Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1998. 49:479­502 Copyright 1998 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved HUMAN ABILITIES Robert J. Sternberg and James C. Kaufman Department of Psychology, Yale University, Box 208205, New Haven, Connecticut 06520-8205; e-mail: robert.sternberg@yale.edu; e-mail: james.kaufman@yale.edu KEY WORDS: intelligence, biological approaches, psychometric approaches, cultural approaches, cognitive approaches ABSTRACT This chapter reviews recent literature, primarily from the 1990s, on human abilities. The review opens with a consideration of the question of what intel- ligence is, and then considers some of the major definitions of intelligence, as well as implicit theories of intelligence around the world. Next, the chap- ter considers cognitive approaches to intelligence, and then biological ap- proaches. It proceeds to psychometric or traditional approaches to intelli- gence, and then to broad, recent approaches. The different approaches raise somewhat different questions, and hence produce somewhat different answers. They have in common, however, the attempt to understand what kinds of mechanisms lead some people to adapt to, select, and shape environments in ways that match particularly well the demands of those environments. CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 480 DEFINITIONS OF INTELLIGENCE. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 480 Western Psychological Views. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 480 Cross-Cultural Views . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 481 COGNITIVE APPROACHES TO INTELLIGENCE. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 484 BIOLOGICAL APPROACHES TO INTELLIGENCE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 485 Early Biological Theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 485 Modern Biological Views and Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 485 THE PSYCHOMETRIC APPROACH TO INTELLIGENCE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487 Theoretical Developments: Carroll's and Horn's Theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487 An Empirical Curiosity: The Flynn Effect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 488 Psychometric Tests . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 489 The Bell Curve Phenomenon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 490 BROAD THEORIES OF INTELLIGENCE AND OF KINDS OF INTELLIGENCE . . . . . 492 Multiple Intelligences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 492 Successful Intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 493 0066-4308/98/0201-0479$08.00 479 True Intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 496 The Bioecological Model of Intelligence. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 496 Emotional Intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 497 CONCLUSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 497 INTRODUCTION The study of intelligence is like a real-world Jeopardy game. Curiously, there is more agreement regarding answers than there is regarding what questions these an- swers answer. For example, it is uncontroversial that on conventional tests of intelli- gence, members of certain socially identified racial and ethnic groups differ on av- erage. But what does such a difference show? What question does it answer? Does it answer the question of whether there are differences across groups in intelli- gence, whether the tests are differentially biased for members of different groups, whether different groups have had different educational opportunities, or whether different groups differ on a narrow subset of skills that constitutes only a small part of intelligence, or some other question still? To understand the field of human abili- ties and intelligence, one must consider questions at least as much as answers. The goal of this chapter is to consider some of the main questions being asked and answers being offered today in the field of human abilities, in general, and of human intelligence, in particular, and to consider the match between them. What are the important questions, and what are the questions that available data answer? We organize our review around some of the main paradigms in the study of human abilities, because the paradigm one uses generates, to a large extent, the questions that are viewed as important or not important. Before we consider these theories, however, we first consider even what intelligence is, going back in history and up to the present. DEFINITIONS OF INTELLIGENCE What is intelligence? It turns out that the answer depends on whom you ask, and that the answer differs widely across disciplines, time, and places. We dis- cuss the diversity of views about what intelligence is because empirical studies often assume rather than explore the nature of the construct they are investigat- ing--in this case, intelligence. Western Psychological Views How have Western psychologists conceived of intelligence? Almost none of these views is adequately expressed by Boring's (1923) operationistic view of intelligence as whatever it is that intelligence tests test. This empty and circu- lar definition is still used by some investigators in the field. For example, in a 1921 symposium (Intelligence and Its Measurement: A Symposium) on experts' definitions of intelligence, researchers emphasized 480 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN the importance of the ability to learn and the ability to adapt to the environ- ment. Sixty-five years later, Sternberg & Detterman (1986) conducted a simi- lar symposium, again asking experts their views on intelligence. Learning and adaptive abilities retained their importance, and a new emphasis crept in: me- tacognition, or the ability to understand and control oneself. Of course, the name is new, but the idea is not, because Aristotle emphasized long before the importance for intelligence of knowing oneself. Cross-Cultural Views In some cases, Western notions about intelligence are not shared by other cul- tures. For example, at the mental level, the Western emphasis on speed of men- tal processing (Sternberg et al 1981) is not shared by many cultures. Other cul- tures may even be suspicious of the quality of work done very quickly and may emphasize depth rather than speed of processing. They are not alone: Some prominent Western theorists have pointed out the importance of depth of proc- essing for full command of material (e.g. Craik & Lockhart 1972). Yang & Sternberg (1997a) have reviewed Chinese philosophical concep- tions of intelligence. The Confucian perspective emphasizes the characteristic of benevolence and of doing what is right. As in the Western notion, the intelli- gent person spends much effort in learning, enjoys learning, and persists in life-long learning with enthusiasm. The Taoist tradition, in contrast, empha- sizes the importance of humility, freedom from conventional standards of judgment, and full knowledge of oneself and of external conditions. The difference between Eastern and Western conceptions of intelligence may persist even today. Yang & Sternberg (1997b) studied contemporary Tai- wanese Chinese conceptions of intelligence and found five factors underlying these conceptions: (a) a general cognitive factor, much like the g factor in con- ventional Western tests; (b) interpersonal intelligence; (c) intrapersonal intel- ligence; (d) intellectual self-assertion; and (e) intellectual self-effacement. In a related study but with different results, Chen (1994) found three factors under- lying Chinese conceptualizations of intelligence: nonverbal reasoning ability, verbal reasoning ability, and rote memory. The difference may be due to dif- ferent subpopulations of Chinese, to differences in methodology, or to differ- ences in when the studies were done. The factors uncovered in both studies differ substantially from those identified in US people's conceptions of intelligence by Sternberg et al (1981)--(a) practical problem solving, (b) verbal ability, and (c) social com- petence--although in both cases, people's implicit theories of intelligence seem to go quite far beyond what conventional psychometric intelligence tests measure. Of course, comparing the Chen (1994) study to the Sternberg et al (1981) study simultaneously varies both language and culture. HUMAN ABILITIES 481 Chen & Chen (1988) varied only language. They explicitly compared the concepts of intelligence of Chinese graduates from Chinese-language versus English-language schools in Hong Kong. They found that both groups consid- ered nonverbal reasoning skills as the most relevant skills for measuring intelli- gence. Verbal reasoning and social skills came next, and then numerical skills. Memory was seen as least important. The Chinese-language-schooled group, however, tended to rate verbal skills as less important than did the English-- language-schooled group. Moreover, in an earlier study, Chen et al (1982) found that Chinese students viewed memory for facts as important for intelligence, whereas Australian students viewed these skills as of only trivial importance. Das (1994), also reviewing Eastern notions of intelligence, has suggested that in Buddhist and Hindu philosophies, intelligence involves waking up, no- ticing, recognizing, understanding, and comprehending but also includes such things as determination, mental effort, and even feelings and opinions in addi- tion to more intellectual elements. Differences between cultures in conceptions of intelligence have been rec- ognized for some time. Gill & Keats (1980) noted that Australian University students value academic skills and the ability to adapt to new events as critical to intelligence, whereas Malay students value practical skills, as well as speed and creativity. Dasen (1984) found that Malay students emphasize both social and cognitive attributes in their conceptions of intelligence. The differences between East and West may be due to differences in the kinds of skills valued by the two kinds of cultures (Srivastava & Misra 1996). Western cultures and their schools emphasize what might be called "techno- logical intelligence" (Mundy-Castle 1974), and so things like artificial intelli- gence and so-called smart bombs are viewed, in some sense, as intelligent, or smart. According to this view, intelligence ends up being oriented toward the development and improvement of technology. Western schooling also emphasizes other things (Srivastava & Misra 1996), such as generalization, or going beyond the information given (Connolly & Bruner 1974, Goodnow 1976), speed (Sternberg 1985a), minimal moves to a solution (Newell & Simon 1972), and creative thinking (Goodnow 1976). Moreover, silence is interpreted as a lack of knowledge (Irvine 1978). In con- trast, the Wolof tribe in Africa views people of higher social class and distinc- tion as speaking less (Irvine 1978). This difference between the Wolof and Western notions suggests the usefulness of looking at African notions of intel- ligence and its manifestations in behavior as a possible contrast to US notions. Studies in Africa, in fact, provide yet another window on the substantial dif- ferences. Ruzgis & Grigorenko (1994) have argued that, in Africa, concep- tions of intelligence revolve largely around skills that help to facilitate and maintain harmonious and stable intergroup relations; intragroup relations are 482 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN probably equally important and at times more important. For example, Serpell (1974, 1977, 1982) found that Chewa adults in Zambia emphasize social re- sponsibilities, cooperativeness, and obedience as important to intelligence; in- telligent children are expected to be respectful of adults. Kenyan parents also emphasize responsible participation in family and social life as important as- pects of intelligence (Super & Harkness 1982; CM Super & S Harkness, un- published manuscript). In Zimbabwe, the word for intelligence, ngware, actu- ally means to be prudent and cautious, particularly in social relationships. Among the Baoule, service to the family and community and politeness to- ward and respect for elders are seen as key to intelligence (Dasen 1984). Similar emphasis on social aspects of intelligence has been found as well among two other African groups--the Songhay of Mali and the Samia of Kenya (Putnam & Kilbride 1980). The Yoruba, another African tribe, empha- size the importance of depth--of listening rather than just talking--to intelli- gence, and of being able to see all aspects of an issue and to place the issue in its proper overall context (Durojaiye 1993). The emphasis on the social aspects of intelligence is not limited to African cultures. Notions of intelligence in many Asian cultures also emphasize the so- cial aspect of intelligence more than does the conventional Western or IQ-- based notion (Azuma & Kashiwagi 1987, Lutz 1985, Poole 1985, White 1985). It should be noted that neither African nor Asian notions emphasize exclu- sively social notions of intelligence. In a collaborative study with a number of investigators, Sternberg & Grigorenko (1997b) are currently studying concep- tions of intelligence in rural Kenya. In one village (Kissumu), many and proba- bly most of the children are at least moderately infected with a variety of para- sitic infections. Consequently, they experience stomachaches quite frequently. Traditional medicine suggests the usefulness of a large variety (actually hun- dreds) of natural herbal medicines that can be used to treat such infections. It appears that at least some of these--although perhaps a small percentage--ac- tually work. More important for our purposes, however, is that children who learn how to self-medicate with these natural herbal medicines are viewed as being at an adaptive advantage over those who do not have this kind of infor- mal knowledge. Clearly, the kind of adaptive advantage that is relevant in this culture would be viewed as totally irrelevant in the West, and vice versa. Although these conceptions of intelligence much more emphasize social skills than do conventional US conceptions of intelligence, they simultane- ously recognize the importance of cognitive aspects of intelligence. Note, however, that there is no one overall US conception of intelligence. Okagaki & Sternberg (1993) found that different ethnic groups in San Jose, California, had rather different conceptions of what it means to be intelligent. For exam- ple, Latino parents of schoolchildren tended to emphasize the importance of HUMAN ABILITIES 483 social-competence skills in their conceptions of intelligence, whereas Asian parents tended rather heavily to emphasize the importance of cognitive skills. Anglo parents also more emphasized cognitive skills. Teachers, representing the dominant culture, more emphasized cognitive- rather than social-compe- tence skills. The rank order of children of various groups' performance (includ- ing subgroups within the Latino and Asian groups) could be perfectly pre- dicted by the extent to which their parents shared the teachers' conception of in- telligence. That is, teachers tended to reward those children who were social- ized into a view of intelligence that happened to correspond to the teachers' own. Yet, as we argue below, social aspects of intelligence, broadly defined, may be as important as or even more important than cognitive aspects of intelli- gence in later life. For example, a team that needs to complete a cognitive task may not be able to do so if the members are unable to work together. Some, how- ever, prefer to study intelligence not in its social aspect but in its cognitive one. COGNITIVE APPROACHES TO INTELLIGENCE Cronbach (1957) called for a merging of the two disciplines of scientific psy- chology--the differential and the experimental approaches. Serious responses to Cronbach came in the 1970s, with cognitive approaches to intelligence at- tempting this merger. Hunt et al (1973) introduced the cognitive-correlates ap- proach, whereby scores on laboratory cognitive tests were correlated with scores on psychometric intelligence tests. Sternberg (1977) introduced the cognitive-components approach, whereby performance on complex psycho- metric tasks was decomposed into elementary information-processing compo- nents. Cronbach & Snow (1977; see also Snow 1994) have summarized and synthesized a large literature on aptitude-treatment interaction approaches, whereby instruction and assessment would be tailored to patterns of abilities. In the 1990s, cognitive and biological approaches (discussed next) have be- gun to merge. A prototypical example is the inspection-time task (Nettlebeck 1982; see review by Deary & Stough 1996). In this task, two adjacent vertical lines are presented tachistoscopically or by computer, followed by a visual mask (to destroy the image in visual iconic memory). The two lines differ in length, as do the lengths of time for which the two lines are presented. The sub- ject's task is to say which line is longer. Instead of using raw response time as the dependent variable, however, investigators typically use measures derived from a psychophysical function estimated after many trials. For example, the measure might be the mean duration of a single inspection trial at which 50% accuracy is achieved. Correlations between this task and measures of IQ ap- pear to be about 0.4, a bit higher than is typical in psychometric tasks. There are differing theories about why such correlations are obtained, but such theories 484 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN generally attempt to relate the cognitive function of visual inspection time to some kind of biological function, such as speed of neuronal conduction. Let us consider, then, some of the biological functions that may underlie intelligence. BIOLOGICAL APPROACHES TO INTELLIGENCE An important approach to studying intelligence is to understand it in terms of the functioning of the brain, in particular, and of the nervous system, in gen- eral. Earlier theories relating the brain to intelligence tended to be global in na- ture, although not necessarily backed by strong empirical evidence. Early Biological Theories Halstead (1951) suggested that there are four biologically based abilities, which he called (a) the integrative field factor, (b) the abstraction factor, (c) the power factor, and (d) the directional factor. Halstead attributed all four of these abilities primarily to the functioning of the cortex of the frontal lobes. More influential than Halstead has been Hebb (1949), who distinguished between two basic types of intelligence: Intelligence A and Intelligence B. Hebb's distinction is still used by some theorists today. According to Hebb, In- telligence A is innate potential; Intelligence B is the functioning of the brain as a result of the actual development that has occurred. These two basic types of intelligence should be distinguished from Intelligence C, or intelligence as measured by conventional psychometric tests of intelligence. Hebb also sug- gested that learning, an important basis of intelligence, is built up through cell assemblies, by which successively more and more complex connections among neurons are constructed as learning takes place. A third biologically based theory is that of Luria (1973, 1980), which has had a major impact on tests of intelligence (Kaufman & Kaufman 1983, Na- glieri & Das 1997). According to Luria, the brain comprises three main units with respect to intelligence: (a) a unit of arousal in the brain stem and midbrain structures; (b) a sensory-input unit in the temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes; and (c) an organization and planning unit in the frontal cortex. Modern Biological Views and Research SPEED OF NEURONAL CONDUCTION More recent theories have dealt with more specific aspects of brain or neural functioning. For example, one view has sug- gested that individual differences in nerve-conduction velocity are a basis for indi- vidual differences in intelligence. Two procedures have been used to measure conduction velocity, either centrally (in the brain) or peripherally (e.g. in the arm). Reed & Jensen (1992) tested brain nerve conduction velocities via two medi- um-latency potentials, N70 and P100, which were evoked by pattern-reversal stimulation. Subjects saw a black and white checkerboard pattern in which the HUMAN ABILITIES 485 black squares would change to white and the white squares to black. Over many trials, responses to these changes were analyzed via electrodes attached to the scalp in four places. Correlations of derived latency measures with IQ were small (generally in the 0.1­0.2 range of absolute value), but were significant in some cases, suggesting at least a modest relation between the two kinds of measures. Vernon & Mori (1992) reported on two studies investigating the relation between nerve-conduction velocity in the arm and IQ. In both studies, nerve- conduction velocity was measured in the median nerve of the arm by attaching electrodes to the arm. In the second study, conduction velocity from the wrist to the tip of the finger was also measured. Vernon & Mori found significant correlations with IQ in the 0.4 range, as well as somewhat smaller correlations (around -0.2) with response-time measures. They interpreted their results as supporting the hypothesis of a relation between speed of information transmis- sion in the peripheral nerves and intelligence. However, these results must be interpreted cautiously, as Wickett & Vernon (1994) later tried unsuccessfully to replicate these earlier results. GLUCOSE METABOLISM Some of the most interesting recent work under the biological approach has been done by Richard Haier and his colleagues. For example, Haier et al (1988) showed that cortical glucose metabolic rates as re- vealed by positron emission tomography (PET) scan analysis of subjects solv- ing Raven Matrix problems were lower for more-intelligent than for less- intelligent subjects, suggesting that the more intelligent subjects needed to ex- pend less effort than the less intelligent ones to solve the reasoning problems. A later study (Haier et al 1992) showed a similar result for more- versus less- practiced performers playing the computer game of Tetris. That is, smart peo- ple or intellectually expert people do not have to work as hard as less-smart or intellectually expert people at a given problem. What remains to be shown, however, is the causal direction of this finding. One could sensibly argue that the smart people expend less glucose (as a proxy for effort) because they are smart, rather than that people are smart because they ex- pend less glucose. Or both high IQ and low glucose metabolism may be related to a third causal variable. In other words, we cannot always assume that the bio- logical event is a cause (in the reductionistic sense). It may be, instead, an effect. BRAIN SIZE Another approach considers brain size. Willerman et al (1991) correlated brain size with Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R) IQs, controlling for body size. They found that IQ correlated 0.65 in men and 0.35 in women, with a correlation of 0.51 for both sexes combined. A follow-up analysis of the same 40 subjects suggested that, in men, a relatively larger left hemisphere better predicted WAIS-R verbal than it predicted nonverbal abil- ity, whereas in women a larger left hemisphere predicted nonverbal ability bet- 486 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN ter than it predicted verbal ability (Willerman et al 1992). These brain-size cor- relations are suggestive, but it is difficult to say what they mean at this point. BEHAVIOR GENETICS Another approach that is at least partially biologically based is that of behavior genetics. A fairly complete review of this extensive literature is found in Sternberg & Grigorenko (1997a). The literature is com- plex, but it appears that about half the total variance in IQ scores is accounted for by genetic factors (Loehlin 1989, Plomin 1997). This figure may be an un- derestimate, because the variance includes error variance and because most studies of heritability have been with children, but we know that heritability of IQ is higher for adults than for children (Plomin 1997). In addition, some stud- ies, such as the Texas Adoption Project (Loehlin et al 1997), suggest higher es- timates: 0.78 in the Texas Adoption Project, 0.75 in the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (Bouchard 1997, Bouchard et al 1990), and 0.78 in the Swedish Adoption Study of Aging (Pedersen et al 1992). At the same time, some researchers argue that effects of heredity and envi- ronment cannot be clearly and validly separated (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci 1994, Wahlsten & Gottlieb 1997). Perhaps, the direction for future research is better to figure out how heredity and environment work together to produce phenotypic intelligence (Scarr 1997), concentrating especially on within- family environmental variation, which appears to be more important than between-family variation (Jensen 1997). Such research requires, at the very least, very carefully prepared tests of intelligence--perhaps some of the newer tests described in the next section. THE PSYCHOMETRIC APPROACH TO INTELLIGENCE The psychometric approach to intelligence is among the oldest of approaches, and dates back to Galton's (1883) psychophysical account of intelligence and attempts to measure intelligence in terms of psychophysical abilities (such as strength of hand grip or visual acuity) and later to Binet & Simon's (1916) ac- count of intelligence as judgment, involving adaptation to the environment, di- rection of one's efforts, and self-criticism. Theoretical Developments: Carroll's and Horn's Theories Two of the major new theories proposed during the past decade have been Car- roll's (1993) and Horn's (1994) theories. The two theories are both hierarchi- cal, suggesting more nearly general abilities higher up in the hierarchy and more nearly specific abilities lower in the hierarchy. Carroll's theory will be described briefly as representative of these new developments. Carroll (1993) proposed his hierarchical model of intelligence, based on the factor analysis of more than 460 data sets obtained between 1927 and 1987. HUMAN ABILITIES 487 His analysis encompasses more than 130,000 people from diverse walks of life and even countries of origin (although non-English-speaking countries are poorly represented among his data sets). The model Carroll proposed, based on his monumental undertaking, is a hierarchy comprising three strata: Stratum I, which includes many narrow, specific abilities (e.g. spelling ability, speed of reasoning); Stratum II, which includes various group-factor abilities (e.g. fluid intelligence, involved in flexible thinking and seeing things in novel ways; and crystallized intelligence, the accumulated knowledge base); and Stratum III, which is just a single general intelligence, much like Spearman's (1904) gen- eral intelligence factor. Of these strata, the most interesting is perhaps the middle stratum, which in- cludes, in addition to fluid and crystallized abilities, learning and memory pro- cesses, visual perception, auditory perception, facile production of ideas (simi- lar to verbal fluency), and speed (which includes both sheer speed of response and speed of accurate responding). Although Carroll does not break much new ground, in that many of the abilities in his model have been mentioned in other theories, he does masterfully integrate a large and diverse factor-analytic lit- erature, thereby giving great authority to his model. An Empirical Curiosity: The Flynn Effect We know that the environment has powerful effects on cognitive abilities. Per- haps the simplest and most potent demonstration of this effect is the "Flynn ef- fect" (Flynn 1984, 1987, 1994). The basic phenomenon is that IQ has in- creased over successive generations around the world through most of the cen- tury--at least since 1930. The effect must be environmental, because obvi- ously a successive stream of genetic mutations could not have taken hold and exerted such an effect over such a short period. The effect is powerful--at least 15 points of IQ per generation for tests of fluid intelligence. And it occurs all over the world. The effect has been greater for tests of fluid intelligence than for tests of crystallized intelligence. The difference, if linearly extrapolated (a hazardous procedure, obviously), would suggest that a person who in 1892 fell at the 90th percentile on the Raven Progressive Matrices, a test of fluid intelli- gence, would, in 1992, score at the 5th percentile. There have been many potential explanations of the Flynn effect, and in 1996 a conference was organized by Ulric Neisser and held at Emory University to try to explain the effect. Some of the possible explanations includes increased schooling, greater educational attainment of parents, better nutrition, and less childhood disease. A particularly interesting explanation is that of more and better parental attention to children (see Bronfenbrenner & Ceci 1994). What- ever the answer, the Flynn effect suggests we need to think carefully about the 488 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN view that IQ is fixed. It probably is not fixed within individuals (Campbell & Ramey 1994, Ramey 1994), and it is certainly not across generations. Psychometric Tests STATIC TESTS Static tests are the conventional kind where people are given problems to solve, and are expected to solve them without feedback. Their fi- nal score is typically the number of items answered correctly, sometimes with a penalty for guessing. Psychometric testing of intelligence and related abilities has generally ad- vanced evolutionarily rather than revolutionarily. Sometimes what are touted as advances seem cosmetic or almost beside the point, as in the case of newer versions of the SAT, which are touted to have not only multiple-choice but fill- in-the-blank math problems. Perhaps the most notable trend is a movement to- ward multifactorial theories--often hierarchical ones--and away from the no- tion that intelligence can be adequately understood only in terms of a single gen- eral, or g, factor (e.g. Gustafsson 1988). For example, the third edition of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children (WISC-III; Wechsler 1991) offers scores for four factors (verbal comprehension, perceptual organization, proc- essing speed, and freedom from distractibility), but the main scores remain the verbal, performance, and total scores that have traditionally dominated interpre- tation of the test. The Fourth Edition of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (Thorndike et al 1986) also escapes from the orientation toward general ability that characterized earlier editions, yielding scores for crystallized intelligence, abstract-visual reasoning, quantitative reasoning, and short-term memory. Two new tests also are constructed on the edifice of the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence (Cattell 1971, Horn 1994): the Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test (KAIT; Kaufman & Kaufman 1993; see also Kauf- man & Kaufman 1996) and the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abil- ity­Revised (Woodcock & Johnson 1989; see also Woodcock 1996) (for a re- view of these and other tests, see Daniel 1997). Although the theory is not new, the tendency to base psychometric tests closely on theories of intelligence is a welcome development. The new Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System (Naglieri & Das 1997) is based not on fluid-crystallized theory but rather on the theory of Luria (1973, 1976; see also Das et al 1994), mentioned above. It yields scores for at- tention, planning, simultaneous processing, and successive processing. DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT In dynamic assessment, individuals learn at the time of test. If they answer an item incorrectly, they are given guided feedback to help them solve the item, until they either get it correct or until the examiner has run out of clues to give them. HUMAN ABILITIES 489 The notion of dynamic testing appears to have originated with Vygotsky (1962, 1978) and was developed independently by Feuerstein et al (1985). Dy- namic assessment is generally based on the notion that cognitive abilities are modifiable, and that there is some kind of zone of proximal development (Vy- gotsky 1978), which represents the difference between actually developed ability and latent capacity. Dynamic assessments attempt to measure this zone of proximal development, or an analogue to it. Dynamic assessment is cause both for celebration and for caution (EL Grigorenko & RJ Sternberg, unpublished manuscript). On the one hand, it rep- resents a break from conventional psychometric notions of a more or less fixed level of intelligence. On the other hand, it is more a promissory note than a real- ized success. The Feuerstein test, The Learning Potential Assessment Device (Feuerstein et al 1985), is of clinical use but is not psychometrically normed or validated. There is only one formally normed test available in the United States (Swanson 1996), which yields scores for working memory before and at vari- ous points during and after training, as well as scores for amount of improve- ment with intervention, number of hints that have been given, and a subjective evaluation by the examiner of the examinee's use of strategies. Other tests are perhaps on the horizon (Guthke & Stein 1996), but their potential for stan- dardization and validity, too, remains to be shown. TYPICAL PERFORMANCE TESTS Traditionally, tests of intelligence have been maximum-performance tests, requiring examinees to work the hardest they can to maximize their scores. Ackerman (1994, Ackerman & Heggestad 1997, Goff & Ackerman 1992) has recently argued that typical-performance tests--which, like personality tests, do not require extensive intellectual effort--should supplement maximal-performance ones. On such tests, subjects might be asked to what extent they are characterized by statements like "I prefer my life to be filled with puz- zles I must solve" or "I enjoy work that requires conscientious, exacting skills." A factor analysis of such tests yielded five factors: intellectual engagement, openness, conscientiousness, directed activity, and science/technology interest. Although the trend has been toward multifaceted views of intelligence and away from reliance on general ability, some have bucked this trend. Among those who have are Herrnstein & Murray (1994). The Bell Curve Phenomenon A somewhat momentous event in the perception of the role of intelligence in so- ciety came with the publication of The Bell Curve (Herrnstein & Murray 1994). The impact of the book is shown by the rapid publication of a number of re- sponses. A whole issue of The New Republic was devoted to the book, and two edited books of responses (Fraser 1995, Jacoby & Glauberman 1995) quickly 490 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN appeared. Some of the responses were largely political or emotional in charac- ter, but others attacked the book on scientific grounds. A closely reasoned at- tack appeared a year after these collections (Fischer et al 1996). The American Psychological Association also sponsored a report that, although not directly a response to The Bell Curve, was largely motivated by it (Neisser et al 1996). Some of the main arguments of the book are that (a) conventional IQ tests measure intelligence, at least to a good first approximation; (b) IQ is an impor- tant predictor of many measures of success in life, including school success but also including economic success, work success, success in parenting, avoidance of criminality, and avoidance of welfare dependence; (c) as a result of this pre- diction, people who are high in IQ are forming a cognitive elite, meaning that they are reaching the upper levels of society, whereas those who are low in IQ are falling toward the bottom; (d) tests can and should be used as a gating mecha- nism, given their predictive success; (e) IQ is fairly highly heritable, and hence is passed on through the genes from one generation to the next, with the heritabil- ity of IQ probably in the .5­.8 range; (f) there are racial and ethnic differences in intelligence, with blacks in the United States, for example, scoring about one standard deviation below whites; (g) it is likely, although not certain, that at least some of this difference between groups is due to genetic factors. Herrnstein & Murray attempted to document their claims, using available literature and also their own analysis of the NLSY (National Longitudinal Study of Youth) data that were available to them. Although their book was written for a trade (popular) audience, the book was unusual among books for such an audience in its use of fairly sophisticated statistical techniques. It is not possible here to review the full range of responses to Herrnstein & Murray (1994). Among psychologists, there seems to be fairly widespread agreement that the social-policy recommendations of Herrnstein & Mur- ray--which call for greater isolation of and paternalism toward those with lower IQs--do not follow from their data, but rather represent a separate ideo- logical statement (Neisser et al 1996). Beyond that, there is a great deal of dis- agreement regarding the claims made by these authors. Our own view (Sternberg 1995) is that it would be easy to draw much stronger inferences from the Herrnstein-Murray analysis than the data warrant, and perhaps even than Herrnstein & Murray themselves would support. First, Herrnstein & Murray (1994) acknowledge that, in the United States, IQ typically accounts only for roughly 10% of the variation, on average, in individual differences across the domains of success they survey. Put another way, about 90% of the variation, and sometimes quite a bit more, remains unexplained. Second, even the 10% figure may be inflated by the fact that US society uses IQ-like tests to select, place, and ultimately, to stratify students, so that some of the outcomes that Herrnstein & Murray mention may actually be re- HUMAN ABILITIES 491 sults of the use of IQ-like tests rather than results of individual differences in intelligence per se. For example, admission to selective colleges in the US typically requires students to take either the Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT) or the American College Test (ACT), both of which, for whatever they may be named, are similar (although not identical) in kind to conventional tests of IQ. Admission to graduate and professional programs requires similar kinds of tests. The result is that those who do not test well may be denied access to these programs, and to the routes that would lead them to job, economic, and other socially sanctioned forms of success in our society. It is thus not surprising, in a sense, that test scores would be highly correlated with, say, job status. People who do not test well have difficulty gaining access to high-status jobs, which in turn pay better than other jobs to which they might be able to gain access. If we were to use some other index instead of test scores--for example, social class or economic class--then different people would be selected for the access routes to societal success. In fact, we do use these alternative measures to some degree, although less so than in the past. Finally, although group differences in IQ are acknowledge by virtually all psychologists to be real, the cause of them remains very much in dispute. What is clear is that the evidence in favor of genetic causes is weak and equivocal (Nisbett 1995; Scarr et al 1977; Scarr & Weinberg 1976, 1983). We are cer- tainly in no position to assign causes at this time. Understanding of group dif- ferences requires further analysis and probably requires looking at these dif- ferences through the lens of broader theories of intelligence. BROAD THEORIES OF INTELLIGENCE AND OF KINDS OF INTELLIGENCE During recent years, there has been a trend toward broad theories of intelli- gence. We consider some of the main such theories next. Multiple Intelligences Gardner (1983) proposed that there is no single, unified intelligence but rather a set of relatively distinct, independent, and modular multiple intelligence. His theory of multiple intelligences (MI theory) originally proposed seven multi- ple intelligences: (a) linguistic, as used in reading a book or writing a poem; (b) logical-mathematical, as used in deriving a logical proof or solving a mathemati- cal problem; (c) spatial, as used in fitting suitcases into the trunk of a car; (d) musical, as used in singing a song or composing a symphony; (e) bodily- kinesthetic, as used in dancing or playing football; (f) interpersonal, as used in understanding and interacting with other people; and (g) intrapersonal, as used in understanding oneself. 492 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN Recently, Gardner 1998 has proposed one additional intelligence as a con- firmed part of his theory--naturalist intelligence--the kind shown by people who are able to discern patterns in nature. Charles Darwin would be a notable example. Gardner has also suggested that there may be two other "candidate" intelligences: spiritual intelligence and existential intelligence. Spiritual intel- ligence involves a concern with cosmic or existential issues and the recogni- tion of the spiritual as the achievement of a state of being. Existential intelli- gence involves a concern with ultimate issues. Gardner believes the evidence for these latter two intelligences to be less powerful than the evidence for the other eight intelligences. Whatever the evidence may be for the other eight, we agree that the evidence for these two new intelligences is speculative at this point. As of 1997, there have been no empirical investigations directly testing the validity of Gardner's theory as a whole. In the past, factor analysis served as the major criterion for identifying abilities. Gardner (1983) proposed a new set of criteria, including but not limited to factor analysis, for identifying the existence of a discrete kind of intelligence: (a) po- tential isolation by brain damage, in that the destruction or sparing of a discrete area of the brain may destroy or spare a particular kind of intelligent behavior; (b) the existence of exceptional individuals who demonstrate an extraordinary ability (or deficit) in a particular kind of intelligent behavior; (c) an identifiable core operation or set of operations that are essential to performance of a particular kind of intelligent behavior; (d) a distinctive developmental history leading from novice to master, along with disparate levels of expert performance; (e) a dis- tinctive evolutionary history, in which increases in intelligence may be plausi- bly associated with enhanced adaptation to the environment; (f) supportive evidence from cognitive-experimental research; (g) supportive evidence from psychometric tests; and (h) susceptibility to encoding in a symbol system. Since the theory was first proposed, many educational interventions have arisen that are based on the theory, sometimes closely and other times less so (Gardner 1993). Many of the programs are unevaluated, and evaluations of others of these programs seem still to be ongoing, so it is difficult to say at this point what the results will be. In one particularly careful evaluation of a well- conceived program in a large southern city, there were no significant gains in student achievement or changes in student self-concept as a result of an inter- vention program based on Gardner's (1983) theory (Callahan et al 1997). There is no way of knowing whether these results are representative of such in- tervention programs, however. Successful Intelligence Sternberg (1996) has suggested that we may wish to pay less attention to con- ventional notions of intelligence and more to what he terms successful intelli- HUMAN ABILITIES 493 gence, or the ability to adapt to, shape, and select environments to accomplish one's goals and those of one's society and culture. A successfully intelligent person balances adaptation, shaping, and selection, doing each as necessary. The theory is motivated in part by repeated findings that conventional tests of intelligence and related tests do not predict meaningful criteria of success as well as they predict scores on other similar tests and school grades (e.g. Stern- berg & Williams 1997). Successful intelligence involves an individual's discerning his or her pat- tern of strengths and weaknesses, and then figuring out ways to capitalize upon the strengths and at the same time to compensate for or correct the weaknesses. People attain success, in part, in idiosyncratic ways that involve their finding how best to exploit their own patterns of strengths and weaknesses. Three broad abilities are important to successful intelligence: analytical, creative, and practical abilities. Analytical abilities are required to analyze and evaluate the options avail- able to oneself in life. They include things such as identifying the existence of a problem, defining the nature of the problem, setting up a strategy for solving the problem, and monitoring one's solution processes. Creative abilities are required to generate problem-solving options in the first place. Creative individuals are ones who "buy low and sell high" in the world of ideas (Sternberg & Lubart 1995, 1996): They are willing to generate ideas that, like stocks with low price-earnings ratios, are unpopular and per- haps even depreciated. Having convinced at least some people of the value of these ideas, they then sell high, meaning that they move on to the next unpopu- lar idea. Research shows that these abilities are at least partially distinct from conventional IQ, and that they are moderately domain-specific, meaning that creativity in one domain (such as art) does not necessarily imply creativity in another (such as writing) (Sternberg & Lubart 1995). Practical abilities are required to implement options and to make them work. Practical abilities are involved when intelligence is applied to real-world contexts. A key aspect of practical intelligence is the acquisition and use of tacit knowledge, which is knowledge of what one needs to know to succeed in a given environment that is not explicitly taught and that usually is not verbal- ized. Research shows that tacit knowledge is acquired through mindful utiliza- tion of experience, that it is relatively domain specific, that its possession is relatively independent of conventional abilities, that it predicts criteria of job success about as well as and sometimes better than does IQ (McClelland 1973, Sternberg & Wagner 1993, Sternberg et al 1995). The separation of practical intelligence from IQ has been shown in a number of different ways in a number of different studies. Scribner (1984, 1986) showed that experienced assemblers in a milk-processing plant used complex 494 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN strategies for combining partially filled cases in a manner that minimized the number of moves required to complete an order. Although the assemblers were the least educated workers in the plant, they were able to calculate in their heads quantities expressed in different base number systems, and they rou- tinely outperformed the more highly educated white collar workers who sub- stituted when the assemblers were absent. Scribner found that the order-filling performance of the assemblers was unrelated to measures of academic skills, including intelligence test scores, arithmetic test scores, and grades. Ceci & Liker (1986) carried out a study of expert racetrack handicappers and found that expert handicappers used a highly complex algorithm for pre- dicting post time odds that involved interactions among seven kinds of infor- mation. Use of a complex interaction term in their implicit equation was unre- lated to the handicappers' IQ. In a series of studies, it has been shown that shoppers in California grocery stores were able to choose which of several products represented the best buy for them (Lave et al 1984, Murtaugh 1985), even though they did very poorly on the same kinds of problems when they were presented in the form of a paper-and-pencil arithmetic computation test. The same principle that applies to adults appears to apply to children as well: Carraher et al (1985) found that Brazilian street children who could apply sophisticated mathematical strate- gies in their street vending were unable to do the same in a classroom setting (see also Ceci & Roazzi 1994, Nunes 1994). One more example of a study of practical intelligence was provided by indi- viduals asked to play the role of city managers for the computer-simulated city of Lohhausen (Dorner & Kreuzig 1983, Dorner et al 1983). A variety of prob- lems were presented to these individuals, such as how best to raise revenue to build roads. The simulation involved more than one thousand variables. No re- lation was found between IQ and complexity of strategies used. There is also evidence that practical intelligence can be taught (Gardner et al 1994), at least in some degree. For example, middle-school children given a program for developing their practical intelligence for school (strategies for effective reading, writing, execution of homework, and taking of tests) im- proved more from pretest to posttest than did control students who received an alternative but irrelevant treatment. None of these studies suggests that IQ is unimportant for school or job per- formance or other kinds of performance, and indeed, the evidence suggests to the contrary (Barrett & Depinet 1991, Hunt 1995, Hunter & Hunter 1984, Schmidt & Hunter 1981, Wigdor & Garner 1982). What the studies do suggest, however, is that there are other aspects of intelligence that are relatively inde- pendent of IQ and that are important as well. A multiple-abilities prediction model of school or job performance would probably be most satisfactory. HUMAN ABILITIES 495 According to the theory of successful intelligence, children's multiple abili- ties are underused in educational institutions because teaching tends to value analytical (as well as memory) abilities at the expense of creative and practical abilities. Sternberg et al (1996) designed an experiment to illustrate this point. They identified 199 high school students from around the United States who were strong in either analytical, creative, or practical abilities; all three kinds of abilities; or none of the kinds of abilities. Students were then brought to Yale University to take a college-level psychology course that was taught in a way that emphasized either memory, analytical, creative, or practical abilities. Some students were matched, and others were mismatched, to their own strength(s). All students were evaluated for memory-based, analytical, crea- tive, and practical achievements. Sternberg et al found that students whose instruction matched their pattern of abilities performed significantly better than did students who were mis- matched. They also found that prediction of course performance was improved by taking into account creative and practical as well as analytical abilities. True Intelligence Perkins (1995) has proposed the theory of true intelligence, which he believes synthesizes classic views as well as new ones. According to Perkins, there are three basic aspects to intelligence: neural, experiential, and reflective. According to Perkins, neural intelligence is in the functioning of people's neurological systems, with some people's systems running faster and with more precision than do the neurological systems of others. He mentions "more finely tuned voltages" and "more exquisitely adapted chemical catalysts" as well as a "better pattern of connectivity in the labyrinth of neurons" (Perkins 1995, p. 97), although it is not entirely clear what any of these terms mean. Perkins believes this aspect of intelligence to be largely genetically deter- mined and unlearnable. This kind of intelligence seems to be somewhat similar to Cattell's (1971) idea of fluid intelligence. The experiential aspect of intelligence is what has been learned from expe- rience. It is the extent and organization of the knowledge base, and thus is simi- lar to Cattell's (1971) notion of crystallized intelligence. The reflective aspect of intelligence refers to the role of strategies in mem- ory and problem solving and appears to be similar to the construct of metacog- nition or cognitive monitoring (Brown & DeLoache 1978, Flavell 1981). Ceci (1996) also believes that reflection is important in intelligence. The Bioecological Model of Intelligence Ceci (1996) has proposed a bioecological model of intelligence, according to which multiple cognitive potentials, context, and knowledge are all essential 496 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN bases of individual differences in performance. Each of the multiple cognitive potentials enables relationships to be discovered, thoughts to be monitored, and knowledge to be acquired within a given domain. Although these poten- tials are biologically based, their development is closely linked to environ- mental context, and hence it is difficult if not impossible to separate cleanly biological from environmental contributions to intelligence. Moreover, abili- ties may express themselves very differently in different contexts. For exam- ple, children given essentially the same task in the context of a video game and in the context of a laboratory cognitive task performed much better when the task was presented in the context of the video game. Part of this superiority may have been a result of differences in emotional response, which brings us to the last broader conception we consider. Emotional Intelligence Emotional intelligence is the ability to perceive accurately, appraise, and ex- press emotion; the ability to access and/or generate feelings when they facili- tate thought; the ability to understand emotion and emotional knowledge; and the ability to regulate emotions to promote emotional and intellectual growth (Mayer & Salovey 1997). The concept was introduced by Salovey & Mayer (Mayer & Salovey 1993, Salovey & Mayer 1990) and popularized and ex- panded upon by Goleman (1995). There is some, though still tentative, evidence for the existence of emo- tional intelligence. For example, Mayer & Gehr (1996) found that emotional perception of characters in a variety of situations correlates with SAT scores, with empathy, and with emotional openness. Full convergent-discriminant validation of the construct, however, appears to be needed. CONCLUSION Cultures designate as "intelligent" the cognitive, social, and behavioral attrib- utes that they value as adaptive to the requirements of living in those cultures. To the extent that there is overlap in these attributes across cultures, there will be overlap in the cultures' conceptions of intelligence. Although conceptions of intelligence may vary across cultures, the underlying cognitive attributes probably do not. There may be some variation in social and behavioral attrib- utes. As a result, there is probably a common core of cognitive skills that un- derlies intelligence in all cultures, with the cognitive skills having different manifestations across the cultures. A variety of paradigms has been used to study intelligence. These para- digms are largely complementary rather than contradictory, looking at differ- ent aspects of and questions about intelligence. Many active research pro- HUMAN ABILITIES 497 grams are pursuing answers to these questions. Although there is no one right approach, we believe that the field particularly needs research that expands our notions about what intelligence is. At the same time, we must be cautious about theories that are advanced without direct empirical support. We also must be cautious about how to interpret behavior-genetic studies, which deal with correlations, not means. The Flynn effect shows that whatever the herita- bility of IQ, IQ is highly modifiable, at least across generations. Perhaps the in- creases in IQ that have been observed across generations will one day start to manifest themselves in people's behavior. To date, signs that increases in IQ are reflected in more intelligent everyday behavior have been conspicuous by their absence. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Preparation of this chapter was supported in part under the Javits Act Program (Grant R206R50001) as administered by the Office of Educational Research and Improvement, US Department of Education. The opinions expressed in this chapter do not necessarily reflect the positions or policies of the Office of Educational Research and Improvement or the US Department of Education. Visit the Annual Reviews home page at http://www.AnnualReviews.org. 498 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN Literature Cited Ackerman P. 1994. Intelligence, attention, and learning: maximal and typical perfor- mance. In Current Topics in Human Intel- ligence: Theories of Intelligence, ed. DK Detterman, 4:1­27. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Ackerman PL, Heggestad ED. 1997. Intelli- gence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychol. Bull. 121: 219­45 Azuma H, Kashiwagi K. 1987. Descriptions for an intelligent person: a Japanese study. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 29:17­26 Barrett GV, Depinet RL. 1991. A reconsidera- tion of testing for competence rather than for intelligence. Am. Psychol. 46:1012­24 Binet A, Simon T. 1916. The Development of Intelligence in Children. Transl. ES Kite. Baltimore: Williams Wilkins Boring GG. 1923. Intelligence as the tests test it. New Republic, June 6, pp. 35­37 Bouchard TJ Jr. 1997. IQ similarity in twins reared apart: findings and responses to critics. Intelligence, Heredity, and Envi- ronment, ed. RJ Sternberg, EL Grigorenko, pp. 126­60. New York: Cam- bridge Univ. Press Bouchard TJ Jr, Lykken DT, McGue M, Segal NL, Tellegen A. 1990. Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science 250: 223­28 Bronfenbrenner U, Ceci SJ. 1994. Nature- nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: a bioecological model. Psy- chol. Rev. 101:568­86 Brown AL, DeLoache JS. 1978. Skills, plans, and self-regulation. In Children's Think- ing: What Develops?, ed. R Siegler, pp. 3­35. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Callahan CM, Tomlinson CA, Plucker J. 1997. Project START using a multiple intelli- gences model in identifying and promoting talent in high-risk students. Storrs, CT: Natl. Res. Cent. Gift. Talent., Univ. Conn. Tech. Rep. Campbell FA, Ramey CT. 1994. Effects of HUMAN ABILITIES 499 early intervention on intellectual and aca- demic achievement: a follow-up study of children from low-income families. Child Dev. 65:684­98 Carraher TN, Carraher D, Schliemann AD. 1985. Mathematics in the streets and in schools. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 3:21­29 Carroll JB. 1993. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Cattell RB. 1971. Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action. Boston: Houghton- Mifflin Ceci SJ. 1996. On Intelligence: A Bioecologi- cal Treatise on Intellectual Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press. Ex- panded ed. Ceci SJ, Liker J. 1986. Academic and nonaca- demic intelligence: an experimental sepa- ration. In Practical Intelligence: Nature and Origins of Competence in the Every- day World, ed. RJ Sternberg, RK Wagner, pp. 119­42. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Ceci SJ, Roazzi A. 1994. The effect of context on cognition: postcards from Brazil. Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence, ed. RJ Sternberg, RK Wagner, pp. 74­101. New York: Cam- bridge Univ. Press Chen MJ. 1994. Chinese and Australian con- cepts of intelligence. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 6:101­17 Chen MJ, Braithwaite V, Huang JT. 1982. At- tributes of intelligent behaviour: perceived relevance and difficulty by Australian and Chinese students. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 13:139­56 Chen MJ, Chen HC. 1988. Concepts of intelli- gence: a comparison of Chinese graduates from Chinese and English schools in Hong Kong. Int. J. Psychol. 223:471­87 Connolly H, Bruner J. 1974. Competence: its nature and nurture. In The growth of Com- petence, ed. K Connolly, J Bruner. New York: Academic Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. 1972. Levels of processing: a framework for memory re- search. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 11: 671­84 Cronbach LJ. 1957. The two disciplines of sci- entific psychology. Am. Psychol. 12: 671­84 Cronbach LJ, Snow RE. 1977. Aptitudes and Instructional Methods. New York: Irving- ton Daniel MH. 1997. . Intelligence testing: status and trends. Am. Psychol. In press Das JP. 1994. Eastern views of intelligence. See Sternberg 1994, p. 391 Das JP, Naglieri JA, Kirby JR. 1994. Assess- ment of Cognitive Processes: The PASS Theory of Intelligence. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn Bacon Dasen P. 1984. The cross-cultural study of in- telligence: Piaget and the Baoule. Int. J. Psychol. 19:407­34 Deary I, Stough C. 1996. Intelligence and in- spection time: achievements, prospects, and problems. Am. Psychol. 51:599­608 Dorner D, Kreuzig H. 1983. Problemlosefa- higkeit und intelligenz [Problem solving and intelligence]. Psychol. Rundsch. 34: 185­92 Dorner D, Kreuzig H, Reither F, Staudel T. 1983. Lohhausen: Vom Umgang mit Un- bestimmtheir und Komplexitat. Bern: Hu- ber Durojaiye MOA. 1993. Indigenous psychol- ogy in Africa. In Indigenous Psychologies: Research and Experience in Cultural Con- text, ed. U Kim, JW Berry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Feuerstein R, Rand Y, Haywood HC, Hoffman M, Jensen M. 1985. The Learning Poten- tial Assessment Device (LPAD): Examin- er's Manual. Jerusalem: Hadassah-Wizo- Canada Res. Inst. Fischer CS, Hout M, Sanchez Janowski M, Lucas SR, Swidler A, Voss K. 1996. Ine- quality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press Flanagan DP, Genshaft JL, Harrison PL, eds. 1996. Beyond Traditional Intellectual As- sessment: Contemporary and Emerging Theories, Tests, and Issues. New York: Guilford Flavell JH. 1981. Cognitive monitoring. In Children's Oral Communication Skills, ed. WP Dickson, pp. 35­60. New York: Aca- demic Flynn JR. 1984. The mean IQ of Americans: massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychol. Bull. 95:29­51 Flynn JR. 1987. Massive IQ gains in 14 na- tions: What IQ tests really measure. Psy- chol. Bull. 101:171­91 Flynn JR. 1994. IQ gains over time. See Stern- berg 1994, pp. 617­23 Fraser S, ed. 1995. The Bell Curve Wars. New York: Basic Books Galton F. 1883. Inquiry into Human Faculty and Its Development. London: Macmillan Gardner H. 1983. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York: Basic Books Gardner H. 1993. Multiple Intelligences: The Theory in Practice. New York: Basic Books 500 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN Gardner H. 1998. Are there additional intelli- gences? The case for naturalist, spiritual, and existential intelligences. In Education, Information, and Transformation, ed. J Kane. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Gardner H, Krechevsky M, Sternberg RJ, Okagaki L. 1994. Intelligence in context: enhancing students' practical intelligences for school. In Classroom Lessons: Inte- grating Cognitive Theory and Classroom Practice, ed. K McGilly, pp. 105­27. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Gill R, Keats DM. 1980. Elements of intellec- tual competence: judgments by Australian and Malay university students. J. Cross- Cult. Psychol. 11:233­43 Goff M, Ackerman PL. 1992. Personality- intelligence relations: assessment of typi- cal intellectual engagement. J. Educ. Psy- chol. 84:537­52 Goleman D. 1995. Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books Goodnow JJ. 1976. The nature of intelligent behavior: questions raised by cross- cultural studies. In The Nature of Intelli- gence, ed. L Resnick, pp. 169­88. Hills- dale, NJ: Erlbaum Gustafsson J-E. 1988. Hierarchical models of individual differences in cognitive abili- ties. In Advances in the Psychology of Hu- man Intelligence, ed. RJ Sternberg, 1: 35­71. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Guthke J, Stein H. 1996. Are learning tests the better version of intelligence tests? Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 12:1­13 Haier RJ, Nuechterlein KH, Hazlett E, Wu JC, Paek J. 1988. Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and at- tention studied with positron emission to- mography. Intelligence 12:199­217 Haier RJ, Siegel B, Tang C, Abel L, Buchsbaum MS. 1992. Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose meta- bolic rate following learning. Intelligence 16:415­26 Halstead WC. 1951. Biological intelligence. J. Pers. 20:118­30 Hebb DO. 1949. The Organization of Behav- ior. New York: Wiley Herrnstein RJ, Murray C. 1994. The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press Horn JL. 1994. Fluid and crystallized intelli- gence, theory of. See Sternberg 1994, pp. 443­51 Hunt EB. 1995. Will We Be Smart Enough? New York: Russell Sage Found. Hunt EB, Frost N, Lunneborg C. 1973. Indi- vidual differences in cognition: a new ap- proach to intelligence. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed. G Bower, pp. 87­122. New York: Academic Hunter HE, Hunter RF. 1984. Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job per- formance. Psychol. Bull. 96:72­98 Intelligence and Its Measurement: A Sympo- sium. J. Educ. Psychol. 12:123­47, 195­216, 271­75 Irvine JT. 1978. "Wolof magical thinking": culture and conservation revisited. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 9:300­10 Jacoby R, Glauberman N, eds. 1995. The Bell Curve Debate: History, Documents, Opin- ions. New York: Times Books Jensen AR. 1997. The puzzle of nongenetic variance. In Intelligence, Heredity, and Environment, ed. RJ Sternberg, EL Gri- gorenko, pp. 42­88. New York: Cam- bridge Univ. Press Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. 1983. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC). Circle Pines, MN: Am. Guid. Serv. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. 1993. Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: Am. Guid. Serv. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. 1996. The Kauf- man adolescent and adult intelligence test (KAIT). See Flanagan et al 1996, pp. 209­29 Lave J, Murtaugh M, de la Roche O. 1984. The dialectic of arithmetic in grocery shop- ping. See Rogoff & Lave 1984, pp. 67­94 Loehlin JC. 1989. Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral de- velopment. Am. Psychol. 44:1285­92 Loehlin JC, Horn JM, Willerman L. 1997. He- redity, environment, and IQ in the Texas Adoption Project. See Sternberg & Grigor- enko 1997a, pp. 105­25 Luria AR. 1973. The Working Brain. New York: Basic Books Luria AR. 1976. Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations. Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press Luria AR. 1980. Higher Cortical Functions in Man. New York: Basic Books. 2nd ed. Lutz C. 1985. Ethnopsychology compared to what? Explaining behaviour and con- sciousness among the Ifaluk. See White & Kirkpatrick 1985, pp. 35­79 Mayer JD, Gehr G. 1996. Emotional intelli- gence and the identification of emotion. Intelligence 22:89­114 Mayer JD, Salovey P. 1993. The intelligence of emotional intelligence. Intelligence 17: 433­42 Mayer JD, Salovey P. 1997. What is emotional intelligence? See Salovey & Sluyter 1997, pp. 3­31 McClelland DC. 1973. Testing for compe- HUMAN ABILITIES 501 tence rather than for "intelligence." Am. Psychol. 28:1­14 Mundy-Castle AC. 1974. Social and Techno- logical Intelligence in Western or Non- western Cultures. Universitas 4:46­52 Murtaugh M. 1985. The practice of arithmetic by American grocery shoppers. Anthropol. Educ. Q. Fall Naglieri J, Das JP. 1997. Das-Naglieri Cogni- tive Assessment System (CAS). Itasca, IL: Riverside Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJ, Boykin AW, Brody N, et al. 1996. Intelligence: knowns and unknowns. Am. Psychol. 51: 77­101 Nettlebeck T. 1982. Inspection time: an index for intelligence? Q. Exp. Psychol. 34A: 299­312 Newell A, Simon HA. 1972. Human Problem Solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Hall Nisbett R. 1995. Race, IQ, and scientism. In The Bell Curve Wars: Race, Intelligence and the Future of America, ed. S Fraser, pp. 36­57. New York: Basic Books Nunes T. 1994. Street intelligence. See Stern- berg 1994, 2:1045­49 Okagaki L, Sternberg RJ. 1993. Parental be- liefs and children's school performance. Child Dev. 64:36­56 Pedersen NL, Plomin R, Nesselroade JR, McClearn GE. 1992. A quantitative ge- netic analysis of cognitive abilities during the second half of the life span. Psychol. Sci. 3:346­53 Perkins DN. 1995. Outsmarting IQ: The Emerging Science of Learnable Intelli- gence. New York: Free Press Plomin R. 1997. Identifying genes for cogni- tive abilities and disabilities. See Stern- berg & Grigorenko 1997a, pp. 89­104 Poole FJP. 1985. Coming into social being: cultural images of infants in Bimin- Kuskusmin folk psychology. See White & Kirkpatrick 1985, pp. 183­244 Putnam DB, Kilbride PL. 1980. A relativistic understanding of social intelligence among the Songhay of Mali and Smaia of Kenya. Presented at Meet. Soc. Cross- Cult. Res., Philadelphia, PA Ramey CT. 1994. Abecedarian project. See Sternberg 1994, pp. 1­3 Reed TE, Jensen AR. 1992. Conduction veloc- ity in a brain nerve pathway of normal adults correlates with intelligence level. Intelligence 16:259­72 Rogoff B, Lave J, eds. 1984. Everyday Cogni- tion: Its Development in Social Context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press Ruzgis P, Grigorenko EL. 1994. Cultural meaning systems, intelligence, and per- sonality. In Personality and Intelligence, ed. RJ Sternberg, P Ruzgis, pp. 248­70. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Salovey P, Mayer JD. 1990. Emotional intelli- gence. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 9:185­211 Salovey P, Sluyter D. 1997. Emotional Devel- opment and Emotional Intelligence: Edu- cational Implications. New York: Basic Books Scarr S. 1997. Behavior-genetic and socializa- tion theories of intelligence: truce and rec- onciliation. See Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1997a, pp. 3­41 Scarr S, Pakstis AJ, Katz SH, Barker WB. 1977. Absence of a relationship between degree of white ancestry and intellectual skill in a black population. Hum. Genet. 39:69­86 Scarr S, Weinberg RA. 1976. IQ test perform- ance of black children adopted by white families. Am. Psychol. 31:726­39 Scarr S, Weinberg RA. 1983. The Minnesota adoption studies: genetic differences and malleability. Child Dev. 54:260­67 Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. 1981. Employment testing: old theories and new research find- ings. Am. Psychol. 36:1128­37 Scribner S. 1984. Studying working intelli- gence. See Rogoff & Lave 1984, pp. 9­40 Scribner S. 1986. Thinking in action: some characteristics of practical thought. In Practical Intelligence: Nature and Origins of Competence in the Everyday World, ed. RJ Sternberg, RK Wagner, pp. 13­30. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Serpell R. 1974. Aspects of intelligence in a developing country. Afr. Soc. Res. 17: 578­96 Serpell R. 1977. Strategies for investigating intelligence in its cultural context. Q. Newsl. Inst. Compar. Hum. Dev. 1:11­15 Serpell R. 1982. Measures of perception, skills, and intelligence. In Review of Child Development Research, ed. WW Hartup, 6:392­440. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press Snow RE. 1994. A person-situation interac- tion theory of intelligence in outline. In In- telligence, Mind, and Reasoning: Struc- ture and Development, ed. A Demetriou, A Efklides, pp. 11­28. Amsterdam: Elsevier Spearman C. 1904. General intelligence, ob- jectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 15:201­93 Srivastava AK, Misra G. 1996. Changing per- spectives on understanding intelligence: an appraisal. Indian Psychol. Abstr. Rev. 3:1­34 Sternberg RJ. 1977. Intelligence, Information Processing, and Analogical Reasoning: 502 STERNBERG & KAUFMAN The Componential Analysis of Human Abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Sternberg RJ. 1985a. Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Sternberg RJ. 1985b. Implicit theories of intel- ligence, creativity, and wisdom. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49:607­27 Sternberg RJ. 1990. Metaphors of Mind: Con- ceptions of the Nature of Intelligence. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Sternberg RJ, ed. 1994. Encyclopedia of Hu- man Intelligence. New York: Macmillan Sternberg RJ. 1995. For whom the Bell Curve tolls: a review of The Bell Curve. Psychol. Sci. 6:257­61 Sternberg RJ. 1996. Successful Intelligence. New York: Simon Schuster Sternberg RJ, Conway BE, Ketron JL, Bern- stein M. 1981. People's conceptions of in- telligence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 41:37­55 Sternberg RJ, Detterman DK, eds. 1986. What Is Intelligence? Contemporary Viewpoints on Its Nature and Definition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Sternberg RJ, Ferrari M, Clinkenbeard PR, Grigorenko EL. 1996. Identification, in- struction, and assessment of gifted chil- dren: a construct validation of a triarchic model. Gifted Child Q. 40:129­37 Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL, eds. 1997a. In- telligence, Heredity, and Environment. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL. 1997b. The cognitive costs of physical and mental ill health: Applying the psychology of the de- veloped world to the problems of the de- veloping world. Eye on Psi Chi. In press Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI. 1995. Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity. New York: Free Press Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI. 1996. Investing in creativity. Am. Psychol. 51:677­88 Sternberg RJ, Wagner RK. 1993. The g-ocen- tric view of intelligence and job perform- ance is wrong. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2:1­4 Sternberg RJ, Wagner RK, Williams WM, Horvath J. 1995. Testing common sense. Am. Psychol. 50:912­27 Sternberg RJ, Williams WM. 1997. Does the Graduate Record Examination predict meaningful success in the graduate train- ing of psychologists?: A Case Study. Am. Psychol. 52:630­41 Super CM, Harkness S. 1982. The infants' niche in rural Kenya and metropolitan America. In Cross-Cultural Research at Issue, ed. LL Adler, pp. 47­55. New York: Academic Swanson HL. 1996. Swanson Cognitive Pro- cessing Test. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Sattler JM. 1986. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Fourth Edition. Itasca, IL: Riverside Vernon PA, Mori M. 1992. Intelligence, reac- tion times, and peripheral nerve conduc- tion velocity. Intelligence 8:273­88 Vygotsky LS. 1962. (1934). Thought and Lan- guage. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Vygotsky LS. 1978. Mind in Society. Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press Wahlsten D, Gottlieb G. 1997. The invalid separation of effects of nature and nurture: lessons from animal experimentation. See Sternberg & Grigorenko 1997a, pp. 163­92 Wechsler D. 1991. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children--Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. White GM. 1985. Premises and purposes in a Solomon Islands ethnopsychology. See White & Kirkpatrick 1985, pp. 328­66 White GM, Kirkpatrick J, eds. 1985. Person, Self, and Experience: Exploring Pacific Ethnopsychologies. Berkeley: Univ. Calif. Press Wickett JC, Vernon PA. 1994. Peripheral nerve conduction velocity, reaction time, and intelligence: an attempt to replicate Vernon and Mori. Intelligence 18:127­32 Wigdor AK, Garner WR, eds. 1982. Ability Testing: Uses, Consequences, and Contro- versies. Washington, DC: Nat. Acad. Press Willerman L, Schultz R, Rutledge JN, Bigler ED. 1991. In vivo brain size and intelli- gence. Intelligence 15:223­28 Willerman L, Schultz R, Rutledge JN, Bigler ED. 1992. Hemispheric size asymmetry predicts relative verbal and nonverbal in- telligence differently in the sexes: an MRI study of structure-function relations. Intel- ligence 16:315­28 Woodcock RW. 1996. The Woodcock- Johnson tests of cognitive ability--Re- vised. See Flanagan et al 1996, pp. 230­46 Woodcock RW, Johnson MB. 1989. Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability--Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Yang S-Y, Sternberg RJ. 1997a. Conceptions of intelligence in ancient Chinese philoso- phy. J. Theor. Philos. Psychol. In press Yang S-Y, Sternberg RJ. 1997b. Taiwanese conceptions of intelligence. Submitted