experienced euphoria, positive mood, vigor and positively experienced derealization, consonant with early retrospective reports, and they also experienced anxiety, tension and dysphoria, as concern over losing control over the self (Cami et al. 2000; Harris et al. 2002; Liechti and Vollenweider 2001; Tancer and Johanson 2003). It is uncertain whether the increases in positive and negative mood occur simultaneously or occur at different times throughout the duration of MDMA effects, though there is some suggestion in reports from two different teams that peaks in negative mood may precede peaks for positive mood (Liechti and Vollenweider 2000a; Tancer and Johanson 2003). MDMA produces modest acute changes in attention and cognition, with previous reports indicating impaired performance on some tasks, as digit-symbol substitution, and not others, as the Stroop task (Cami et al. 2000; Gamma et al. 2000; Vollenweider et al. 1998b). Larger doses of MDMA attenuated prepulse inhibition in rats, defined as a reduced startle response to an intense stimulus when it is preceded by a less intense cue. However, in humans, MDMA enhances PPI (Vollenweider et al. 1999). A recent series of studies conducted in the Netherlands that examined the effects of MDMA on skills needed for driving cars reported transient and selective changes in verbal and visual attention and memory after 75 or 100 mg MDMA (Kuypers and Ramaekers 2005; 2007; Ramaekers and Kuypers 2006). Changes included difficulty learning or remembering lists of words and difficulty recalling object position within an array of objects, but without any impairment in spotting scene changes, reduced weaving in a driving simulation, but excessively cautious response to the actions of another car in an assessment of actual driving. MDMA acutely improved performance on one measure of impulsivity while failing to improve or impair performance on other impulsivity measures (Ramaekers and Kuypers 2006). The cause or causes behind these changes are unclear but may relate to changes in attention, salience of visual objects and altered time Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 16 perception. Changes in visuospatial recall and driving skills are likely associated with serotonin release or indirect action on serotonin receptors, as the noradrenergic and dopaminergic drug methylphenidate (Ritalin) did not produce similar changes. These changes in cognitive function and psychomotor skills occurred during peak drug effects but were not detectable 24 hours later. Retrospective surveys of people who had used MDMA or ecstasy offered similar accounts of MDMA effects to those observed and reported in controlled studies. These studies surveyed or interviewed members of several populations, including college students, psychotherapists, and individuals recruited via word of mouth or in public spaces. Study respondents reported experiencing stimulant-like effects, as greater energy or talkativeness, and hallucinogen-like effects, as perceptual changes or poor concentration. They also reported that MDMA/ecstasy increased feelings of closeness, compassion or empathy toward the self or others (Cohen 1995; Liester et al. 1992; Peroutka et al. 1988; Solowij et al. 1992). The disparity in detection of entactogenic findings in retrospective versus controlled studies is largely due to failure to measure these effects, but might also relate to aspects of setting in controlled studies that do not permit enough unstructured interpersonal contact to produce or facilitate feelings of interpersonal closeness. 3. Pharmacokinetics and biological disposition MDMA is metabolized in the liver and has a half-life of seven to nine hours (de la Torre et al. 2004), though a half-life of 11 hours has been reported (Pizarro et al. 2004) and is distributed throughout the body (De Letter et al. 2004), though a study in rats reported greater disposition in brain than in plasma (Chu et al. 1996). After 100 mg MDMA, Tmax is reached at 2 hours, at a time close to peak physiological and subjective effects, and urinary recovery over a 24 hour period is 15% (de la Torre et al. 2004). The pharmacokinetics of MDMA have been primarily characterized by a group of Spanish researchers, with the exception of one publication from researchers in the Netherlands. The Spanish team first reported nonlinear pharmacokinetics for MDMA, findings that are confirmed in recent studies in nonhuman primates (Mechan et al. 2006). MDMA is metabolized by several CYPD enzymes, including but not limited to CYP2D6, CYP1A2 and CYP3A4. Monoamine oxidase and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) also metabolize MDMA. The pharmacokinetics of MDMA in humans have been characterized in blood and urine samples using oral doses of up to 150 mg MDMA. Metabolites of MDMA which have been identified in humans include 3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine (MDA), 4-hydroxy- 3-methoxy-methamphetamine (HMMA), 4-hydroxy-3-methoxyamphetamine (HMA), 3,4-dihydroxyamphetamine (DHA, also called α-methyldopamine), 3,4dihydroxymethamphetamine (DHMA, also called HHMA), 3,4methylenedioxyphenylacetone, and N-hydroxy-3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine (de Boer et al. 1997; Helmlin et al. 1996; Helmlin and Brenneisen 1992; Lanz et al. 1997; Ortuno et al. 1999). Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 17 Metabolites are primarily excreted as glucuronide and sulfate conjugates (Helmlin et al. 1996). Subsequent studies examining metabolism after 100 mg MDMA reported excretion values similar to those reported by de la Torre and associates (Farre et al. 2004; Pizarro et al. 2004; Pizarro et al. 2003; Pizarro et al. 2002; Segura et al. 2001). Urinary excretion of the MDMA metabolite HHMA after 100 mg MDMA in four men was 91.8 ± 23.8 mol and 17.7% recovery (Segura et al. 2001). By contrast, urinary recovery of the major metabolite HMMA after 100 mg was 40% (de la Torre et al. 2004). As was the case for maximal plasma values, urinary recoveries for MDMA and MDA were higher after a second dose of 100 mg MDMA than after an initial dose of 100 mg MDMA (Farre et al. 2004). Figure 1: Metabolism of MDMA in rats and humans; enzymes involved in human metabolism in red ink. Reproduced with permission of R de la Torre. 4. Safety and effectiveness in humans obtained from prior clinical studies When Merck first patented MDMA, it was solely as an intermediate step toward the production of another compound (Freudenmann et al. 2006), and there were no early clinical investigations of MDMA. Published accounts of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy first appeared during the time of hearings for the scheduling of MDMA (Adamson 1985). Shortly afterwards, the only published study of MDMA-assisted therapy appeared, an uncontrolled study conducted in 29 individuals with mild to moderate psychiatric problems (Greer and Tolbert 1986). These accounts suggested that, when combined with psychotherapy in a supportive setting, MDMA offered benefits to people experiencing various forms of anxiety disorder, including PTSD and anxiety in association with a lifethreatening illness. The Swiss government permitted psychotherapists to conduct MDMA-assisted psychotherapy between 1988 and 1993 (Gasser 1994; Widmer 1998). These therapists reported that MDMA-assisted psychotherapy was tolerated and did not report any serious adverse events occurring after MDMA administration. The Swiss Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 18 psychotherapists did not publish any formal analyses of the treatment. Permission to conduct MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in Switzerland was revoked due to events unrelated to the safety or efficacy of MDMA and due to the lack of any published research results. Narrative accounts report that individuals experienced less anxiety and sometimes reported feelings of reconciliation with the self or others or greater positive attitudes after MDMA-assisted psychotherapy (Greer and Tolbert 1998; Metzner and Adamson 2001). A majority of the participants in the uncontrolled study of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy followed two months to two years later reported experiencing increased positive mood and more positive attitude changes since undergoing MDMA-assisted therapy (Greer and Tolbert 1986). To date, there are four investigations underway to study the safety and efficacy of MDMA-related psychotherapy in people with PTSD and in people with anxiety arising from diagnosis with advanced-stage cancer (Halpern 2006). Three planned or ongoing investigations to date are sponsored by the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies (MAPS), a member-based nonprofit organization dedicated to supporting and developing research into the therapeutic potential of MDMA and psychedelic drugs. The fourth is sponsored by John Halpern MD. MDMA-assisted psychotherapy for PTSD Two studies of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in people with PTSD are underway, one in South Carolina and the other in Switzerland. A third study is nearly underway in Israel. None of these studies are complete as of yet. The first study has enrolled all or nearly all study participants, and the second has enrolled approximately half of the planed participants. Michael Mithoefer MD has enrolled nineteen participants in a randomized, placebocontrolled study of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in 20 people with PTSD symptoms that have not responded to psychotherapy or pharmacotherapy, and a 21st subject who will be a veteran with PTSD who has not being willing or able to receive these treatments. He has enrolled all participants, but to date only 16 participants have completed the study. The study includes an open-label continuation for participants who received placebo during the course of the study. MDMA-assisted psychotherapy is conducted during two extended sessions as part of a course of psychotherapy (Mithoefer 2006). Participants who received MDMA during the course of the study are eligible for a third open-label session as well. An independent rater assesses psychotherapy symptoms at the start of the study and two months after participants have had their second MDMAassisted therapy session. The original study design involved the administration of 125 mg MDMA, and the current design allows for a supplemental administration of 62.5 mg approximately 2.5 hours after the initial dose. MDMA has been tolerated in participants, and there have been no drug-related serious adverse events to date. An informal analysis of preliminary data suggests that participants assigned to receive MDMA experience fewer PTSD symptoms two months after the second MDMA or placebo-assisted session. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 19 There is no indication that receiving MDMA is associated with any changes in cognitive function. Peter Oehen MD, the principal investigator for the study underway in Switzerland, is assessing the effects of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in twelve people with PTSD. So far, he has enrolled up to seven participants in this study, and five have undergone at least one MDMA-assisted psychotherapy session. This study is nearly identical to the investigation underway in the US except that it is active placebo controlled, with 25 mg MDMA as the active placebo and a booster dose of 62.5 or 12.5mg respectively after 2.5 hours, and participants undergo three instead of two MDMA-assisted psychotherapy sessions. One individual dropped out of the study owing to distress during the first of three MDMA-assisted sessions, the previous pharmacological treatment was then reinstalled immediately. A study of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in twelve people with war or terrorism-related PTSD is recruiting participants and will take place in Israel sometime in early 2008. The study will follow procedures similar to those described in the US and Swiss studies. To date, no one has undergone an MDMA-assisted session in this study. MDMA for anxiety arising from diagnosis with a potentially life-threatening illness A randomized, active placebo controlled dose-response study of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in twelve people with anxiety arising from diagnosis with advanced stage cancer is underway at McLean Hospital, affiliated with Harvard Medical School. John Halpern, MD is currently recruiting participants for this study involving two MDMAassisted sessions, and will include introductory sessions beforehand and integrative sessions after each MDMA-assisted session. This study also employs 25 mg MDMA followed by 12.5 mg, administered during both occasions, as an active placebo. Participants in the active dose condition will receive 83.3 mg and 41.7 mg during their first MDMA-assisted session, producing a cumulative dose of 125 mg, and 125 mg followed by 62.5 mg during the second session. No participants are enrolled in this study to date. MDMA and Parkinson’s disease Studies in rodents and nonhuman primates have examined the effects of MDMA and related compounds in alleviating the symptoms of Parkinson’s disease (PD), which include slow, halting movements, or in treating dyskinesia, involuntary movements and twitches that can arise from using medications to treat the condition (Iravani et al. 2003; Lebsanft et al. 2005a; Lebsanft et al. 2005b; Sotnikova et al. 2005). Researchers found that MDMA and other entactogens, such as MDE, could attenuate abnormally slow movements in rodents without functioning dopamine systems (Sotnikova et al. 2005), and that monkeys with dopamine systems damaged by MPTP also improved after treatment with MDMA (Iravani et al. 2003). While this program of research is likely inspired by the account of a former stuntman with young-onset PD, there are no human trials of MDMA or any other entactogenic compound as a possible treatment for PD or PDrelated dyskinesias underway. Because of the sympathomimetic effects of MDMA and the potential of MDMA neurotoxicity with daily dosing, it seems unlikely that MDMA Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 20 will be developed as a medication for PD. It appears that the anti-Parkinsonian effects of MDMA may be due at least in part to indirect activation of 5HT1A receptors, and perhaps to activation of trace amine receptors (Bishop et al. 2006; Sotnikova et al. 2005). 5. Possible Risks and Side Effects Overview MDMA was administered to perhaps thousands of people prior to scheduling, and as of late 2007, it has been administered to approximately 390 people in uncontrolled and controlled studies. People continue to use ecstasy around the world in various nonmedical settings (Beck and Rosenbaum 1994; Carlson et al. 2005; Cole and Sumnall 2003a; Solowij et al. 1992; Sumnall et al. 2006), including dance events, large gatherings, concerts and small parties. While a number of serious adverse events, including fatalities, have been reported after ecstasy use in unsupervised and uncontrolled settings, these are events are relatively rare given the prevalence of ecstasy use (Baggott 2002; Gore 1999). Drug-related serious adverse events have not occurred in any of the human MDMA research studies so far. In 2005, the number of MDMA/ecstasy related emergency room visits logged into the Drug Abuse Warning Network (DAWN) was 10,752 of approximately 1,449,154 ED visits related to any form of drug, approximately 0.007% or calculated to be 3.6 per population of 100,000 (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration 2007). Operating on national survey data on drug use and emergency department admissions in the US and a study of Australian polydrug users, Baggott and colleagues estimated that between 2.9 and 11 emergency department visits might arise from 10,000 uses of ecstasy (Baggott et al. 2001, pp. 148-150). Fatalities Fatalities have occurred after the use of MDMA or related drugs in non-medical settings (Baggott et al. 2001; Henry and Rella 2001). Ecstasy-related fatalities are rare (Baggott 2002; Gore 1999). Most are related to hyperthermia and complications arising from hyperthermia. Other causes of death include hyponatremia and cardiac events (as arrhythmias or heart attack). Some ecstasy-related fatalities may be due to reckless behavior, such as driving under the influence of ecstasy. Baggott and colleagues found that men outnumbered women in most ecstasy-related fatalities except in the case of hyponatremia, where women outnumbered men (Baggott et al. 2001). The association between MDMA/ecstasy and fatalities is generally dose-dependent, except in the case of hyponatremia-related fatalities (see for example Greene et al. 2003). At least half the ecstasy-related fatalities listed seem to involve use of other drugs (Gilhooly and Daly 2002; Raikos et al. 2002; Schifano et al. 2003). Common Adverse Effects and Side Effects Common adverse and side effects of MDMA include elevation in blood pressure and heart rate, increased anxiety or dysphoria, and dilated pupils (Cami et al. 2000; Harris et al. 2002; Liechti et al. 2001b; Mas et al. 1999). Some reports indicated decreased rather than increased alertness (Cami et al. 2000). Other common side effects reported in controlled studies of MDMA are listed in Table 2 and include reduced appetite, dizziness, tight jaw or bruxism (tooth-grinding), difficulty concentrating, impaired gait or balance, Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 21 dry mouth, and thirst. Participants in some studies also reported or exhibited changes in cognition, such as increases in speed of thought or thought blocking, facilitated imagination or facilitated recall (Vollenweider et al. 1998), and unusual thoughts or ideas (Harris et al. 2002). Other less common side effects include parasthesias (unusual body sensations) such as tingling sensations, or feeling hot or cold. These effects are transient and recede with the waning of drug effects. One study found that women were more likely than men to experience most commonly reported side effects of MDMA, though men were more likely than women to experience the specific side effects of nausea and sweating (Liechti et al. 2001b). Sub-acute effects appearing 24 to 48 hours (1 to 2 days) after MDMA include insomnia, fatigue, weakness, heavy legs, dry mouth, low mood or irritability (Baggott et al. 2001; see also Liechti et al. 2001a), with sub-acute effects waning by or within 72 hours of MDMA administration. While ecstasy users in naturalistic studies reported increased feelings of depression or aggressiveness four days after taking ecstasy (Hoshi et al. 2007a; Verheyden et al. 2003), far fewer participants in controlled studies report moodrelated sub-acute effects. Naturalistic studies examining the time course of sub-acute effects of ecstasy use have reported that a similar trajectory for side effects, with subacute effects most apparent three to four days later and no longer apparent seven days later (Hoshi et al. 2004; Huxster et al. 2006). The possibility of long-term effects is discussed in more detail below. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 22 Reproduced from Baggott et al. 2001 Table 3: Acute Side Effects of MDMA Overall Incidence After Placebo Overall Incidence After MDMA Downing 1986 Gamma et al. 2000 Greer & Tolbert 1986 Liechti, Saur, et al. 2000 Liechti & Vollenweider 2000a Liechti & Vollenweider 2000b Vollenweider et al. 1998 N: 13-57 13-112 10 16 29 14 14 16 13 MDMA Dose(s): 0 0.5-4.18 mg/kg 1.76- 4.18 mg/kg 1.7 mg/kg 75-150, 200 mg 1.5 mg/kg 1.5 mg/kg 1.5 mg/kg 1.7 mg/kg Measurement Time: - - 2-5 h ? ? ? ? ? 0-360 min Lack Of Appetite 2% 70% 100% 63% 97% 50% 50% 50% 62% Jaw Clenching 0% 63% 60% 64% 76% 57% 71% 44% 62% Dry Mouth Na 57% na na Na 57% 57% na na Thirst 4% 48% na 50% Na 57% 57% 38% 38% Restless Legs 0% 45% na na Na na na 44% 46% Impaired Balance/Gait 0% 44% 70% na 10% 71% 43% 50% 62% Difficulty Concentrating 16% 42% 30% 50% 3% 71% 50% 63% 62% Dizziness 2% 40% na na Na 57% 21% 50% 31% Restlessness 0% 39% na na Na 50% 29% 44% 31% Sensitivity To Cold 7% 38% na na Na na na na 38% Private Worries 23% 38% na na Na na na na 38% Heavy Legs 0% 38% na na Na na na na 38% Palpitations 0% 33% na 38% Na 43% 21% na 31% Feeling Cold 4% 33% na na Na 43% na na 23% Perspiration 0% 30% na 50% Na 36% na na 0% Drowsiness 50% 23% na na 14% 43% na na na Nystagmus Na 23% 80% na 3% na na na na Hot Flashes 0% 23% na na Na na na na 23% Nausea 4% 21% 10% na 24% 36% na na 8% Trismus Na 21% na na 3% 57% na na na Inner Tension 0% 17% na na 3% 43% 14% 19% 23% Insomnia 0% 17% 0% na Na na na na 31% Anxiety 0% 16% na na 17% 14% na na na Weakness 0% 16% na na 3% 36% na na 23% Urge To Urinate 8% 15% na na Na na na na 15% Tremor 0% 14% na na 3% 21% 14% na 31% Muscle Aches / Tension Na 14% na na 21% na na na 0% Forgetfulness 0% 14% na na 3% na na na 38% Fatigue 26% 13% na na 7% na 29% na 8% Parasthesias 0% 12% na na 3% na na na 31% Lack Of Energy 14% 12% na na 3% 29% na na na Brooding 0% 12% na na 3% 29% na na na Fainting Na 3% na na 3% na na na na Blurred Vision Na 3% na na 3% na na na na Lip Swelling Na 2% na na 3% na na na 0% Headaches Na 2% 0% na 3% na 0% na na na: not available Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 23 Medical Emergencies and Adverse Events in Ecstasy Users An examination of the literature published up through early 2001 located over 205 published case reports or case series concerning adverse events after ecstasy use. The most frequently reported events were hyperthermia (25.1% of 199 case reports), psychological symptoms or psychosis (22.1% of 199), hepatotoxicity, or liver conditions and problems (16.1% of 199 cases), and hyponatremia (9.5%) accounted for the majority of the serious adverse events after ecstasy use (Baggott et al. 2001). A second examination of the literature in 2004 found that these continued to be the most frequently reported problems reported in literature assessed after the initial examination (Jerome 2004), with only two new conditions reported in the literature, gingivitis from maintaining an ecstasy tablet between the lips and gum (Brazier et al. 2003), and chorioretinopathy, an eye condition sometimes associated with use of sympathomimetic drugs that cleared up after cessation of use (Michael et al. 2003). Set and setting may play a role in the development of some ecstasy-related adverse events, such as rigorous exercise, lack of attention to somatic cues and too little or too much hydration in the case of hyperthermia and hyponatremia (Henry and Rella 2001). Hall and Henry address medical emergencies related to ecstasy use, describing all events mentioned in Baggott and colleagues (Hall and Henry 2006). While case reports do not provide an appropriate basis for estimating the relative frequency of these events, they can provide information on the possibility of an event occurring. Most ecstasy-related emergency department admissions are the result of people experiencing anxiety or panic reactions after use and involve supportive care only (Cregg and Tracey 1993; Liechti et al. 2005; Williams et al. 1998). It is notable that none of these events has occurred during a human MDMA study, so that even psychological distress has not required pharmacological intervention or hospitalization. Other serious adverse events occurring after ecstasy use include cardiac problems (as arrhythmias), cerebrovascular events, hematological, respiratory events (as pneumostadium), dermatological, ophthalmological and dental events (Baggott et al. 2001). As with the four most common serious adverse events, none of these events have occurred in the context of human MDMA research. In vitro and in vivo investigations of the effects of MDMA on cardiac, hepatic (liver) and kidney tissues or cells have occurred over the past ten years (Baggott et al. 2001; Jerome 2005) (see for example Beitia et al. 2000; Caballero et al. 2002) (Varela-Rey et al. 1999). Researchers conducting in vitro studies often use large doses (in micromoles) that are unlikely to occur after a typical human dose. At these high or extremely high doses, MDMA damages liver cells, particularly under warm ambient temperature, possibly mimicking hyperthermia-related hepatotoxicity in humans. In one case series of postmortem heart tissue, Patel and colleagues determined that 58.3% of the hearts from ecstasy-associated deaths were larger than normal versus 18.7% of the hearts from nonecstasy related deaths (Patel et al. 2005). Since myocardial hypertrophy (an enlarged heart) is associated with stimulant use, and given the large extent of polydrug use in ecstasy users, this study cannot rule out the possibility that the increase they saw was not a result of psychostimulant use. A recent retrospective comparative study using echocardiograms in 29 heavy ecstasy users (reporting use of 3.6 tablets per week) and 29 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 24 age and gender matched undergraduate controls detected abnormalities indicative of potential valvular heart disease (VHD) in eight ecstasy users and none in controls, though less pronounced than abnormalities seen in people taking the anti-Parkinson’s disease medication pergolide (Droogmans et al. 2007). The average cumulative dose in people with detectable abnormalities was 943 +/- 1162 tablets versus 242 +/-212 tablets in those without abnormalities). The authors hypothesize that the observed cardiac changes in ecstasy users were less prominent than in people taking pergolide because of weekly rather than daily use, and because drug-induced VHD is reversible. Given the extensive and frequent use in this sample, the risk of similar cardiac abnormalities developing in people taking part in human MDMA studies is very low. However, these findings also suggest that regular use of ecstasy may have some of the same risks as regular use of other 5HT2B agonists, as some migraine medications and the appetite suppressant fenfluramine. Long-Term Effects Central Serotonin Function, Cognition and Affect: Retrospective Studies There is a wealth of research examining the effects of repeated doses of MDMA in nonhuman animals (Cole and Sumnall 2003b; Green et al. 2003). Findings included reduction in brain serotonin, signs of impaired transport of serotonin and some behavioral changes, as increased anxiety (Callahan et al. 2001; Gurtman et al. 2002; Hatzidimitriou et al. 1999). These findings suggested that MDMA could damage serotonin axons, producing a form of neurotoxicity. However, as noted earlier, the majority of these studies employed large doses of MDMA that overestimated human-equivalent doses. It now appears that lower doses of MDMA fail to reduce brain serotonin. Monkeys allowed to self-administer MDMA for an 18-month period had no reductions in brain dopamine, slight reductions in brain serotonin and no chemical markers of neuronal injury (Fantegrossi et al. 2004), and rats receiving lower doses of MDMA do not exhibit signs of neurotoxicity, such as changes in serotonin transporter sites or markers of neuronal injury (Wang et al. 2005). While a recent report detected increases in one marker of neuronal injury without detecting any decreases in brain serotonin after administering two human-equivalent doses of MDMA to rhesus monkeys for two days (Meyer et al. 2006). However, studies in very moderate ecstasy users failed to see an increase in this marker (de Win et al. 2007), and only one of three studies of this marker in humans detected it in heavy users (Chang et al. 1999; Cowan et al. 2007; Reneman et al. 2002). Changes in Serotonin Function and Indicators of Neuronal Injury or Repair Spurred on by nonhuman animal studies that found that repeated or high doses of MDMA damaged the axons of serotonin neurons, researchers began studying the effects of repeated non-medical or recreational use of ecstasy in humans (see for example Krystal et al. 1992; McCann et al. 1999; McCann et al. 1994; Semple et al. 1999). These studies detected differences in mood and cognition in ecstasy users, and possible changes in brain serotonin uptake sites. Researchers assumed that if MDMA reduced serotonin function, it should produce observable effects, as reduced brain serotonin uptake sites or changes in mood or psychological well-being. These early investigations possessed a number of methodological flaws, including retrospective design and poor matching of Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 25 ecstasy users with appropriate controls (Baggott et al. 2001; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank and Daumann 2006a; b). Later studies sought to remedy some of these problems by using carefully matched polydrug user or cannabis user controls, or by relying on a sample with relatively low exposure to psychoactives, including alcohol (Buchert et al. 2004; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al. 2003; Halpern et al. 2004; Thomasius et al. 2003). Some of these investigators also conducted longitudinal studies, comparing ecstasy users, sometimes alongside controls, at two separate time points (Daumann et al. 2004b; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al. 2005); (Buchert et al. 2006; Zakzanis and Campbell 2006; Zakzanis and Young 2001). For the most part, these studies suggested that heavy but not moderate ecstasy users had impaired verbal memory and lower numbers of estimated SERT sites, with heavy use often defined as being at or greater than 50 times or tablets. Estimated SERT sites returned to control levels after sustained abstinence from ecstasy, while cognitive function did not return to control levels (though see Zakzanis and Campbell 2006). These studies failed to find improved cognitive function after abstinence from ecstasy use, and they failed to find further deterioration after continued use. The different pattern of findings for SERT sites and cognitive function suggested that changes in one domain should not be treated as an indicator of changes in another domain. Impaired Cognitive Function Two recent findings concerning cognitive function in ecstasy users have appeared in the literature, with one finding measuring a facet of visual perception and the other pertaining to effects of anxiety over confirming others’ negative views might have on ecstasy user participants. In a study of “heading,” or perceived self-motion, in ecstasy users and cannabis user controls, Rizzo and associated found that both ecstasy and cannabis users had difficulties with “heading” in a simulated visual perception task, with ecstasy users performing less well than cannabis users under angles (Rizzo et al. 2005). Study participants had to a minimum lifetime ecstasy use of at least ten occasions, and average cumulative use was 36.9 +/- 32.1 occasions, with an average period of abstinence of roughly half a year (226.9 +/- 134 days). Hence while cumulative use was lower than in other studies of cognitive function in ecstasy users, it is greater than expected during human MDMA studies. Ecstasy users performing tests of cognitive function may be affected by stereotype threat, the fear of confirming negative beliefs people hold about a specific group membership, as race or gender. When ecstasy users heard from investigators that ecstasy use had no effects on memory, they scored higher on measures of memory than ecstasy users given information stating that ecstasy use impairs memory, while both groups of ecstasy users scored similarly to non-user controls on measures of executive function (Cole et al. 2006). Hence it may be the case that findings of impaired cognitive function in ecstasy users are due in part to the disruptive effects of stereotype threat.1 Two independent meta-analyses (cross-study statistical analyses) of memory in ecstasy users arrived at somewhat contradictory conclusions (Laws and Kokkalis 2007; Zakzanis et al. 2007). While both analyses detected an association between ecstasy use and impaired performance on at least some measures of memory, one analysis, that of Laws and Kokkalis, reported that this association had a medium to large effect size and found 1 The author of this document is a coauthor of the paper on the effects of stereotype threat on memory. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 26 no effect of ecstasy dose, while the other, that of Zakzanis and colleagues, reported that the association had a small to medium effect size and found a dose effect. Zakzanis and colleagues also concluded that use of other drugs independently impaired cognitive function, while Laws and Kokkalis failed to find an association between cannabis use and verbal memory performance, relating it instead to visual memory performance. It is unclear why the two analyses reached somewhat different conclusions. Both examined a similar though not identical set of retrospective studies. It is important to note that minimum cumulative use in both analysis was above ten uses, and that average cumulative use was considerably higher (287 tablets in Zakzanis’ analysis and 327 in Laws and Kokkalis’ paper). Previous research has established a link between repeated ecstasy use and impaired executive function, defined here as planning, decision-making, and inhibiting a welllearned response (Baggott et al. 2001; Cole and Sumnall 2003; Jerome 2005). The nature and strength of the association between regular ecstasy use and impaired executive function remains inconclusive, with some reports finding impaired executive function in ecstasy users, particularly heavy users (Halpern et al. 2004; Wareing et al. 2004) while others failed to find differences between ecstasy user and non-user executive function (Thomasius et al. 2003), or found executive function impairments only in male ecstasy users (von Geusau et al. 2004). Current studies continue to support both presence and absence of a relationship between ecstasy use and executive function. It is possible that polydrug use may also contribute to ecstasy users’ impaired executive function (Hoshi et al. 2007b; Medina and Shear 2006). Psychological Well-being, Affect, Impulsivity, and Sleep Previous reports had found an association between ecstasy use and reporting greater increases in symptoms of depression or anxiety (see for example MacInnes et al. 2001; Parrott et al. 2000). A meta-analysis of self-reported depressive symptoms detected an association between ecstasy use and scores on the Beck Depression Inventory, a popular self-report measure of depression symptoms (Sumnall and Cole 2005). Sumnall and Cole noted that the association was strongest in studies with small samples, and noted that drug use variables are often incompletely reported and not verified through any methods save self-report in these studies. Findings concerning the long-term effects of ecstasy use on mood, including findings from longitudinal studies, suggested that an association between increased feelings or symptoms of anxiety and depression and ecstasy use exists, but that these findings were more strongly related to polydrug use rather than to use of any one substance (Milani et al. 2004; Sumnall et al. 2004) (Medina and Shear 2006). Some studies found that continued use of cannabis rather than ecstasy was associated with self-reported psychological problems (Dafters et al. 2004; Daumann et al. 2004a; Daumann et al. 2001). At least one study found an association between possessing a genetic variation on the serotonin transporter gene and increased self-reported depression in heavy ecstasy users (Roiser et al. 2006), and another study reported finding greater self-reported psychological problems in ecstasy users than cannabis users, though the two samples did not differ on memory task performance (Lamers et al. 2006). On the other hand, a study of heavy ecstasy users failed to find any increase in diagnosis of depression (de Win et al. 2004), and two studies examining large samples of ecstasy users either Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 27 failed to find increased depressive symptoms or found increased symptoms only in a subset of heavy users (Falck et al. 2006; Guillot and Greenway 2006). The relationship between ecstasy use and impulsivity has also been extensively examined, with some researchers reporting greater impulsivity in ecstasy users while others failing to find any differences (see for example McCann et al. 1994; Morgan 1998). Recent studies using both behavioral and self-report measures of impulsivity reached contradictory conclusions (Morgan et al. 2006; Quednow et al. 2007; Roiser et al. 2007). Two recent studies even used the same measure of behavioral impulsivity in samples of heavy ecstasy users, yet obtained different findings concerning the relationship between ecstasy use and impulsivity (Quednow et al. 2007; Roiser et al. 2007). It is possible and likely that people who self-administer ecstasy may already possess above-average levels of sensation-seeking and impulsiveness. To date, all such studies have used retrospective study designs and cannot rule out this possibility. Taken together, self-reported changes or deterioration in psychological well-being and impulsivity in ecstasy users are likely multiply determined and only partially, if at all, uniquely related to ecstasy use. Drawing conclusions from retrospective studies continued to raise questions concerning the strength and causal link between findings, especially when studies in representative samples suggested that people who chose to use ecstasy and other drugs were liable to have psychological problems prior to use (Huizink et al. 2006; Lieb et al. 2002), and given the fact that polysubstance use is so prevalent among ecstasy users (GouzoulisMayfrank and Daumann 2006a). It is notable that some of the most recent studies have either found that polydrug use was equally associated with impaired cognitive function as was ecstasy use (Hoshi et al. 2007b), or that use of cannabis or other drugs make additional and even greater contributions to differences between ecstasy users and controls (Jager et al. 2007a; Montgomery and Fisk 2007). Researchers at Johns Hopkins University have investigated the effects of regular ecstasy use on sleep (Allen et al. 1993; McCann et al. 2007). Their earlier study reported detecting less stage II sleep, while a recent study found some differences between ecstasy users and controls, in this case, including less stage II sleep and less reduction in sleep latency after administering a compound that reduced catecholamine neurotransmitters (McCann et al. 2007). To date, no other researchers have examined sleep in ecstasy users, but both studies consisted largely of samples of heavy ecstasy users, and the recent study detected an association between degree of ecstasy use and effects on sleep. Prospective Studies The Netherlands XTC Toxicity team mentioned at the beginning of this document is the first to perform prospective research studies comparing ecstasy users before and after their first few uses, sometimes comparing them with controls who have not yet taken ecstasy (De Win et al. 2005). Average cumulative use in these studies ranged from 1.8 to 6 tablets, and maximum use in two of three studies was 10 tablets. In one study, the researchers imaged the brains of 30 people before and approximately seven weeks after having used ecstasy. They failed to find any chemical markers of neuronal injury in Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 28 ecstasy users, and they found very few changes in cerebral blood flow, with the exception of decreased cerebral blood volume in the dorsolateral frontal cortex (De Win 2006). Another study examined working memory in 25 people reporting an average use of 2 tablets with 24 controls, failing to find any significant differences either in brain activity as assessed via functional magnetic imagery (fMRI) or on tests of working memory and selective attention (Jager et al. 2007b). A study examining self-reported depression symptoms, failed to find an association between low ecstasy use and symptoms of depression, though they also failed to find a relationship between symptoms of depression and likelihood of taking ecstasy (de Win et al. 2006). Finally, a study comparing 58 people reporting use of 3.2 tablets with 60 controls before and after use, up to 18 months later, and found an association between ecstasy use and performance on measures of verbal memory, and not attention or working memory (Schilt et al. 2007). While all participants exhibited scores within the normal range both times they were tested, people who did not use ecstasy showed greater improvement in performance than did people who used it. Analyses in the study assessing cognitive function apparently included one individual reporting higher cumulative ecstasy use, 30 tablets. In contrast, data from a prospective controlled study that was presented at a conference failed to find impaired cognitive function in drug-naïve individuals after two doses of 1.5 to 1.7 mg/kg MDMA (Ludewig S et al. 2003). Taken together, a majority of the prospective studies failed to find indications of structural or functional change after low ecstasy use, while one study found impairment in memory after low to moderate use. While these prospective studies do not and cannot demonstrate either a definite lack or presence of long-term effects from a few exposures to MDMA, they did not find the types of changes seen in heavy ecstasy users, and they suggest that risks of long-term effects associated with taking part in human MDMA studies are low. Conclusions Many studies in nonhuman animals suggest that frequent or high doses of MDMA can damage serotonin neurons, and some studies in ecstasy using humans suggest that repeated use, especially when heavy, can affect serotonergic function and specific domains of cognitive function. Ecstasy users exhibit impairment in specific areas of cognitive function, particularly verbal memory. However, when apparent, most long-term effects seem to be more strongly associated with heavy and not moderate use. The risk of impaired serotonin function or verbal memory after exposure to one to there doses of MDMA in the course of a controlled study remains possible, but evidence from retrospective and prospective studies of ecstasy users suggest that this risk is minimal after a low number of exposures. While there may also be risks related to psychological well-being such as increased symptoms of anxiety or depression, support for these longterm effects are even less strong than for the previously listed changes. Abuse Potential The US Drug Enforcement Administration (DEA) placed MDMA in Schedule 1, the most restrictive schedule reserved for compounds with high abuse potential and no medical value, and most other nations followed the lead of the US in making MDMA a tightly controlled substance. Studies in humans and nonhuman animals suggest MDMA possesses some abuse potential. However, it also appears that MDMA has fewer or less Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 29 intensely rewarding effects than psychostimulants, and even heavy ecstasy users fail to report the intensive patterns of use seen with other stimulants. Hence MDMA possesses moderate abuse liability that is greater than abuse liability for serotonergic hallucinogens but lesser than for psychostimulants. Mice, rats and monkeys will self-administer MDMA (Fantegrossi et al. 2004; Schenk et al. 2003; Trigo et al. 2006), indicating that MDMA has rewarding properties in nonhuman animals. Monkeys chose to self-administer MDMA in doses equivalent to or only slightly higher than doses used by humans (Fantegrossi et al. 2004), but they reduced their MDMA intake over time. While monkeys will work hard to obtain MDMA, they will work harder to obtain other psychostimulants, such as cocaine or methamphetamine (Lile et al. 2005; Wang and Woolverton 2007). Of the small number of individuals assessed in a representative sample of Munich residents aged 14 to 24, only 1% were diagnosed with ecstasy abuse and 0.6% with dependence (von Sydow et al. 2002), though other reports of non-representative samples have reported higher percentages of MDMA abuse or dependence (Cottler et al. 2001), and approximately 25% of polydrug users who had used ecstasy reported abuse or dependency (Topp et al. 1999). Reproductive and Developmental Toxicity Previous research supported a possible link between ecstasy use and birth defects (McElhatton et al. 1999), while an epidemiological study conducted in 2004 in a large cohort of pregnant women in England failed to support this link, at least in respect to a specific cardiac defect (Bateman et al. 2004). However, the authors also stated that exposure to MDMA in their sample was too low to establish risk. An earlier survey of a drug-using population suggests that most women cease using ecstasy when they learn they are pregnant (Ho et al. 2001). Several teams of researchers have performed studies of developmental toxicity in rodents (see for example (Koprich et al. 2003a; Koprich et al. 2003b; Piper and Meyer 2004; Williams et al. 2005). In some studies, the researchers administered large, repeated doses to pregnant rats, and in others, the MDMA was administered to neonatal rats. The researchers did not report gross structural abnormalities in rats exposed to high doses of MDMA in utero. However, studies of MDMA in neonatal rats found changes in numbers of serotonin or dopamine cells and impaired learning or memory, particularly when MDMA was administered from the 11th to the 20th day after birth. If this period is similar to the third trimester of human gestation, then it is possible that MDMA in humans could have similar developmental effects. Some researchers found that neonatal rat MDMA exposure exacerbated hyperthermic response to a subsequent dose to MDMA (Green et al. 2005), while others found that it attenuated this response (Piper et al. 2005). Given differences in rodent development and thermoregulation, it is not clear whether either or both findings can be generalized to humans. Because there may be a critical period during which exposure to MDMA could alter development, and as a result of the relative lack of information concerning its developmental toxicity, women who are pregnant or who are not using an effective means of birth control should not receive MDMA. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 30 Some investigators have claimed that MDMA affects sub-adult rats differently than adults. Giving somewhat large doses of MDMA to sub-adult rats produced long-term reductions in anxiety and impaired object recognition (Piper et al. 2004). An initial dose of MDMA in young rats also produced less of an increase in BT and fewer signs of “serotonin syndrome” when given another dose of MDMA in adulthood (Piper et al. 2005). These nonhuman animal studies suggest that adolescents could be more vulnerable to some effects of MDMA. 6. Research trial data Information is being gathered and prepared. Side effects reported in the first clinical trials are similar to those reported in controlled studies, though anxiety may be more prevalent, due in part to the condition under study and in part to the nature of the setting, as participants are encouraged to confront emotionally upsetting thoughts, memories and feelings. In this setting anxiety is not chiefly viewed as a side effect, but as an element of the underlying disorder and the therapeutic process. 7. Acknowledgements I am grateful to Matthew Baggott for collaborating in creation of the first two documents and for continued support, knowledge and wisdom concerning the topics of MDMA and especially pharmacology. I am also thankful for the occasional help and information from a number of respected researchers for their insight, information and debate. I am thankful for the principal investigators Michael Mithoefer MD, John Halpern MD and Peter Oehen MD for embarking or continuing on their work, and I thank the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies (MAPS) and Rick Doblin PhD for supporting this work. Receptor binding profiles for MDMA was generously provided by the National Institute of Mental Health's Psychoactive Drug Screening Program, a publicly available database of drug pharmacological activity, Contract # NO1MH32004 (NIMH PDSP), with data accessed between November 12 and November 14, 2007. References Acquas E, Pisanu A, Spiga S, Plumitallo A, Zernig G, Di Chiara G (2007) Differential effects of intravenous R,S-(+/-)-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, Ecstasy) and its S(+)- and R(-)-enantiomers on dopamine transmission and extracellular signal regulated kinase phosphorylation (pERK) in the rat nucleus accumbens shell and core. J Neurochem 102: 121-32 Adamson S (1985) Through the gateway of the heart: Accounts of experiences With MDMA and other empathogenic substances. Four Trees Publications, Four Trees Publications Allen RP, McCann UD, Ricaurte GA (1993) Persistent effects of (+/- )3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "ecstasy") on human sleep. Sleep 16: 560-4 Anderson GMd, Braun G, Braun U, Nichols DE, Shulgin AT (1978) Absolute configuration and psychotomimetic activity. NIDA Res Monogr 22: 8-15 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 31 Baggott M, Jerome L, Stuart R (2001) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA); A review of the English-language scientific and medical literature Original Investigator's Brochure for MDMA. Multidiscplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies Baggott MJ (2002) Preventing problems in Ecstasy users: reduce use to reduce harm. J Psychoactive Drugs 34: 145-62 Banks ML, Sprague JE, Kisor DF, Czoty PW, Nichols DE, Nader MA (2007) Ambient Temperature Effects on 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA)-induced Thermodysregulation and Pharmacokinetics in Male Monkeys. Drug Metab Dispos Bankson MG, Yamamoto BK (2004) Serotonin-GABA interactions modulate MDMAinduced mesolimbic dopamine release. J Neurochem 91: 852-9 Bartz JA, Hollander E (2006) The neuroscience of affiliation: forging links between basic and clinical research on neuropeptides and social behavior. Horm Behav 50: 518- 28 Bateman DN, McElhatton PR, Dickinson D, Wren C, Matthews JN, O'Keeffe M, Thomas SH (2004) A case control study to examine the pharmacological factors underlying ventricular septal defects in the North of England. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 60: 635-41 Battaglia G, Brooks BP, Kulsakdinun C, De Souza EB (1988) Pharmacologic profile of MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) at various brain recognition sites. Eur J Pharmacol 149: 159-63 Baumann MH, Wang X, Rothman RB (2007) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) neurotoxicity in rats: a reappraisal of past and present findings. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 407-24 Baylen CA, Rosenberg H (2006) A review of the acute subjective effects of MDMA/ecstasy. Addiction 101: 933-47 Beck J, Rosenbaum M (1994) In Pursuit of Ecstasy: The MDMA Experience. SUNY Press, SUNY Press Beitia G, Cobreros A, Sainz L, Cenarruzabeitia E (2000) Ecstasy-induced toxicity in rat liver. Liver 20: 8-15 Bexis S, Docherty JR (2005) Role of alpha2A-adrenoceptors in the effects of MDMA on body temperature in the mouse. Br J Pharmacol 146: 1-6 Bexis S, Docherty JR (2006) Effects of MDMA, MDA and MDEA on blood pressure, heart rate, locomotor activity and body temperature in the rat involve alphaadrenoceptors. Br J Pharmacol 147: 926-34 Bishop C, Taylor JL, Kuhn DM, Eskow KL, Park JY, Walker PD (2006) MDMA and fenfluramine reduce L-DOPA-induced dyskinesia via indirect 5-HT1A receptor stimulation. Eur J Neurosci 23: 2669-76 Bond AJ, Feizollah S, Lader M (1995) The effects of d-fenfluramine on mood and performance, and on neuroendocrine indicators of 5-HT function. J Psychopharmacol 9: 1-8 Bowyer JF, Young JF, Slikker W, Itzak Y, Mayorga AJ, Newport GD, Ali SF, Frederick DL, Paule MG (2003) Plasma levels of parent compound and metabolites after doses of either d-fenfluramine or d-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 32 (MDMA) that produce long-term serotonergic alterations. Neurotoxicology 24: 379-90 Brazier WJ, Dhariwal DK, Patton DW, Bishop K (2003) Ecstasy related periodontitis and mucosal ulceration -- a case report. Br Dent J 194: 197-9 Brunner D, Hen R (1997) Insights into the neurobiology of impulsive behavior from serotonin receptor knockout mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci 836: 81-105 Buchert R, Thomasius R, Petersen K, Wilke F, Obrocki J, Nebeling B, Wartberg L, Zapletalova P, Clausen M (2006) Reversibility of ecstasy-induced reduction in serotonin transporter availability in polydrug ecstasy users. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 33: 188-99 Buchert R, Thomasius R, Wilke F, Petersen K, Nebeling B, Obrocki J, Schulze O, Schmidt U, Clausen M (2004) A voxel-based PET investigation of the long-term effects of "Ecstasy" consumption on brain serotonin transporters. Am J Psychiatry 161: 1181-9 Caballero F, Lopez-Navidad A, Cotorruelo J, Txoperena G (2002) Ecstasy-induced brain death and acute hepatocellular failure: multiorgan donor and liver transplantation. Transplantation 74: 532-7. Callahan BT, Cord BJ, Ricaurte GA (2001) Long-term impairment of anterograde axonal transport along fiber projections originating in the rostral raphe nuclei after treatment with fenfluramine or methylenedioxymethamphetamine. Synapse 40: 113-21. Cami J, Farre M, Mas M, Roset PN, Poudevida S, Mas A, San L, de la Torre R (2000) Human pharmacology of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("ecstasy"): psychomotor performance and subjective effects. J Clin Psychopharmacol 20: 455-66 Carlson RG, Falck RS, McCaughan JA, Siegal HA (2004) MDMA/Ecstasy use among young people in Ohio: perceived risk and barriers to intervention. J Psychoactive Drugs 36: 181-9 Carlson RG, Wang J, Falck RS, Siegal HA (2005) Drug use practices among MDMA/ecstasy users in Ohio: a latent class analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend 79: 167-79 Chang L, Ernst T, Grob CS, Poland RE (1999) Cerebral (1)H MRS alterations in recreational 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "ecstasy") users. J Magn Reson Imaging 10: 521-6 Chang L, Grob CS, Ernst T, Itti L, Mishkin FS, Jose-Melchor R, Poland RE (2000) Effect of ecstasy [3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA)] on cerebral blood flow: a co-registered SPECT and MRI study. Psychiatry Res 98: 15-28 Chu T, Kumagai Y, DiStefano EW, Cho AK (1996) Disposition of methylenedioxymethamphetamine and three metabolites in the brains of different rat strains and their possible roles in acute serotonin depletion. Biochem Pharmacol 51: 789-96 Cohen RS (1995) Subjective reports on the effects of the MDMA ('ecstasy') experience in humans. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 19: 1137-45 Cole JC, Michailidou K, Jerome L, Sumnall HR (2006) The effects of stereotype threat on cognitive function in ecstasy users. J Psychopharmacol 20: 518-25 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 33 Cole JC, Sumnall HR (2003a) Altered states: the clinical effects of Ecstasy. Pharmacol Ther 98: 35-58 Cole JC, Sumnall HR (2003b) The pre-clinical behavioural pharmacology of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA). Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27: 199- 217 Cole JC, Sumnall HR, Smith GW, Rostami-Hodjegan A (2005) Preliminary evidence of the cardiovascular effects of polysubstance misuse in nightclubs. J Psychopharmacol 19: 67-70 Connor TJ, Kelly JP, Leonard BE (2000) An assessment of the acute effects of the serotonin releasers methylenedioxymethamphetamine, methylenedioxyamphetamine and fenfluramine on immunity in rats. Immunopharmacology 46: 223-35 Cottler LB, Womack SB, Compton WM, Ben-Abdallah A (2001) Ecstasy abuse and dependence among adolescents and young adults: applicability and reliability of DSM-IV criteria. Hum Psychopharmacol 16: 599-606 Cowan RL, Bolo NR, Dietrich M, Haga E, Lukas SE, Renshaw PF (2007) Occipital cortical proton MRS at 4 Tesla in human moderate MDMA polydrug users. Psychiatry Res Crean RD, Davis SA, Taffe MA (2007) Oral administration of (+/-)3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine and (+)methamphetamine alters temperature and activity in rhesus macaques. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 87: 11-9 Crean RD, Davis SA, Von Huben SN, Lay CC, Katner SN, Taffe MA (2006) Effects of (+/-)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, (+/-)3,4methylenedioxyamphetamine and methamphetamine on temperature and activity in rhesus macaques. Neuroscience 142: 515-25 Cregg MT, Tracey JA (1993) Ecstasy abuse in Ireland. Ir Med J 86: 118-20 Dafters RI (1994) Effect of ambient temperature on hyperthermia and hyperkinesis induced by 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or "ecstasy") in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 114: 505-8 Dafters RI, Hoshi R, Talbot AC (2004) Contribution of cannabis and MDMA ("ecstasy") to cognitive changes in long-term polydrug users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 173: 405-10 Daumann J, Hensen G, Thimm B, Rezk M, Till B, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E (2004a) Selfreported psychopathological symptoms in recreational ecstasy (MDMA) users are mainly associated with regular cannabis use: further evidence from a combined cross-sectional/longitudinal investigation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 173: 398- 404 Daumann J, Jr., Fischermann T, Heekeren K, Thron A, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E (2004b) Neural mechanisms of working memory in ecstasy (MDMA) users who continue or discontinue ecstasy and amphetamine use: evidence from an 18-month longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 56: 349-55 Daumann J, Pelz S, Becker S, Tuchtenhagen F, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E (2001) Psychological profile of abstinent recreational Ecstasy (MDMA) users and significance of concomitant cannabis use. Hum Psychopharmacol 16: 627-633 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 34 Davis WM, Hatoum HT, Waters IW (1987) Toxicity of MDA (3,4methylenedioxyamphetamine) considered for relevance to hazards of MDMA (Ecstasy) abuse. Alcohol Drug Res 7: 123-34 de Boer D, Tan LP, Gorter P, van de Wal RM, Kettenes-van den Bosch JJ, de Bruijn EA, Maes RA (1997) Gas chromatographic/mass spectrometric assay for profiling the enantiomers of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine and its chiral metabolites using positive chemical ionization ion trap mass spectrometry. J Mass Spectrom 32: 1236-46 De La Garza R, 2nd, Fabrizio KR, Gupta A (2007) Relevance of rodent models of intravenous MDMA self-administration to human MDMA consumption patterns. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 425-434 de la Torre R, Farre M, Ortuno J, Mas M, Brenneisen R, Roset PN, Segura J, Cami J (2000) Non-linear pharmacokinetics of MDMA ('ecstasy') in humans. Br J Clin Pharmacol 49: 104-9 de la Torre R, Farre M, Roset PN, Pizarro N, Abanades S, Segura M, Segura J, Cami J (2004) Human pharmacology of MDMA: pharmacokinetics, metabolism, and disposition. Ther Drug Monit 26: 137-44 De Letter EA, Bouche MP, Van Bocxlaer JF, Lambert WE, Piette MH (2004) Interpretation of a 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) blood level: discussion by means of a distribution study in two fatalities. Forensic Sci Int 141: 85-90 De Win M, Jager, G., Reneman, L., Booij, J., van den Brink, W., Den Heeten, G., et al. (2006) Ecstasy: Is It Safe for the Brain? First Prospective Study on Effects of Low Doses of Ecstasy on the Brain in New Ecstasy Users, Using a Combination of Advanced MR Imaging Techniques and [123I]ß-CIT SPECT Radiological Society of North America (RSNA), Chicago, IL De Win MM, Jager G, Vervaeke HK, Schilt T, Reneman L, Booij J, Verhulst FC, Den Heeten GJ, Ramsey NF, Korf DJ, Van den Brink W (2005) The Netherlands XTC Toxicity (NeXT) study: objectives and methods of a study investigating causality, course, and clinical relevance. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 14: 167-85 de Win MM, Reneman L, Jager G, Vlieger EJ, Olabarriaga SD, Lavini C, Bisschops I, Majoie CB, Booij J, den Heeten GJ, van den Brink W (2007) A Prospective Cohort Study on Sustained Effects of Low-Dose Ecstasy Use on the Brain in New Ecstasy Users. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 458-470 de Win MM, Reneman L, Reitsma JB, den Heeten GJ, Booij J, van den Brink W (2004) Mood disorders and serotonin transporter density in ecstasy users--the influence of long-term abstention, dose, and gender. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 173: 376- 82 de Win MM, Schilt T, Reneman L, Vervaeke H, Jager G, Dijkink S, Booij J, van den Brink W (2006) Ecstasy use and self-reported depression, impulsivity, and sensation seeking: a prospective cohort study. J Psychopharmacol 20: 226-35 Domes G, Heinrichs M, Glascher J, Buchel C, Braus DF, Herpertz SC (2007) Oxytocin Attenuates Amygdala Responses to Emotional Faces Regardless of Valence. Biol Psychiatry Downing J (1986) The psychological and physiological effects of MDMA on normal volunteers. J Psychoactive Drugs 18: 335-40 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 35 Droogmans S, Cosyns B, D'Haenen H, Creeten E, Weytjens C, Franken PR, Scott B, Schoors D, Kemdem A, Close L, Vandenbossche JL, Bechet S, Van Camp G (2007) Possible association between 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine abuse and valvular heart disease. Am J Cardiol 100: 1442-5 Dumont GJ, Verkes RJ (2006) A review of acute effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 20: 176-87 Duxon MS, Kennett GA, Lightowler S, Blackburn TP, Fone KC (1997) Activation of 5HT2B receptors in the medial amygdala causes anxiolysis in the social interaction test in the rat. Neuropharmacology 36: 601-608 Falck RS, Wang J, Carlson RG, Siegal HA (2006) Prevalence and correlates of current depressive symptomatology among a community sample of MDMA users in Ohio. Addict Behav 31: 90-101 Fallon JK, Shah D, Kicman AT, Hutt AJ, Henry JA, Cowan DA, Forsling M (2002) Action of MDMA (ecstasy) and its metabolites on arginine vasopressin release. Ann N Y Acad Sci 965: 399-409 Fantegrossi WE (2007) Reinforcing effects of methylenedioxy amphetamine congeners in rhesus monkeys: are intravenous self-administration experiments relevant to MDMA neurotoxicity? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 471-82 Fantegrossi WE, Godlewski T, Karabenick RL, Stephens JM, Ullrich T, Rice KC, Woods JH (2003) Pharmacological characterization of the effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("ecstasy") and its enantiomers on lethality, core temperature, and locomotor activity in singly housed and crowded mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 166: 202-11 Fantegrossi WE, Woolverton WL, Kilbourn M, Sherman P, Yuan J, Hatzidimitriou G, Ricaurte GA, Woods JH, Winger G (2004) Behavioral and neurochemical consequences of long-term intravenous self-administration of MDMA and its enantiomers by rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 1270-81 Farre M, Abanades S, Roset PN, Peiro AM, Torrens M, B OM, Segura M, de la Torre R (2007) Pharmacological Interaction Between 3,4Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, ecstasy) and Paroxetine: Pharmacological effects and pharmacokinetics. J Pharmacol Exp Ther Farre M, de la Torre R, Mathuna BO, Roset PN, Peiro AM, Torrens M, Ortuno J, Pujadas M, Cami J (2004) Repeated doses administration of MDMA in humans: pharmacological effects and pharmacokinetics. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 173: 364-75 Finnegan KT, Ricaurte GA, Ritchie LD, Irwin I, Peroutka SJ, Langston JW (1988) Orally administered MDMA causes a long-term depletion of serotonin in rat brain. Brain Res 447: 141-4 Freedman RR, Johanson CE, Tancer ME (2005) Thermoregulatory effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 183: 248-56 Frei E, Gamma A, Pascual-Marqui R, Lehmann D, Hell D, Vollenweider FX (2001) Localization of MDMA-induced brain activity in healthy volunteers using low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA). Hum Brain Mapp 14: 152-65 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 36 Frenzilli G, Ferrucci M, Giorgi FS, Blandini F, Nigro M, Ruggieri S, Murri L, Paparelli A, Fornai F (2007) DNA fragmentation and oxidative stress in the hippocampal formation: a bridge between 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) intake and long-lasting behavioral alterations. Behav Pharmacol 18: 471-481 Freudenmann RW, Oxler F, Bernschneider-Reif S (2006) The origin of MDMA (ecstasy) revisited: the true story reconstructed from the original documents. Addiction 101: 1241-5 Frith CH, Chang LW, Lattin DL, Walls RC, Hamm J, Doblin R (1987) Toxicity of methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in the dog and the rat. Fundam Appl Toxicol 9: 110-9 Gamma A, Buck A, Berthold T, Liechti ME, Vollenweider FX (2000) 3,4Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) modulates cortical and limbic brain activity as measured by [H(2)(15)O]-PET in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 23: 388-95 Gamma A, Buck A, Berthold T, Vollenweider FX (2001) No difference in brain activation during cognitive performance between ecstasy (3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine) users and control subjects: a [H2(15)O]positron emission tomography study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 21: 66-71 Gasser P (1994) Psycholytic therapy with MDMA and LSD in Switzerland. MAPS Bulletin 5: 3-7 Gaston TR, Rasmussen, G. T. (1972) Identification of 3,4-methylenedioxy-nmethylamphetamine;. Microgram 5: 60-63 Giannaccini G, Betti L, Pirone A, Palego L, Fabiani O, Fabbrini L, Mascia G, Giusti L, Macchia M, Giusiani M, Martini C, Lucacchini A (2007) Short-term effects of 3,4-methylen-dioxy-metamphetamine (MDMA) on 5-HT(1A) receptors in the rat hippocampus. Neurochem Int 51: 496-506 Gilhooly TC, Daly AK (2002) CYP2D6 deficiency, a factor in ecstasy related deaths? Br J Clin Pharmacol 54: 69-70. Giorgi FS, Pizzanelli C, Ferrucci M, Lazzeri G, Faetti M, Giusiani M, Pontarelli F, Busceti CL, Murri L, Fornai F (2005) Previous exposure to (+/-) 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine produces long-lasting alteration in limbic brain excitability measured by electroencephalogram spectrum analysis, brain metabolism and seizure susceptibility. Neuroscience 136: 43-53 Glennon RA, Young R (2000) MDMA stimulus generalization to the 5-HT(1A) serotonin agonist 8-hydroxy-2- (di-n-propylamino)tetralin. 66: 483-488 Glennon RA, Young R, Dukat M, Chang-Fong J, El-Zahabi M (2007) N-Methyl-1-(4methoxyphenyl)-2-aminopropane (PMMA) and N-Methyl-1-(3,4methylenedioxyphenyl)-2-aminopropane (MDMA) produce non-identical discriminative stimuli in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 86: 477-84 Gordon CJ (2007) Thermophysiological responses to hyperthermic drugs: extrapolating from rodent to human. Prog Brain Res 162: 63-79 Gore SM (1999) Fatal uncertainty: death-rate from use of ecstasy or heroin. Lancet 354: 1265-6 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J (2006a) The confounding problem of polydrug use in recreational ecstasy/MDMA users: a brief overview. J Psychopharmacol 20: 188- 93 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 37 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J (2006b) Neurotoxicity of methylenedioxyamphetamines (MDMA; ecstasy) in humans: how strong is the evidence for persistent brain damage? Addiction 101: 348-61 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Fischermann T, Rezk M, Thimm B, Hensen G, Daumann J (2005) Memory performance in polyvalent MDMA (ecstasy) users who continue or discontinue MDMA use. Drug Alcohol Depend 78: 317-23 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Thelen B, Habermeyer E, Kunert HJ, Kovar KA, Lindenblatt H, Hermle L, Spitzer M, Sass H (1999) Psychopathological, neuroendocrine and autonomic effects of 3,4-methylenedioxyethylamphetamine (MDE), psilocybin and d-methamphetamine in healthy volunteers. Results of an experimental double-blind placebo-controlled study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 142: 41-50 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Thimm B, Rezk M, Hensen G, Daumann J (2003) Memory impairment suggests hippocampal dysfunction in abstinent ecstasy users. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 27: 819-27 Graeff FG, Guimaraes FS, De Andrade TG, Deakin JF (1996) Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 54: 129-41 Green AR, Mechan AO, Elliott JM, O'Shea E, Colado MI (2003) The pharmacology and clinical pharmacology of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "ecstasy"). Pharmacol Rev 55: 463-508 Green AR, O'Shea E, Saadat KS, Elliott JM, Colado MI (2005) Studies on the effect of MDMA ('ecstasy') on the body temperature of rats housed at different ambient room temperatures. Br J Pharmacol 146: 306-12 Greene SL, Dargan PI, O'Connor N, Jones AL, Kerins M (2003) Multiple toxicity from 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("ecstasy"). Am J Emerg Med 21: 121-4 Greer G, Tolbert R (1986) Subjective reports of the effects of MDMA in a clinical setting. J Psychoactive Drugs 18: 319-27 Greer GR, Tolbert R (1998) A method of conducting therapeutic sessions with MDMA. J Psychoactive Drugs 30: 371-379 Grob C (2001) Unpublished data on human study of psychological and physiological effects of MDMA Grob CS, Poland RE, Chang L, Ernst T (1996) Psychobiologic effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine in humans: methodological considerations and preliminary observations. Behav Brain Res 73: 103-7 Guillot C (2007) Is recreational ecstasy (MDMA) use associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms? J Psychoactive Drugs 39: 31-9 Guillot C, Greenway D (2006) Recreational ecstasy use and depression. J Psychopharmacol 20: 411-6 Gurtman CG, Morley KC, Li KM, Hunt GE, McGregor IS (2002) Increased anxiety in rats after 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine: association with serotonin depletion. Eur J Pharmacol 446: 89-96. Hall AP, Henry JA (2006) Acute toxic effects of 'Ecstasy' (MDMA) and related compounds: overview of pathophysiology and clinical management. Br J Anaesth 96: 678-85 Halpern JH (2006) Update on the MDMA-Assisted Psychotherapy Study for TreatmentResistant Anxiety Disorders Secondary to Advanced Stage Cancer. MAPS Bulletin 16: 16 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 38 Halpern JH, Pope HG, Jr., Sherwood AR, Barry S, Hudson JI, Yurgelun-Todd D (2004) Residual neuropsychological effects of illicit 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in individuals with minimal exposure to other drugs. Drug Alcohol Depend 75: 135-47 Han DD, Gu HH (2006) Comparison of the monoamine transporters from human and mouse in their sensitivities to psychostimulant drugs. BMC Pharmacol 6: 6 Hardman HF, Haavik CO, Seevers MH (1973) Relationship of the structure of mescaline and seven analogs to toxicity and behavior in five species of laboratory animals. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 25: 299-309 Harris DS, Baggott M, Mendelson JH, Mendelson JE, Jones RT (2002) Subjective and hormonal effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 162: 396-405 Hatzidimitriou G, McCann UD, Ricaurte GA (1999) Altered serotonin innervation patterns in the forebrain of monkeys treated with (+/-)3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine seven years previously: factors influencing abnormal recovery. J Neurosci 19: 5096-107 Helmlin HJ, Bracher K, Bourquin D, Vonlanthen D, Brenneisen R (1996) Analysis of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and its metabolites in plasma and urine by HPLC-DAD and GC-MS. J Anal Toxicol 20: 432-40 Helmlin HJ, Brenneisen R (1992) Determination of psychotropic phenylalkylamine derivatives in biological matrices by high-performance liquid chromatography with photodiode-array detection. J Chromatogr 593: 87-94 Henry JA, Rella JG (2001) Medical risks associated with MDMA use. In: Holland J (ed) Ecstasy: A Complete Guide. Inner Traditions, Rochester, VT, pp 71-86. Ho E, Karimi-Tabesh L, Koren G (2001) Characteristics of pregnant women who use Ecstasy (3, 4- methylenedioxymethamphetamine). Neurotoxicol Teratol 23: 561- 7. Ho YJ, Pawlak CR, Guo L, Schwarting RK (2004) Acute and long-term consequences of single MDMA administration in relation to individual anxiety levels in the rat. Behav Brain Res 149: 135-44 Hoshi R, Bisla J, Curran HV (2004) The acute and sub-acute effects of 'ecstasy' (MDMA) on processing of facial expressions: preliminary findings. Drug Alcohol Depend 76: 297-304 Hoshi R, Cohen L, Lemanski L, Piccini P, Bond A, Curran HV (2007a) Ecstasy (MDMA) does not have long-term effects on aggressive interpretative bias: a study comparing current and ex-ecstasy users with polydrug and drug-naive controls. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 15: 351-8 Hoshi R, Mullins K, Boundy C, Brignell C, Piccini P, Curran HV (2007b) Neurocognitive function in current and ex-users of ecstasy in comparison to both matched polydrug-using controls and drug-naive controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 194: 371-9 Huizink AC, Ferdinand RF, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC (2006) Symptoms of anxiety and depression in childhood and use of MDMA: prospective, population based study. Bmj 332: 825-8 Huxster JK, Pirona A, Morgan MJ (2006) The sub-acute effects of recreational ecstasy (MDMA) use: a controlled study in humans. J Psychopharmacol 20: 281-90 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 39 Iravani MM, Jackson MJ, Kuoppamaki M, Smith LA, Jenner P (2003) 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) inhibits dyskinesia expression and normalizes motor activity in 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine-treated primates. J Neurosci 23: 9107-15 Irvine RJ, Keane M, Felgate P, McCann UD, Callaghan PD, White JM (2006) Plasma drug concentrations and physiological measures in 'dance party' participants. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 424-30 Jager G, de Win MM, van der Tweel I, Schilt T, Kahn RS, van den Brink W, van Ree JM, Ramsey NF (2007a) Assessment of Cognitive Brain Function in Ecstasy Users and Contributions of Other Drugs of Abuse: Results from an fMRI Study. Neuropsychopharmacology Jager G, de Win MM, Vervaeke HK, Schilt T, Kahn RS, van den Brink W, van Ree JM, Ramsey NF (2007b) Incidental use of ecstasy: no evidence for harmful effects on cognitive brain function in a prospective fMRI study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) Jerome L (2004) MAPS' Investigator's Brochure Update #2: A Review of Research in Human and Non-human animals. Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies Jerome L (2005) MDMA Literature update; March 2004-January 2005. Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies Jerome L, Baggott M (2003) MAPS' MDMA investigator's brochure update #1: A review of research in humans and non-human animals. Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies Johnson MP, Hoffman AJ, Nichols DE (1986) Effects of the enantiomers of MDA, MDMA and related analogues on [3H]serotonin and [3H]dopamine release from superfused rat brain slices. Eur J Pharmacol 132: 269-76 Jones DC, Lau SS, Monks TJ (2004) Thioether metabolites of 3,4methylenedioxyamphetamine and 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine inhibit human serotonin transporter (hSERT) function and simultaneously stimulate dopamine uptake into hSERT-expressing SK-N-MC cells. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 311: 298-306 Kindlundh-Hogberg AM, Svenningsson P, Schioth HB (2006) Quantitative mapping shows that serotonin rather than dopamine receptor mRNA expressions are affected after repeated intermittent administration of MDMA in rat brain. Neuropharmacology 51: 838-47 Kirsch P, Esslinger C, Chen Q, Mier D, Lis S, Siddhanti S, Gruppe H, Mattay VS, Gallhofer B, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2005) Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. J Neurosci 25: 11489-93 Koprich JB, Campbell NG, Lipton JW (2003a) Neonatal 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) alters dopamine and serotonin neurochemistry and increases brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the forebrain and brainstem of the rat. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 147: 177-82 Koprich JB, Chen EY, Kanaan NM, Campbell NG, Kordower JH, Lipton JW (2003b) Prenatal 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) alters exploratory behavior, reduces monoamine metabolism, and increases forebrain tyrosine hydroxylase fiber density of juvenile rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 25: 509-17 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 40 Krystal JH, Price LH, Opsahl C, Ricaurte GA, Heninger GR (1992) Chronic 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) use: effects on mood and neuropsychological function? Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 18: 331-41 Kuypers KP, Ramaekers JG (2005) Transient memory impairment after acute dose of 75mg 3.4-Methylene-dioxymethamphetamine. J Psychopharmacol 19: 633-9 Kuypers KP, Ramaekers JG (2007) Acute dose of MDMA (75 mg) impairs spatial memory for location but leaves contextual processing of visuospatial information unaffected. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 557-63 Lamers CT, Bechara A, Rizzo M, Ramaekers JG (2006) Cognitive function and mood in MDMA/THC users, THC users and non-drug using controls. J Psychopharmacol 20: 302-11 Lamers CT, Ramaekers JG, Muntjewerff ND, Sikkema KL, Samyn N, Read NL, Brookhuis KA, Riedel WJ (2003) Dissociable effects of a single dose of ecstasy (MDMA) on psychomotor skills and attentional performance. J Psychopharmacol 17: 379-87 Lanz M, Brenneisen R, Thormann W (1997) Enantioselective determination of 3,4methylene-dioxymethamphetamine and two of its metabolites in human urine by cyclodextrin-modified capillary zone electrophoresis. Electrophoresis 18: 1035-43 Lavelle A, Honner V, Docherty JR (1999) Investigation of the prejunctional alpha2adrenoceptor mediated actions of MDMA in rat atrium and vas deferens. Br J Pharmacol 128: 975-80 Laws KR, Kokkalis J (2007) Ecstasy (MDMA) and memory function: a meta-analytic update. Hum Psychopharmacol Lebsanft HB, Kohles T, Kovar KA, Schmidt WJ (2005a) 3,4Methylenedioxymethamphetamine counteracts akinesia enantioselectively in rat rotational behavior and catalepsy. Synapse 55: 148-55 Lebsanft HB, Kovar KA, Schmidt WJ (2005b) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine and naloxone in rat rotational behaviour and open field. Eur J Pharmacol 516: 34- 9 Lester SJ, Baggott M, Welm S, Schiller NB, Jones RT, Foster E, Mendelson J (2000) Cardiovascular effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine. A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Ann Intern Med 133: 969-73 Lieb R, Schuetz CG, Pfister H, von Sydow K, Wittchen H (2002) Mental disorders in ecstasy users: a prospective-longitudinal investigation. Drug Alcohol Depend 68: 195-207. Liechti ME, Baumann C, Gamma A, Vollenweider FX (2000a) Acute psychological effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "Ecstasy") are attenuated by the serotonin uptake inhibitor citalopram. Neuropsychopharmacology 22: 513-21 Liechti ME, Gamma A, Vollenweider FX (2001a) Gender differences in the subjective effects of MDMA. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 154: 161-8 Liechti ME, Gamma A, Vollenweider FX (2001b) Gender differences in the subjective effects of MDMA. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 154: 161-8. Liechti ME, Kunz I, Kupferschmidt H (2005) Acute medical problems due to Ecstasy use. Case-series of emergency department visits. Swiss Med Wkly 135: 652-7 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 41 Liechti ME, Saur MR, Gamma A, Hell D, Vollenweider FX (2000b) Psychological and physiological effects of MDMA ("Ecstasy") after pretreatment with the 5-HT(2) antagonist ketanserin in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 23: 396-404 Liechti ME, Vollenweider FX (2000a) Acute psychological and physiological effects of MDMA ("Ecstasy") after haloperidol pretreatment in healthy humans. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 10: 289-95 Liechti ME, Vollenweider FX (2000b) The serotonin uptake inhibitor citalopram reduces acute cardiovascular and vegetative effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine ('Ecstasy') in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 14: 269-74 Liechti ME, Vollenweider FX (2001) Which neuroreceptors mediate the subjective effects of MDMA in humans? A summary of mechanistic studies. Hum Psychopharmacol 16: 589-598 Liester MB, Grob CS, Bravo GL, Walsh RN (1992) Phenomenology and sequelae of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine use. J Nerv Ment Dis 180: 345-52; discussion 353-4 Lile JA, Ross JT, Nader MA (2005) A comparison of the reinforcing efficacy of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "ecstasy") with cocaine in rhesus monkeys. Drug Alcohol Depend 78: 135-40 Ludewig S, Ludewig K, Hasler F, FX V (2003) No lasting effects of moderate doses of MDMA (Ecstasy) on memory performance and mood states in health humans. . Biol Psychiatry 52 (Suppl) . 205S Lyon RA, Glennon RA, Titeler M (1986) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA): stereoselective interactions at brain 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 88: 525-6 MacInnes N, Handley SL, Harding GF (2001) Former chronic methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or ecstasy) users report mild depressive symptoms. J Psychopharmacol 15: 181-6. Mahmood I, Balian JD (1996) Interspecies scaling: a comparative study for the prediction of clearance and volume using two or more than two species. Life Sci 59: 579-85 Malberg JE, Seiden LS (1998) Small changes in ambient temperature cause large changes in 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA)-induced serotonin neurotoxicity and core body temperature in the rat. J Neurosci 18: 5086-94 Mas M, Farre M, de la Torre R, Roset PN, Ortuno J, Segura J, Cami J (1999) Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine effects and pharmacokinetics of 3, 4methylenedioxymethamphetamine in humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 290: 136-45 McCann UD, Mertl M, Eligulashvili V, Ricaurte GA (1999) Cognitive performance in (+/-) 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "ecstasy") users: a controlled study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 143: 417-25 McCann UD, Peterson SC, Ricaurte GA (2007) The Effect of Catecholamine Depletion by Alpha-Methyl-Para-Tyrosine on Measures of Cognitive Performance and Sleep in Abstinent MDMA Users. Neuropsychopharmacology McCann UD, Ridenour A, Shaham Y, Ricaurte GA (1994) Serotonin neurotoxicity after (+/-)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA; "Ecstasy"): a controlled study in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 10: 129-38 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 42 McElhatton PR, Bateman DN, Evans C, Pughe KR, Thomas SH (1999) Congenital anomalies after prenatal ecstasy exposure [letter]. Lancet 354: 1441-2 Mechan A, Yuan J, Hatzidimitriou G, Irvine RJ, McCann UD, Ricaurte GA (2006) Pharmacokinetic profile of single and repeated oral doses of MDMA in squirrel monkeys: relationship to lasting effects on brain serotonin neurons. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 339-50 Medina KL, Shear PK (2006) Anxiety, depression, and behavioral symptoms of executive dysfunction in ecstasy users: Contributions of polydrug use. Drug Alcohol Depend Metzner R, Adamson S (2001) sing MDMA in healing, psychotherapy and spiritual practice. In: J H (ed) cstasy, A Complete Guide: A Comprehensive Look at the Risks and Benefits of MDMA. Inner Traditions, Rochester VT, pp 182-207 Meyer JS, Brevard ME, Piper BJ, Ali SF, Ferris CF (2006) Neural effects of MDMA as determined by functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetic resonance spectroscopy in awake marmoset monkeys. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1074: 365-76 Michael JC, Pak J, Pulido J, de Venecia G (2003) Central serous chorioretinopathy associated with administration of sympathomimetic agents. Am J Ophthalmol 136: 182-5 Milani RM, Parrott AC, Turner JJ, Fox HC (2004) Gender differences in self-reported anxiety, depression, and somatization among ecstasy/MDMA polydrug users, alcohol/tobacco users, and nondrug users. Addict Behav 29: 965-71 Mithoefer M (2006) MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): ninth update on study progress. MAPS Bulletin 16: 14-15 Mlinar B, Corradetti R (2003) Endogenous 5-HT, released by MDMA through serotonin transporter- and secretory vesicle-dependent mechanisms, reduces hippocampal excitatory synaptic transmission by preferential activation of 5-HT1B receptors located on CA1 pyramidal neurons. Eur J Neurosci 18: 1559-71 Montgomery C, Fisk J (2007) Everyday memory deficits in ecstasy-polydrug users. J Psychopharmacol Mordenti J, Chappell W (1989) The use of interspecies scaling in toxicokinetics. In: Yacobi A, Kelly J, Batra V (eds) Toxicokinetics in new drug development. Pergamon Press, New York, NY, pp 42-96 Morgan MJ (1998) Recreational use of "ecstasy" (MDMA) is associated with elevated impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 19: 252-64 Morgan MJ, Impallomeni LC, Pirona A, Rogers RD (2006) Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making in abstinent Ecstasy (MDMA) users compared to polydrug and drug-naive controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 1562-73 Morley KC, Arnold JC, McGregor IS (2005) Serotonin (1A) receptor involvement in acute 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) facilitation of social interaction in the rat. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 29: 648-57 Mortimore C, Anderson IM (2000) d-Fenfluramine in panic disorder: a dual role for 5hydroxytryptamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 149: 251-8 Nair SG, Gudelsky GA (2005) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) enhances the release of acetylcholine by 5-HT4 and D1 receptor mechanisms in the rat prefrontal cortex. Synapse 58: 229-35 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 43 Nair SG, Gudelsky GA (2006) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine enhances the release of acetylcholine in the prefrontal cortex and dorsal hippocampus of the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 184: 182-9 Nash JF, Jr., Meltzer HY, Gudelsky GA (1988) Elevation of serum prolactin and corticosterone concentrations in the rat after the administration of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 245: 873-9 Nichols DE (1986) Differences between the mechanism of action of MDMA, MBDB, and the classic hallucinogens. Identification of a new therapeutic class: entactogens. J Psychoactive Drugs 18: 305-13 Oehen P (2006) MDMA/PTSD psychotherapy study in Switzerland launched. MAPS Bulletin 16: 15 Ortuno J, Pizarro N, Farre M, Mas M, Segura J, Cami J, Brenneisen R, de la Torre R (1999) Quantification of 3,4-methylenedioxymetamphetamine and its metabolites in plasma and urine by gas chromatography with nitrogen-phosphorus detection. J Chromatogr B Biomed Sci Appl 723: 221-32 Pacifici R, Pichini S, Zuccaro P, Farre M, Segura M, Ortuno J, Di Carlo S, Bacosi A, Roset PN, Segura J, de la Torre R (2004) Paroxetine inhibits acute effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine on the immune system in humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 309: 285-92 Pacifici R, Zuccaro P, Farre M, Pichini S, Di Carlo S, Roset PN, Lopez CH, Ortuno J, Segura J, Cami J, de la Torre R (2000) Immunomodulating activity of MDMA. Ann N Y Acad Sci 914: 215-24 Pacifici R, Zuccaro P, Farre M, Pichini S, Di Carlo S, Roset PN, Ortuno J, Pujadas M, Bacosi A, Menoyo E, Segura J, de la Torre R (2001) Effects of repeated doses of MDMA ("ecstasy") on cell-mediated immune response in humans. Life Sci 69: 2931-41 Pacifici R, Zuccaro P, Farre M, Pichini S, Di Carlo S, Roset PN, Ortuno J, Segura J, de la Torre R (1999) Immunomodulating properties of MDMA alone and in combination with alcohol: a pilot study. Life Sci 65: PL309-16 Pacifici R, Zuccaro P, Farre M, Pichini S, Di Carlo S, Roset PN, Palmi I, Ortuno J, Menoyo E, Segura J, de la Torre R (2002) Cell-mediated immune response in MDMA users after repeated dose administration: studies in controlled versus noncontrolled settings. Ann N Y Acad Sci 965: 421-33 Parrott AC (2004) MDMA (3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine) or ecstasy: the neuropsychobiological implications of taking it at dances and raves. Neuropsychobiology 50: 329-35 Parrott AC, Sisk E, Turner JJ (2000) Psychobiological problems in heavy 'ecstasy' (MDMA) polydrug users. Drug Alcohol Depend 60: 105-10 Patel MM, Belson MG, Wright D, Lu H, Heninger M, Miller MA (2005) Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy)-related myocardial hypertrophy: an autopsy study. Resuscitation 66: 197-202 PDSP (2007) MDMA receptor binding profiles from Psychoactive Drug Screening Program Database. National Institute of Mental Health Peng W, Premkumar A, Mossner R, Fukuda M, Lesch KP, Simantov R (2002) Synaptotagmin I and IV are differentially regulated in the brain by the Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 44 recreational drug 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA). Brain Res Mol Brain Res 108: 94-101. Peroutka SJ, Newman H, Harris H (1988) Subjective effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine in recreational users. Neuropsychopharmacology 1: 273-7 Phelps EA, O'Connor KJ, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Grillon C, Davis M (2001) Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat Neurosci 4: 437-41 Piper BJ, Fraiman JB, Meyer JS (2005) Repeated MDMA ("Ecstasy") exposure in adolescent male rats alters temperature regulation, spontaneous motor activity, attention, and serotonin transporter binding. Dev Psychobiol 47: 145-57 Piper BJ, Meyer JS (2004) Memory deficit and reduced anxiety in young adult rats given repeated intermittent MDMA treatment during the periadolescent period. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 79: 723-31 Pizarro N, Farre M, Pujadas M, Peiro AM, Roset PN, Joglar J, de la Torre R (2004) Stereochemical analysis of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine and its main metabolites in human samples including the catechol-type metabolite (3,4dihydroxymethamphetamine). Drug Metab Dispos 32: 1001-7 Pizarro N, Llebaria A, Cano S, Joglar J, Farre M, Segura J, de la Torre R (2003) Stereochemical analysis of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine and its main metabolites by gas chromatography/mass spectrometry. Rapid Commun Mass Spectrom 17: 330-6 Pizarro N, Ortuno J, Farre M, Hernandez-Lopez C, Pujadas M, Llebaria A, Joglar J, Roset PN, Mas M, Segura J, Cami J, de la Torre R (2002) Determination of MDMA and its metabolites in blood and urine by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry and analysis of enantiomers by capillary electrophoresis. J Anal Toxicol 26: 157-65 Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Hoppe C, Westheide J, Maier W, Daum I, Wagner M (2007) Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making cognition in heavy users of MDMA ("Ecstasy"). Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 517-30 Quinn ST, Guiry PJ, Schwab T, Keenan AK, McBean GJ (2006) Blockade of noradrenaline transport abolishes 4-methylthioamphetamine-induced contraction of the rat aorta in vitro. Auton Autacoid Pharmacol 26: 335-44 Raikos N, Tsoukali H, Psaroulis D, Vassiliadis N, Tsoungas M, Njau SN (2002) Amphetamine derivative related deaths in northern Greece. Forensic Sci Int 128: 31-4 Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KP (2006) Acute effects of 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) on behavioral measures of impulsivity: alone and in combination with alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 1048-55 Reneman L, Majoie CB, Flick H, den Heeten GJ (2002) Reduced N-acetylaspartate levels in the frontal cortex of 3,4- methylenedioxymethamphetamine (Ecstasy) users: preliminary results. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 23: 231-7. Reveron ME, Maier EY, Duvauchelle CL (2006) Experience-dependent changes in temperature and behavioral activity induced by MDMA. Physiol Behav 89: 358- 63 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 45 Ricaurte GA, DeLanney LE, Wiener SG, Irwin I, Langston JW (1988) 5Hydroxyindoleacetic acid in cerebrospinal fluid reflects serotonergic damage induced by 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine in CNS of non-human primates. Brain Res 474: 359-63 Rizzo M, Lamers CT, Sauer CG, Ramaekers JG, Bechara A, Andersen GJ (2005) Impaired perception of self-motion (heading) in abstinent ecstasy and marijuana users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 179: 559-66 Roiser JP, Rogers RD, Cook LJ, Sahakian BJ (2006) The effect of polymorphism at the serotonin transporter gene on decision-making, memory and executive function in ecstasy users and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 188: 213-27 Roiser JP, Rogers RD, Sahakian BJ (2007) Neuropsychological function in ecstasy users: a study controlling for polydrug use. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 505-16 Rudnick G, Wall SC (1992) The molecular mechanism of "ecstasy" [3,4methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (MDMA)]: serotonin transporters are targets for MDMA-induced serotonin release. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 89: 1817-21 Schechter MD (1986) Discriminative profile of MDMA. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 24: 1533-7 Schenk S, Gittings D, Johnstone M, Daniela E (2003) Development, maintenance and temporal pattern of self-administration maintained by ecstasy (MDMA) in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 169: 21-7 Schifano F, Oyefeso A, Corkery J, Cobain K, Jambert-Gray R, Martinotti G, Ghodse AH (2003) Death rates from ecstasy (MDMA, MDA) and polydrug use in England and Wales 1996-2002. Hum Psychopharmacol 18: 519-24 Schilt T, de Win MM, Koeter M, Jager G, Korf DJ, van den Brink W, Schmand B (2007) Cognition in novice ecstasy users with minimal exposure to other drugs: a prospective cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64: 728-36 Segura M, Ortuno J, Farre M, McLure JA, Pujadas M, Pizarro N, Llebaria A, Joglar J, Roset PN, Segura J, de La Torre R (2001) 3,4-Dihydroxymethamphetamine (HHMA). A Major in Vivo 3,4- methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) Metabolite in Humans. Chem Res Toxicol 14: 1203-1208. Selken J, Nichols DE (2007) α1-Adrenergic Receptors Mediate the Locomotor Response to Systemic Administration of (±)-3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav In press: xx-xx Semple DM, Ebmeier KP, Glabus MF, O'Carroll RE, Johnstone EC (1999) Reduced in vivo binding to the serotonin transporter in the cerebral cortex of MDMA ('ecstasy') users. Br J Psychiatry 175: 63-9 Setola V, Hufeisen SJ, Grande-Allen KJ, Vesely I, Glennon RA, Blough B, Rothman RB, Roth BL (2003) 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "Ecstasy") induces fenfluramine-like proliferative actions on human cardiac valvular interstitial cells in vitro. Mol Pharmacol 63: 1223-9 Shulgin A, Shulgin A (1991) Pihkal: A Chemical Love Story, 1st edn. Transform Press, Transform Press Shulgin AT (1986) The background and chemistry of MDMA. J Psychoactive Drugs 18: 291-304 Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 46 Shulgin AT, Nichols DE (1978) Characterization of three new psychotomimetics. In: Stillman RC, Willette RE (eds) The Pharmacology of Hallucinogens. Pergamon, New York Solowij N, Hall W, Lee N (1992) Recreational MDMA use in Sydney: a profile of 'Ecstacy' users and their experiences with the drug. Br J Addict 87: 1161-72 Sotnikova TD, Beaulieu JM, Barak LS, Wetsel WC, Caron MG, Gainetdinov RR (2005) Dopamine-independent locomotor actions of amphetamines in a novel acute mouse model of Parkinson disease. PLoS Biol 3: e271 Spitzer M, Franke B, Walter H, Buechler J, Wunderlich AP, Schwab M, Kovar K, Hermle L, Gron G (2001) Enantio-selective cognitive and brain activation effects of N-ethyl-3,4- methylenedioxyamphetamine in humans. Neuropharmacology 41: 263-71. Stolaroff M (1997) The Secret Chief: Conversations with a pioneer of the underground therapy movement. Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies, Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration OoAS (2007) Drug Abuse Warning Network 2005: National Estimates of Emergency Department Visits DAWN Series D-29,. DHHS, Rockville, MD, Sumnall HR, Cole JC (2005) Self-reported depressive symptomatology in community samples of polysubstance misusers who report Ecstasy use: a meta-analysis. J Psychopharmacol 19: 84-92 Sumnall HR, Cole JC, Jerome L (2006) The varieties of ecstatic experience: an exploration of the subjective experiences of ecstasy. J Psychopharmacol 20: 670- 82 Sumnall HR, Wagstaff GF, Cole JC (2004) Self-reported psychopathology in polydrug users. J Psychopharmacol 18: 75-82 Taffe MA, Lay CC, Von Huben SN, Davis SA, Crean RD, Katner SN (2006) Hyperthermia induced by 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine in unrestrained rhesus monkeys. Drug Alcohol Depend 82: 276-81 Tancer M, Johanson CE (2003) Reinforcing, subjective, and physiological effects of MDMA in humans: a comparison with d-amphetamine and mCPP. Drug Alcohol Depend 72: 33-44 Tancer M, Johanson CE (2007) The effects of fluoxetine on the subjective and physiological effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 565-73 Tancer ME, Johanson CE (2001) The subjective effects of MDMA and mCPP in moderate MDMA users. Drug Alcohol Depend 65: 97-101. Thiriet N, Ladenheim B, McCoy MT, Cadet JL (2002) Analysis of ecstasy (MDMA)induced transcriptional responses in the rat cortex. Faseb J 16: 1887-94. Thomasius R, Petersen K, Buchert R, Andresen B, Zapletalova P, Wartberg L, Nebeling B, Schmoldt A (2003) Mood, cognition and serotonin transporter availability in current and former ecstasy (MDMA) users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 167: 85- 96 Thompson MR, Callaghan PD, Hunt GE, Cornish JL, McGregor IS (2007) A role for oxytocin and 5-HT(1A) receptors in the prosocial effects of 3,4 methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("ecstasy"). Neuroscience Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 47 Topp L, Hando J, Dillon P, Roche A, Solowij N (1999) Ecstasy use in Australia: patterns of use and associated harm. Drug Alcohol Depend 55: 105-15 Trigo JM, Panayi F, Soria G, Maldonado R, Robledo P (2006) A reliable model of intravenous MDMA self-administration in naive mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 184: 212-20 Varela-Rey M, Montiel-Duarte C, Beitia G, Cenarruzabeitia E, Iraburu MJ (1999) 3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("Ecstasy") stimulates the expression of alpha1(I) procollagen mRNA in hepatic stellate cells. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 259: 678-82 Verheyden SL, Henry JA, Curran HV (2003) Acute, sub-acute and long-term subjective consequences of 'ecstasy' (MDMA) consumption in 430 regular users. Hum Psychopharmacol 18: 507-17 Verrico CD, Miller GM, Madras BK (2007) MDMA (Ecstasy) and human dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin transporters: implications for MDMA-induced neurotoxicity and treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 489-503 Vollenweider FX, Gamma A, Liechti M, Huber T (1998a) Psychological and cardiovascular effects and short-term sequelae of MDMA ("ecstasy") in MDMAnaive healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 19: 241-51 Vollenweider FX, Gamma A, Liechti M, Huber T (1999) Is a single dose of MDMA harmless? Neuropsychopharmacology 21: 598-600 Vollenweider FX, Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen MF, Babler A, Vogel H, Hell D (1998b) Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. Neuroreport 9: 3897-902 von Geusau NA, Stalenhoef P, Huizinga M, Snel J, Ridderinkhof KR (2004) Impaired executive function in male MDMA ("ecstasy") users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 175: 331-41 Von Huben SN, Lay CC, Crean RD, Davis SA, Katner SN, Taffe MA (2006) Impact of Ambient Temperature on Hyperthermia Induced by (+/-)3,4Methylenedioxymethamphetamine in Rhesus Macaques. Neuropsychopharmacology von Sydow K, Lieb R, Pfister H, Hofler M, Wittchen HU (2002) Use, abuse and dependence of ecstasy and related drugs in adolescents and young adults-a transient phenomenon? Results from a longitudinal community study. Drug Alcohol Depend 66: 147-59 Wang X, Baumann MH, Xu H, Morales M, Rothman RB (2005) (+/-)-3,4Methylenedioxymethamphetamine administration to rats does not decrease levels of the serotonin transporter protein or alter its distribution between endosomes and the plasma membrane. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 314: 1002-12 Wang Z, Woolverton WL (2007) Estimating the relative reinforcing strength of (+/-)-3,4methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and its isomers in rhesus monkeys: comparison to (+)-methamphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 483-8 Wareing M, Fisk JE, Murphy P, Montgomery C (2004) Verbal working memory deficits in current and previous users of MDMA. Hum Psychopharmacol 19: 225-34 Widmer S (1998) Listening into the heart of things: The awakening of love: On MDMA and LSD: The undesired psychotherapy. . Basic Editions, Basic Editions Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com   Investigator’s Brochure: MDMA MAPS: 12/2007 48 Williams H, Dratcu L, Taylor R, Roberts M, Oyefeso A (1998) "Saturday night fever": ecstasy related problems in a London accident and emergency department. J Accid Emerg Med 15: 322-6 Williams MT, Schaefer TL, Ehrman LA, Able JA, Gudelsky GA, Sah R, Vorhees CV (2005) 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine administration on postnatal day 11 in rats increases pituitary-adrenal output and reduces striatal and hippocampal serotonin without altering SERT activity. Brain Res 1039: 97-107 Wirth MM, Schultheiss OC (2006) Effects of affiliation arousal (hope of closeness) and affiliation stress (fear of rejection) on progesterone and cortisol. Horm Behav 50: 786-95 Xie T, Tong L, McLane MW, Hatzidimitriou G, Yuan J, McCann U, Ricaurte G (2006) Loss of serotonin transporter protein after MDMA and other ring-substituted amphetamines. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 2639-51 Yarosh HL, Katz EB, Coop A, Fantegrossi WE (2007) MDMA-like behavioral effects of N-substituted piperazines in the mouse. Pharmacol Biochem Behav Zakzanis KK, Campbell Z (2006) Memory impairment in now abstinent MDMA users and continued users: a longitudinal follow-up. Neurology 66: 740-1 Zakzanis KK, Campbell Z, Jovanovski D (2007) The neuropsychology of ecstasy (MDMA) use: a quantitative review. Hum Psychopharmacol 22: 427-35 Zakzanis KK, Young DA (2001) Memory impairment in abstinent MDMA ("Ecstasy") users: a longitudinal investigation. Neurology 56: 966-9. Downloaded  from  http://www.tidyforms.com