III LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 1 PHONEMIC CONTENT AND PHONEMIC SYSTEM If all the above-stated rules are applied correctly, a complete inventory of all phonemes of a given language can be established. But the phonemic content of each individual phoneme must be determined as well. By phonemic content we understand all phonologically distinctive properties of a phoneme, that is, those properties which are common to all variants of a phoneme and which distinguish it from all other phonemes of the same language, especially from those that are most closely related. German "k" cannot be defined as "velar" because only some of its variants possess this property. Before / and <7, for example, "/<-" is realized as palatal. A definition of German "k" as "dorsal" (a sound produced with the dorsum of the tongue), on the other hand, would also be inadequate since "g" as well as "c/?" are "dorsal." The phonemic content of the German phoneme k can only be formulated as follows: "tense nonnasalized dorsal occlusive." In other words, only the following properties are distinctive for the German phoneme k: (1) complete occlusion (as opposed to "c/j"); (2) blocking of the entrance to the nasal cavity (as opposed to "ng"); (3) tightening of the muscles of the tongue and simultaneous relaxation of the muscles of the larynx (as opposed to "g"); (4) participation of the dorsum (as opposed to "t" and "p"). k shares the first of these four characteristics with /, p, tz, pf, d, b, g, m, n, and ng; the second with g, t, d, p, and h; the 66 third with p, t, ss, andf; and the fourth with g, ch, and ng. Only the sum of all four marks is characteristic for k alone. From what has been said it is evident that the determination of the phonemic content of a phoneme presupposes its prior classification in the system of distinctive oppositions existing in the language in question. The definition of the content of a phoneme depends on what position this phoneme takes in the given phonemic system, that is, in final analysis, with which other phonemes it is in opposition. A phoneme can therefore sometimes be defined in purely negative terms. For example, if one considers all the optional and combinatory variants of the German phoneme /•, the only way in which this phoneme can be defined is as a "nonlateral liquid." This is a purely negative definition since a "liquid" itself is a "nonnasal sonorant," and a "sono-rant" is a "nonobstruent." 2 CLASSIFICATION OF OPPOSITIONS A On the Basis of Their Relationship to the Entire System of Oppositions: Multilateral and Bilateral, Isolated and Proportional Oppositions; and the Structure of the Phonemic Systems Based Thereon The phonemic inventory of a language is actually only a corollary of the system of distinctive oppositions. It should always be remembered that in phonology the major role is played, not by the phonemes, but by the distinctive oppositions. Each phoneme has a definable phonemic content only because the system of distinctive oppositions shows a definite order or 67 68 logical classification of distinctive oppositions logical classification of distinctive oppositions 69 structure. In order to understand this structure, various types of distinctive oppositions must be studied. Above all, certain notions must be introduced which are of decisive importance, not only for phonological systems of oppositions but for any other kind as well.1 An opposition not only presupposes those properties by which the opposition members are distinguished from each other, but also those properties that are common to both opposition members. Tfie latter properties may be termed "the basis for comparison." Two things that have no basis for comparison, that do not have a single property in common, as, for example, an inkpot and free will, do not form an opposition. In a system of oppositions such as the phonological system of a language, two types of oppositions are to be distinguished: bilateral and multilateral. In the case of bilateral oppositions the basis for comparison, that is, the sum of the properties common to both opposition members, is common to these two opposition members alone. It does not recur in any other member of the same system. The basis for comparison of a multilateral opposition, on the other hand, is not limited exclusively to the two respective opposition members. It also extends to other members of the same system. The difference between bilateral and multilateral oppositions can be illustrated by examples from the Latin alphabet. The opposition of the letters £ and F is bilateral because the sum of the features common to these two letters—a vertical bar and two horizontal lines extending to the right, the one extending from the upper end of the bar, the other from its middle—does not recur in any other letter of the Latin alphabet. The opposition of the letters P and R, on the other hand, is multilateral, because the sum of the features that both letters have in common, that is, a loop toward the right on the upper end of a vertical bar, in addition to its occurrence in these two letters, also occurs in the letter B. The distinction between bilateral and multilateral oppositions is extremely important for the general theory of oppositions. It can be made in any system of oppositions and so, of course, also in systems of distinctive oppositions (or phoneme inventories). For example, the opposition t-d is bilateral in German because / and d are the only dental occlusives of the German phonemic system. The opposition d-b, on the other hand, is multilateral in German because the weak occlusion that the two phonemes have in common also recurs in another German phoneme, namely g. Consequently, every distinctive opposition can be recognized quite accurately and clearly as being bilateral or multilateral. Of course, only the phonologically distinctive properties are to be considered. However, some i. nondistinctive properties may be taken into consideration as well if, on the basis of these properties, the members of the opposition in question are placed in opposition with other phonemes of the same system. For example, the opposition d-n (as in French) is to be considered bilateral because its members are the only voiced dental occlusives. Yet neither voicing nor occlusion is distinctive for n, as neither \oiceless nor spirantal /; occur as independent phonemes in the respective system. In every system of oppositions the multilateral oppositions outnumber the bilateral ones. The consonantal systc.ii of stage German, for example, contains twenty consonant phonemes (o, cli, d, j\ g, h, k, I, in, n, ng, p, pf, r, ss, s, sch, t, it-, and tz)* and consequently one hundred and ninety possible oppositions. Of these, only thirteen are bilateral (b-p, d-t, g-k, b-m, d-n, g-ng, pf-f, k-ch, tz-ss,f-w, ss-.s, x\-.sch, and r-l). All the others, that is, 93 percent of the entire system, are multilateral. There are phonemes that do not take part in any bilateral oppositions: among the German consonants, /; is such a phoneme. But every phoneme must take part in multilateral oppositions. Among the oppositions in which a specific phoneme participates, the multilateral oppositions are always more numerous than the bilateral ones. Every German consonant phoneme participates in nineteen oppositions. At the most, only two of these are bilateral. But it is precisely the bilateral oppositions that are the most important for the determination of the phonemic content of a phoneme. Consequently, despite their relatively low number, bilateral oppositions play an important role in the structure of phonological systems. Within the multilateral oppositions homogeneous and heterogeneous oppositions are to be distinguished. Homogeneous oppositions are those multilateral oppositions whose members can be conceived of as the outermost points in a "chain."2 For example, in German the opposition u-e is multilateral: both phonemes, have only in common that they are vowels. This property is not limited to them alone, but is also shared by a whole number of other German phonemes, namely, by all vowels. The members of the opposition u-e are nevertheless to be conceived of as the outermost points of the chain u-o, o-d, 6-e, consisting entirely of bilateral oppositions: in the German vowel system u and o arc the only back rounded vowels, o * Translator's note: The letters of the alphabet rather than phonetic symbols arc used—which is slightly confusing Mi = phonetic symbol "x," ng = "rj," ss = "s," s = "z,"scli = "s,"w = "v"). The German consonant phonemes in phonetic transcription thus are: p, t, k, b, d, g, p, c,f, S, s, x, h, i: -~. (r), in, n, n, /, and r. As regards "y," see footnote 3. :, not mentioned above, occurs loanwords. 70 LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 71 and o the only rounded vowels with a mid-degree of aperture, and 6 and e the only front vowels with a mid-degree of aperture. The opposition u-e is therefore homogeneous. Also homogeneous is the multilateral opposition x-ij ("ch-ng") of the German consonant system: it can be analyzed into a chain of the following bilateral oppositions: x-k, k-g, g-ij. The multilateral opposition p-t, on the other hand, is heterogeneous since there are no phonemes between p and / that could be conceived of as standing in a relation of bilateral opposition to these two phonemes as well as to each other. It is clear that with respect to the total phonemic system of a language, the heterogeneous multilateral oppositions must always be more numerous than the homogeneous ones. But homogeneous oppositions are very important for the determination of the phonemic content of a phoneme and consequently also for the entire structure of a given phonemic system. Two types of homogeneous multilateral oppositions can be distinguished, depending on whether the opposition members can be related to each other by means of one or several "chains" of bilateral oppositions. These are the linear and the nonlinear oppositions. Of the two exajnples given above, the opposition x-ij is linear because the "chain" x-k-g-ij is the only possible one in the framework of the German phonemic syste'm. The opposition n-e, on the other hand, is nonlinear because the "path" from u to e within the German phonemic system can be conceived of via several "chains" of bilateral oppositions {u-o-d-e, u-ii-o-e, u-ii-i-e, or u-o-a-ci-e). Of no less importance than the distinction between bilateral and multilateral oppositions is the distinction between proportional and isolated oppositions. An opposition is proportional if the relation between its members is identical with the relation between the members of another opposition or several other oppositions of the same system. For example, the opposition p-h in German is proportional because the relation between p and h is identical with that between / and cl or between k and g. The opposition p-s, on the other hand, is isolated because the German phonemic system does not have any other pair of phonemes whose members would be related to each other in the same way as p is related to s. The distinction between proportional and isolated oppositions can exist in the case of bilateral as well as multilateral oppositions: in German, for example, the opposition p-b is bilateral and proportional, r-l bilateral and isolated, p-t multilateral and proportional (see b-cl, m-ri), and p-s multilateral and isolated. In every system the isolated oppositions are more numerous than the proportional ones. In the German consonant system, for example, only 40 oppositions are proportional and 150 (i.e., 80 percent) are isolated. They are distributed as follows: bilateral proportional 11 (67,) bilateral isolated 2 (170) multilateral proportional 29 (157,,) multilateral isolated 148 (78%). This means that the bilateral oppositions are predominantly proportional, the multilateral oppositions predominantly isolated. The absolute figures, of course, vary from language to language. In principle, however, the ratio remains the same everywhere: the largest group is formed by isolated multilateral oppositions, the smallest by isolated bilateral oppositions. The proportional oppositions are found between these extreme points, with multilateral oppositions always outnumbering the bilateral ones. What is important for the characterization of a given system is not so much the numerical ratio between the various opposition classes, as the percentage of phonemes participating in each of these classes. The German consonant phonemes include only a single phoneme, /;, which participates exclusively in an isolated multilateral opposition; three phonemes participate only in an isolated bilateral opposition, namely, s, r, and /; all the rest (i.e., 80 percent of all consonant phonemes) participate simultaneously in bilateral and multilateral proportional oppositions. In Russian the consonants that take part in proportional oppositions constitute 88 percent, in Burmese as much as 97 percent. Even more important is the ratio of the number of bilateral proportional oppositions to the number of phonemes participating in these oppositions. While 16 phonemes take part in 11 bilateral proportional oppositions in the German consonant system, 30 consonant phonemes participate in 27 of these oppositions in Russian, and 60 consonant phonemes in 79 such oppositionsin Burmese.* If the number of bilateral proportional oppositions is divided by the number of participant phonemes, one obtains 0.69 for the German consonant system, 0.90 for the Russian, and 1.32 for the Burmese. These different types of oppositions determine the inner order or structure of the phonemic inventory as a system of distinctive oppositions. All proportional oppositions that show identical relations between their members can be combined into a "proportion," hence the term "proportional." For example, in German b-d = p-t = ni-n, or u-o = ii-ii = i-e. * Translator's note: In "Outline of Burmese Grammar" by W. S. Cornyn, Lang. Diss. No. .18, published by the Linguistic Society of America, 1944, the total number of phonemes in Burmese is given as 29 consonants, 9 vowels, and 4 tones. 72 LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE Oi OS1TIONS 73 On the other hand, we have already mentioned those "chains" of bilateral oppositions which can be interpolated between the members of the homogeneous, and in particular of the linear-homogeneous, multilateral oppositions: in German, for example, x-k-g-ij or u-ii-i. If one of the oppositions of such a "chain" is proportional, the "chain" intersects with a "proportion." If a phoneme participates simultaneously in several proportional oppositions, several "proportions" intersect. A phonemic system can therefore be represented in the form of a series of intersecting-parallels. In the German consonant system the proportions b-d — p-t — m-n, b-p = cl-l, and b-m = d-n form an intersection that can be represented in the form of two parallel chains: p-b-m and t-d-n. The proportions p-b = t-d — k-g and b-m = d-n = g-tj result in the parallelism of the chains p-b-m and t-d-n with k-g-ij. However, the last chain can be augmented by an additional member. It then takes on the shape x-k-g-y. The relation x-k (stricture/occlusion) is identical in essence with the relations f-p and s-c which themselves are only a section of the parallel chains v-f-p and z-s-c. Finally, s is simultaneously a member of the bilateral isolated opposition s-s. Thus the following picture emerges: P b v z X f s t k p c d g n rj This comprises seventeen phonemes, that is 85 percent of the entire consonant system of German. Outside this scheme are the phonemes r and / which, as the only liquids of German, form an isolated bilateral opposition; and, too, the phoneme h, which stands exclusively in a relation of multilateral isolated opposition to all the other consonants.3 The order achieved by dividing phonemes into parallel rows does not exist only on paper and is not only a matter of graphics. It corresponds to phonological reality. Due to the fact that a specific relation between two phonemes obtains in several proportional oppositions, the relation itself can now be thought of and used phonologically independently of individual phonemes. As a result, the particular properties of the respective phonemes are recognized as such with particular clarity and the phonemes can be more readily dissolved into their phonological features. It is a basic fact of phonology that the phonemic content of a phoneme depends on the position of this phoneme in the phonemic system and consequently on the structure of that system. Since the systems of distinctive oppositions differ from language to language and from dialect to dialect, the phonemic content of the phoncr.ies also varies according to language and dialect. This difference can also affect the realization of the phonemes. The phoneme r in the various languages n,.ty serve as an illustration. As we have seen, German r stands in a relahon of bilateral opposition only to /. Its phonemic content is very poor, actually purely negative: it is not a vowel, not a specific obstruent, not a ni...al, nor an /. Consequently it also varies greatly with respect to its realization. Before vowels it is dental vibrant for some speakers of German, a uvular vibrant for others For still others it is some sort of almost noiseless guttural spirant. In positions other than before vowels it is generally pronounced either as a nonsyllabic vowel of indeterminate quality, or as an incompletely articulated guttural, only rarely as a weak vibrant. Czech r has a much richer phonemic content, as it stands in a relation of bilateral opposition not only to / but also to a special Czech phoneme f: r and / are the only liquids, r and /"the only two vibrants of Czech, r is distinguished from in that it is not an obstruent but a liquid; from / in that it is a vibrant. For this reason, Czech r is always, and in all positions, pronounced as a clear and energetically trilled sonorant. In contrast with t'c German r, it cannot be "slurred over." A uvular pronunciation is unpopular for Czech r since the opposition r-f would thereby lose some of its distinctness. Czech /• is normally dental (i.e., it is an alveolar r). A uvular /■ only occurs as an extremely rare individual variant and is considered inc. rrect.4 The /• phoneme of Gilyak, a language that is spoken in Eastern Siberia at the mouth of the Amur and in the northern part of Sakhalin Island, presents quite a different picture.5 In addition to voiced r, this language also has a voiceless j with clear friction. Since this j is considered a voiceless spirant, the opposition r-i is not only bilateral but also proportional, and forms a proportion with the oppositions v-f, z-s, y-x, y-x. Consequently r in this case is regarded as a voiced spirant. When the r in Gilyak is articulated energetically, especially when it occurs in gemination, a f-tvpe friction can clearly be heard. This can never be the case with Czech /■ since it might then be confused with r. Furthermore, the oppositions v-f, :-s, y-x, y-x are linked with the chains b-p-p\ 3-c-c\ g-k-k\ g-k-k'. In parallel manner r-i arc also linked with d-t-t\ Thus, the following diagram emerges: d b 3 g g t p c k k /' p' c' k' \- r v z y y u f s x x 74 LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OP DISTINCTIVh OPPOSITIONS LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DIS'ITNCMVL OPPOSITIONS 75 A uvular pronunciation of Gilyak r is therefore completely out of the question. It is always realized as a dental. The phonemic content of Gilyak r is thus "a voiced continuant of the dental series." Since Gilyak also has an /, r must be pronounced as a distinct vibrant. Last in this series of examples, the Japanese r may be discussed. This is the only liquid of the Japanese phonemic system. It stands in a relation of bilateral opposition with only one phoneme, namely, palatalized r'. But since all Japanese consonants have palatalized equivalents, palatalization cannot be considered a specific peculiarity off. Japanese r must therefore.be defined as a "nonpalatal liquid." (The term liquid refers to a consonant phoneme that is neither an obstruent nor a nasal.) The realization of this phoneme is therefore rather indeterminate. / sometimes occurs as an optional variant, but even when this is not the case, r cannot be vigorously trilled since by so doing it would acquire too distinct a character, r is mostly realized as a single "flap of the tongue." A uvular articulation is impossible, as this might place in jeopardy the proportionality of the opposition r-r'. The number of examples could be increased ad infinitum, and illustrations could be taken from many other languages to show the dependence of the phonemic content of the phoneme r on its position in the phonemic system and, therefore, on the structure of this system. And in most cases the phonetic realization of r, the number of its variants, etc., can be deduced from its phonemic content. Any other phoneme could be chosen instead of r. The result would remain unchanged. In summary, one can say that the phonemic content of a phoneme depends on the structure of the corresponding phonemic system. And since phonemic systems are structured differently in every language and even in every dialect, it is relatively rare to find a phoneme with exactly the same phonemic content in two different languages. One must not be misguided by the use of common international symbols of transcription. These symbols are only useful expedients. If the same letters should only be used for phonemes with fully equivalent phonemic content, a separate alphabet would have to be used for every language. B Classification of Oppositions on the Basis of the Relation between Opposition Members: Privative, Gradual, and Equipollent Oppositions The structure of a phonemic system depends on the distribution of the bilateral, multilateral, proportional, and isolated oppositions. The division of oppositions into these four classes is therefore of importance. The principles of classification then relate to the phonemic system: whether an opposition is bilateral or multilateral depends on whether the properties shared by the opposition members in question are common to these members alone or recur in still other members of the same system. Whether an opposition is proportional or isolated depends on whether or not the same relation of opposition recurs in still other oppositions of the same system.6 But the different types of phonological oppositions can also be classified without consideration of the respective system by establishing a principle of classification based on the purely logical relations obtaining between two opposition members. Such a classification is of no importance for the purely external structure of the phonemic inventory. It becomes very important, however, from the standpoint of the function of the phonemic systems. In regard to the relation existing between opposition members, phonological oppositions can be divided into three types: a. Privative oppositions are oppositions in which one member is characterized by the presence, the other by the absence, of a mark. For example: -Jk*4^** "voiced "/"voiceless," "nasalized"/"nonnasalized," "rounded"/"un-rounded." The opposition member that is characterized by the presence of the mark is called "marked," the member characterized by its absence "unmarked." This type of opposition is extremely important for phonology. b. Gradual oppositions are oppositions in which the members are characterized by various degrees or gradations of the same property. For example: the opposition between two different degrees of aperture in vowels, as in German u-o, ii-ii, i-e, or between various degrees of tonality. The member of a gradual opposition that possesses an extreme (either minimal or maximal) degree of the particular property is the extreme or external member, while the other member is the mid member. Gradual oppositions are relatively rare and not as important as privative oppositions. c. Equipollent oppositions are oppositions in which both members are logically equivalent, that is, they are neither considered as two degrees of one property nor as the absence or presence of a property. For example: German and f-k. Equipollent oppositions are the most frequent in any system. A phonic opposition, taken out of the context of the phonemic system and its functioning and considered in isolation, is always at once equipollent and gradual. As an example, let us study the opposition between voiced and voiceless obstruents. Instrumental phonetics teaches that consonants are only rarely absolutely voiced or absolutely voiceless: most cases merely involve various degrees of voice participation. Further, the voicing of 76 logical classification of distinctive oppositions an obstruent is connected with the relaxation of the muscles of the vocal organs. Voicelessness, on the other hand, is related to their tensing. The relation between d and /, as, for example, in Russian or French, is ambiguous from a purely phonetic point of view. In order to interpret their relation as privative, it is first of all important to focus attention on a single discriminative property alone (for example, only on voice or only on tensing of the muscles of the tongue). All others must be disregarded. In the second place, the lesser degree of the particular property must be "equated with zero." For example, the relation between u and o is privative as well if one regards these two vowels as the two extreme degrees of opening or closure, and if one interprets one of the degrees of aperture or closure as "zero degree": u is then the "unopen," o the "open" vowel, or, vice versa, !/ is the "close" and o the "unclose" rounded (or back) vowel phoneme. But the same relation of u-o becomes one of gradual opposition if the same vowel system has still another vowel with a degree of aperture exceeding that of o: u is then the extreme, and o the mid, member of a gradual opposition. The interpretation of a distinctive opposition as equipollent, gradual, or privative thus depends on the standpoint from which it is viewed. Yet, one should not assume that its interpretation is purely subjective and arbitrary. The structure and the functioning of the phonemic system in most cases indicates quite unequivocally and clearly how each opposition is to be evaluated. In a language that in addition to u and o has still other back, or back and rounded, vowels with a degree of opening greater than that of o, for example, o or a, the opposition u-o must be evaluated as gradual. On the other hand, in a language where u and o are the only back vowels, there is no reason to regard the opposition u-o as gradual. The opposition t-d, which was given as an example above, would only have to be evaluated as gradual in the case where the respective phonemic system contains still a third "dental" occlusive with a degree of voicelessness (and tensing of the muscles of the tongue) greater and more complete than that of t, or, vice versa, smaller than that of d. In cases where this condition does not prevail, there is no reason to interpret the opposition t-d as gradual. If the functioning of the phonemic system points to / as the unmarked member of the opposition t-d, the opposition /-r/must be considered privative. The tensing of the muscles of the tongue must then be considered an irrelevant side phenomenon, the degree of voicing of / being "zero," so that / is to be regarded as "voiceless" and d as "voiced." But if, on the other hand, in accordance with the functioning of the phonemic system, not / but d is the unmarked member, voicing becomes an irrelevant side phenomenon, and the tensing of the muscles of the tongue the discriminative mark of the logical classification of distinctive oppositions 77 opposition. / must then be considered "tense" and das "lax." Finally, if from the standpoint of the functioning of the phonemic system neither d nor t can be considered unmarked, the opposition t-d must be regarded as equipollent.7 The classification of concrete oppositions into gradual or privative oppositions thus depends partly on the structure and partly on the functioning of the phonemic system. But in addition, the opposition itself must contain something that makes its evaluation either gradual or privative possible. An opposition such as k-l can be neither privative nor gradual under any circumstances because its members can be conceived of neither as the presence and absence, nor as two dilierent degrees, of the same property. The opposition u-o, on the other hand, can be conceived of as privative ("close"/"unclose" or "open"/"uaopen") as well as gradual. Whether it actually must be regarded as privative, gradual, or equipollent depends on the structure and functioning of the respective phonemic system. It is therefore possible to distinguish potentially or logically privative or gradual oppositions from oppositions that are actually privative or gradual, and logically equipollent oppositions from those actually equipollent. Logically equipollent oppositions arc always actually equipollent as well. Actually equipollent oppositions, on the other hand, are not always logically equipollent but are sometimes logically privative or logically gradual. Presented in a diagram, they are as follows: logically gradual-* logically equipollent logically privative ■ actually gradual | actually equipollent actually privative C Classification of Oppositions on the Basis of the Extent of Their Distinctive Force: Constant anu Neutralizable Oppositions By the functioning of a phonemic system we understand the combinations of phonemes permissible in a given laneaage, as well as the rules governing the distinctive force of the individual oppositions. So far we have spoken of phonemes, distinctive oppositions, and systems of oppositions, without consideration of the actual distribution of the phonological units in the formation of words and forms. The role of the individual oppositions in a given language is rather varied, depending on the extent to which they actually possess distinctive force in all positions.8 In Danish x and e occur in all conceivable positions: they form a constant distinctive opposition whose members are independent phonemes. In Russian e occurs only before j and before palatalized consonants, while 78 logical classification of distinctive oppositions logical classification of distinctive oppositions 79 £ occurs in all other positions: here e and e are noninterchangeable phones that cannot be considered two independent phonemes but are two combinatory variants of a single phoneme. In French, however, e and e. occur only finally in open syllable as members of a distinctive opposition "les"/ "lait," "allez'V'allait". In all other positions the occurrence of e and e is predictable: r. occurs in closed syllable, c in open. These two vowels must thus be considered two phonemes only in final open syllable and combinatory variants of a single phoneme in all other positions. The distinctive opposition in French is thus neutralized in certain positions. We call such oppositions neittralizable oppositions, the positions in which the neutralization takes place, positions of neutralization, and those positions where the opposition is relevant, positions of relevance. The psychological difference between constant and neutralizable distinctive oppositions is very great. Constant distinctive oppositions are perceived clearly even by those members of the speech community who have had no phonetic training. The terms of such an opposition are considered two distinct "phonic entities." In neutralizable distinctive oppositions perception fluctuates: in positions of relevance both opposition members are clearly distinguished; in positions of neutralization, on the other hand, it is often not possible to indicate which of the two had just been produced or perceived. However, even in the position of relevance, members of a neutralizable opposition are often felt only as two meaning-differentiating nuances, that is, as two distinct yet closely related phonic entities. This sense of intimate relatedness is especially characteristic of opposition members of this type. From a purely phonetic point of view, the difference between French / and e is not greater than the difference between e and e. But the closeness of the relationship between e and e is apparent to any Frenchman, while in the case of / and e there can be no question of any particular closeness: the reason for this phenomenon is, of course, that the opposition between e and e is neutralizable, while the opposition between / and e is constant. Still, it should not be assumed that the distinction between neutralizable and constant distinctive oppositions is meaningful only from a psychological point of view. This distinction is of extreme importance for the functioning of phonemic systems, as was first stressed by N. Durnovo. It must be considered one of the basic principles of the theory of phonemic systems. Neutralization and neutralizability of distinctive oppositions therefore deserve a detailed discussion. Above all, the term itself must be clearly delineated. Not every type of distinctive opposition ca- be "neutralized." In those positions in which a neutralizable opposition s actually neutralized, the specific marks of an opposition member lose their distinctive force. Only those features which are common to both opposition members, that is, which serve as the basis for comparison for the respective opposition, remain relevant. One member of the opposition thus becomes the representative of the "archiphoneme" of the respective opposition in the position of neutralization. By the term "archiphoneme" we understand the sum of distinctive properties that 5 two phonemes have in common.9 It follows that only bilateral oppositions can be neutralized. In effect, only those oppositions that can be contrasted with all other phonological units of a given system have archiphonemes. And it is this contrastive capacity that is the basic prerequisite for phonological existence in general. In German the bilateral opposition d-t is neutralized in final position. The opposition member, which occurs in the position of neutralization, from a phonological point of view is neither a voiced stop nor a voiceless stop but "the nonnasal dental occlusive in general." As such it can be placed in opposition with the dental nasal //, as well as with the nonnasal labial and velar stops. However, the fact that German / and d cannot occur before / in word-initial position, while b and p do occur in that position, cannot effect a neutralization of the oppositions d-b and p-t: in a word such as " Blatt" (leaf) b retains all its properties, that is, it remains a voiced labial stop. It cannot be considered the representative of the archiphoneme of the opposition d-b because the phonological content of such an archiphoneme could only be a "voiced stop in general." But the b in "Blatt" cannot be interpreted as such because theg in "glatt" (smooth) is also a voiced stop. Actual neutralization, by which an opposition member becomes the representative of an archiphoneme, is therefore only possible in cases of distinctive bilateral oppositions. But this by no means implies that all bilateral oppositions are in effect neutralizable: constant bilateral oppositions probably exist in almost any language. But, whenever a language has a neutralizable opposition, it is always bilateral. How is the archiphoneme representative of a neutralizable opposition to be realized ? There are four possible cases: Case I.—The representative of the archiphoneme of a neutralizable opposition occurring in the position of neutralization is not identical with either of the opposition members. a. It is realized by a sound phonetically related to both opposition members but not identical with either one. In Russian the opposition between palatalized and nonpalatalized labials is neutralized before palatalized dentals. A special type of "semipalatalized" labial occurs in the position of neutralization: in English, where the opposition between| voiced lenes b, d, g, and voiceless fortes /;, t, k, is neutralized after s, at 80 logical classification of distinctive oppositions logical classification of distinctive oppositions special type of voiceless Icncs consonant occurs in that position; in certain Bavaro-Austrian dialects, in which the opposition between fortes and lenes is neutralized initially, special "semifortes" or "semilenes" sounds occur in that position, and so on. The number of examples could easily be multiplied. .In all these cases the archiphoneme is represented by a phone intermediary to the two opposition members. b. Somewhat different are those cases in which the representative of the archiphoneme, in addition to the features that it shares with .orie or the other opposition member, has still other specific features proper to it alone. Features of the latter category are then a result of assimilation to the phoneme next to which the opposition is neutralized. In the Peking dialect of Chinese, for example, the opposition k-c is neutralized before / and ii and a palatal č' appears as the representative of the archiphoneme.10 In Yami, a language spoken on Tobago Island, palatalized /, occurs before / as the archiphoneme of the opposition "dental /"/"retroflex /," etc.11 In all these cases, that is, in the cases discussed under (a) as well as (b), the phone that occurs in the position of neutralization is some kind of combinatory variant of the one as well as the other opposition member. Cases in which the archiphoneme is represented by a phone not fully identical with either of the opposition members are rather numerous. However, they are still less frequent than cases in which the sound occurring in the position of neutralization is more or less identical with the realization of a specific opposition member in the position of relevance. Case II.—The representative of the archiphoneme is identical with the realization of one of the opposition members, the choice of the archiphoneme representative being conditioned externally. This is possible only in cases where the neutralization of a neutralizable opposition depends on the proximity of some particular phoneme. The opposition member that "bears a closer resemblance or relation" to such a neighboring phoneme, or is even identical with it, becomes the representative of the archiphoneme. In many languages where the opposition between voiced and voiceless (or fortes and lenes) obstruents is neutralized before other obstruents of the same type of articulation, only voiced obstruents can occur before -oiced (or lenes) obstruents, and only voiceless obstruents before voiceless (or fortes) obstruents. In Russian, where the opposition between palatalized and nonpalatalized consonants is neutralized before nonpalatalized dentals, only nonpalatalized consonants can occur in that position, and so on. In cases of this type which are relatively rare, the choice of an opposition member as the representative of the respective archiphoneme is conditioned purely externally by the nature of the position of neutralization. Case III.—The choice of an opposition member as the archiphoneme representative is conditioned internally. a. In cases of this type one of the opposition members occurs in the position of neutralization, and its choice is in no way related to the nature of the position of neutralization. However, due to the fact that one of the opposition members occurs in that position as the representative of the respective archiphoneme, its specific features become nonrelevant, while the specific features of its partner receive full phonological relevance: the former opposition member is thus considered "archiphoneme + zero," while the latter is considered "archiphoneme J a specific mark." In other words, the opposition member that is permitted i;. the position of neutralization is unmarked from the standpoint of the respective phonemic system, while the opposing member is marked. This, of course, can only be the case where the neutralizable opposition is logically privative. However, most neutralizable distinctive oppositions belong to this category, that is, they are regarded as oppositions between unm;*rked and marked members, the member in the position of neutralization being regarded as unmarked. b. If, however, the neutralizable opposition is not privative but gradual, as, for example, the opposition between the different degrees of aperture in vowels, or between the various levels of tone, it is always the external or extreme opposition member that occurs in the Position of neutralization. In the dialects of Bulgarian and Modern Grei' . in which the oppositions u-o and i-e are neutralized in unstressed syllables, the maximally close (actually minimally open) u and / serve as representatives of the respective archiphonemes in the position of neutralization. In Russian, where the opposition o-a is neutralized in unstressed syllables, the maximally open (actually minimally close) a represents the respective archiphoneme in the immediately pretonic syllable. In Lamba, a Bantu language of N. Rhodesia, where the opposition between low and mid tone is neutralized in final position, the position of neutralization, that is, in final syllables, permits only low tone.12 Examples could easily be multiplied. The reason for this phenomenon is not always clear. As we have already emphasized, a gradual opposition can be regarded as gradual only it J>g «.,uese circumstances be regarded as "the more genuine." And since the ^Opposition "sharp" j/"soft" z is logically privative, one can probably ■gard it as actually privative, and the "soft" z as its unmarked member. ». Thus there are cases in which the neutralization of a privative opposi-jBon clearly and objectively indicates which member of that opposition is fenmarked and which is marked: in Case III the unmarked member of the tutralized opposition serves as the only representative of the archi-honeme; in Casf. IV it serves as the archiphoneme representative in the ^Osition of maximal phonemic differentiation. jfoThe neutralization of an opposition sometimes points to the marked jCTaracter ofthe member of another opposition. That is to say, a neutraliz-flble opposition in the vicinity ofthe marked member of a related opposi-6n is frequently neutralized. For example, in Artshi, an East Caucasian jiguage, the opposition between rounded and unrounded consonants is eutralized before o and u, whereby o and u are proved to be the marked ^embers of the opposition o-e and it-i. |§p Logically privative oppositions thus become actually privative by Cutralization, and the distinction between unmarked and marked opposition members obtains an objective basis. CORRELATIONS pj, Two phonemes that are in a relation of bilateral opposition to each other dlarc by that very fact closely related to each other: what is common to wboth of them does not recur in any other phoneme of the same system. gThey arc therefore the only ones of their kind. In placing them in opposition Jfcwith each other, that which is unique to each of them is clearly brought into |?relief with that which links them to each other. Two phonemes that are jyn a relation of multilateral opposition with each other, on the other hand, l?iPPear as indivisible units. In the case of phonemes that participate in a jkjjroportional opposition, it is easy to separate the discriminative property Jlfrom the other properties since the discriminative property recurs as such pin several phoneme pairs of the same system. Consequently it can be fceasily abstracted or thought of as independent from all other properties. jtln contrast, in the case of phonemes that participate in an isolated opposi-jfion, the discriminative property cannot be abstracted as easily, simply jJbecause it occurs only once in such a system, namely, together with the IjOther properties of those phonemes to which it pertains. Of all possible ^jitlations between two phonemes, it is the privative relation that most early shows the presence or absence of certain properties ofthe particular ff 84 logical classification of distinctive oppositions logical classification of distinctive oppositions 85 phonemes. The analysis of the phonemic content of phonemes that are in a relation of privative opposition with each other is therefore easiest. In contrast, the phonemic content of phonemes that are in an equipollent relation to each other is the most difficult to analyze. Two phonemes that participate in a neutralizable opposition are considered as closely related,, even in the position of relevance. Each of them is regarded as a special variety of the archiphoneme in question, and the reality of the latter is guaranteed by means of its occurrence in the position of neutralization. The appurtenance to one archiphoneme, on the other hand, is much less evident for two phonemes that are in a relation of constant, nonneutralizable opposition. The following conclusion can be drawn from what has been said: the participation of two phonemes in a bilateral, proportional, privative, and thus neutralizable, opposition has as a result, first that the phonemic content of such phonemes can be analyzed most clearly since the discriminative property is clearly brought into relief with what constitutes the basis for comparison; and second, that the two phonemes are considered as particularly closely related to each other. In contrast, two phonemes that are in a relation of isolated, multilateral (and consequently nonneutralizable) opposition with each other are maximally opaque with respect to their phonemic content, and maximally distant from each other in relatedness. (These features are particularly prominent in the case of a heterogeneous opposition.) If one considers the neutralizable, privative, proportional, bilateral oppositions and the isolated, heterogeneous, multilateral oppositions as two extremes, all remaining types of opposition can be classified between these two extreme points. The more neutralizable, privative, proportional, bilateral, and homogeneous oppositions there are in a system, the greater its cohesion. On the other hand, the more logically equipollent, isolated, multilateral, and heterogeneous oppositions there are in a system, the greater the noncohesion of that system. It therefore seems appropriate to distinguish the privative, proportional, bilateral oppositions from all other oppositions by a special term. In phonemic literature the term correlation is used for this purpose. But the definition that is given for the term "correlation" and some other related notions in the "Projet de terminologie phonologique standardisee" (TCLP, IV, 1930) must be changed to some degree since it was formulated at a time when the theory of oppositions had not as yet completely developed. We now propose the following definitions. By correlation pair we understand two phonemes that are in a relation of logically privative, proportional, bilateral opposition with each other. A correlation mark is a phonological property whose presence or absence characterizes a series of correlation pairs, as, for example, the nasality of nasals which in French distinguishes between the correlation pairs an-a, on-o, in-e, un-eu. By correlation is to be unders od the sum of all correlation pairs characterized by the same correlation mark. A paired phoneme is a phoneme that participates in a correlation pair, while an unpaired phoneme is one that does not participate in any correlative pair. The notion "correlation" is certainly a very fruitful one for the development of phonology, though its importance had been somewhat overestimated during the first period after its discovery. All oppositions whose members did not form correlative pairs were thrown together and designated by the general term "disjunction," so that two types of relations were recognized between phonological units: they cither formed a correlation or a disjunction. But a closer examination revealed that in fact several types of distinctive oppositions had to be distinguished, and that the term "disjunction" was unproductive in its original, too general formulation. Furthermore, the fundamental difference between neutralizable and nonneutralizable correlations had to be uncovered. A nonneutralizable correlation, incidentally, also retains its importance for the cohesion of the phonemic system. Subject to this reservation, the theory of correlations may take the place it deserves in phonology.14 Depending on the correlation mark, different types of correlations are distinguished: for example, the correlation of voice (French d-t, b-p, g-k, z-s, etc.) or the correlation of quantity (a-a, i-i, etc.). These various correlation types are related to each other in varying degrees and can be classified in related groups. The relation of the correlation mark to the other properties of the respective phonemes serves as the basis for comparison. For example, the correlation of voice (French d-t, b-p) and the correlation of aspiration (Sanskrit /-//;, p-ph) belong to the same related class because their correlation marks represent different types of work performed by the larynx and different types of tensing in the oral cavity, independent of the place of articulation in the oral cavity. The classification of correlations in related groups is not merely a theoretical artifice. It corresponds to concrete reality. Even naive linguistic consciousness "feels" quite clearly that the oppositions u-ii and 6-e in German, though different, are still on the same olane, while the opposition between long and short a lies on quite a different plane. The projection of distinctive oppositions (and thus also of correlations) sometimes onto the same and sometimes onto different planes is the psychological consequence of just those kin relationships between the correlation marks on which the classification of correlations into related classes is based. 86 LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 4 CORRELATION BUNDLES In cases where a phoneme participates in several correlations of the same related class, all phonemes taking part in the same correlative pairs unite to form a multimember correlation bundle. The structure of such a bundle is quite varied. It depends not only on the number of participant correlations but also on their mutual relationship. Bundles of two related correlations are the most frequent. Here two possibilities exist: both members of the one correlation may also participate in the other correlation, or both correlations have only one member in common. In the first case the result is a four-member, in the second a three-member, correlational bundle. These two cases can best be illustrated by Sanskrit and Classical Greek. In both languages stops participated simultaneously in the correlations of voice and of aspiration. In Sanskrit the result was a four-member bundle: p-ph k-kh t-th b-bh g-gli d-dh, etc. In Classical Greek it was a three-member bundle: LOGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 87 ß 9 y By linking three correlations that are related in type it is theoretically possible to have bundles of four to eight members. In fact many of these types can be attested by examples from different languages. In most languages of the Caucasus the correlation of voice and the correlation based on type of expiration combine with the correlation of occlusiveness. The latter term refers to the opposition of stops or affricates with spirants. In Chechen, for example, four-member bundles result as follows:15 Y q ^n^rT^0CduS[^ is relev-' only in the case of voice-lessness ( .and - are reahzed as affricates initially, and as sniranK mechally and finally), the opposition of expiration onV in the Zof occlusives (and affricates). .„ Georgian the s'ame correlations yields- member bundles because the correlation of occlusiveness extends to both members of the correlation of voice: Finally, in Circassian the same correlations make up a six-member bundle because in the latter case the correlation based on type of expiration extends to both members of the correlation of occlusiveness: The tie between members of a correlation bundle becomes particularly close when the entire bundle can be neutralized. Such neutralizable bundles are not rare. The above-mentioned four-member bundles of Sanskrit were neutralizable before obstruents and in final position (the unaspirated tenuis occurring as the only archiphoneme in absolutely final position). In Korean, where the occlusives form three-member bundles (lenis-fortis-aspirate), these bundles are neutralized in final position.' The corresponding archi-phonemes are represented by implosives. But the Korean consonants also form a three-member correlation bundle with respect to timbre (neutral-palatalized-labialized). These bundles are neutralized in final position. Their archiphonemes are represented by consonants of neutral timbre. But in addition, the correlation of palatalization is neutralized before /, the representative of the archiphoneme being conditioned externally; the correlation of labialization is neutralized before;/ and y, the representative of the archiphoneme being conditioned internally.Ih In Artshi, a language of the Eastern Caucasus group, the acute sibilants form a six-member correlation bundle (media-voiceless affricates without glottal occlusion-weak affricates without glottal occlusion-strong affricates with glottal occlusion-weak voiceless spirants-and strong voiceless spirants) which is neutralized before t and d. The archiphoneme in this case is represented by the (weak?) spirant. Examples can easily be multiplied. As a result of the projection of all members of a correlation bundle onto the same plane, and as a result of the close interrelatedness of these members, it is sometimes rather difficult to analyze the bundle into individual correlations. For example, in cases where different prosodic correlations combine into a bundle, the members of such a bundle are sometimes conceived of as different "accents," with little attention given to differences of 88 logical classification of distinctive oppositions logical classification of distinctive oppositions 89 quantity or differences of type of tone close. Sometimes they are conceived of as different degrees in quantity without regard to differences in tone movement. Mistakes of this type are made not only by laymen and untutored speakers. They are also made by theoreticians and sometimes even by professional phoneticians. Cases of this type are proof of the psychological reality of the classification of correlations into related groups. They are possible only where a correlation bundle is actually present, that is, where a phoneme participates in several correlations of the same related group. If a phoneme participates simultaneously in several correlations of different related groups, such correlations do not combine into "bundles": they are not projected on the same plane but superimposed on one another. German stressed long / participates simultaneously in Several correlations, namely, in the correlation of accent, the correlation of quantity, and the correlation of rounding. But while the former two form a bundle (the prosodic correlation bundle), the correlation of rounding (i-ii, e-o) clearly belongs to another " plane." It can happen, of course, that two correlation bundles belonging to different "planes" are superimposed on each other and that both are neutralized in certain positions. We have already mentioned Korean, in which occlusives form a correlation bundle consisting of lenes-fortes-aspirates, and in which, further, all consonants, including the occlusives, form bundles of timbre consisting of a neutral, a palatalized, and a labialized member. Both correlation bundles are neutralized in final position. Consequently in word-final position in Korean, the guttural im-plosive A' represents an archiphoneme that corresponds to nine phonemes medially—g, k, k';g', k', k";g", k°, k°l. Still, the bundles g-k-k' andg-g'-g° obviously belong to quite different planes. 8 On this point, cf. our essay " Die Aufhebung der phonologischen Gegensätze," in TCLP, VI, 29 ff., as well as A. Martinet, "Neutralisation et archiphoneme," in ibid., pp. 46 ff. 9 Cf. R. Jakobson in TCLP, 11, 8 f. 10 Cf. Henri Frei in Bulletin de la Maison franco-japonaise, VIII (1936), no. 1,130. 11 Cf. Erin Assai, "A study of Yami Langu; -c, an Indonesian Language spoken on Botel Tobago Island" (Leiden, 1935), p. 15. 12 Cf. Clement M. Doke, "A Study of Lamba Phonetics," in Bantu Studies (July 1928). 13 What has been said applies, of course, only to those neutralizable gradual oppositions in which one member is "extreme." In cases where both members show different "mid" degrees of the same property, either the one or the other member can represent the archiphoneme. It depends on how the particular property is treated from the viewpoint of the given language. Most cases actually involve the opposition between two types of e or o vowels. In some languages close e and o are considered unmarked, in others open e and o. This can be seen from their occurrence in the position of neutralization. Accordingly the opposition in such cases ceases to be gradual from a phonological point of view. 14 With respect to what follows, cf. (subject to the above reservation) N. S. Trubetzkoy, "Die phonologischen Systeme," in TCLP, IV, 96 ff. The term "correlation," proposed and defined by R. Jakobson, was used for the lirst time with reference to a proportional bilateral opposition in his proposal to the Congress of Linguists in The Hague, cosigned by S. Karcevskij and the present writer. See /" Congres International de Linguistes (La Haye, 1928), "Propositions," pp. 36 ff., and Actes du I" Congres International de Linguistes, pp. 36 ff., and TCLP, II, 6 f. 15 Cf. N. S. Trubetzkoy, "Die Konsonantensysteme der ostkaukasischen Sprachen," in Caucasica, VIII (1931). 16 Cf. A. Cholodovic, "O latinizacii korejskogo pis'ma," in Sovetskoje jazykoznanije, I, 144 ff. 1 In this connection cf. N. S. Trubetzkoy, " Essai ďune théorie des oppositions phonologiques," in Journal de psychologie, XXXIII, 5-18. 2 The term was first used by N. Durnovo. 3 The " phoneme j" is nonexistent in stage German. Stage German j should be regarded as a combinatory variant of the vowel i. Accordingly it does not belong to the consonant system. 4 Cf. Fr. Trávníček, Správná česká výslovnost (Brno, 1935), p. 24. 5 In this regard cf. E. A. Krejnovič, "Nivchskij (giljaekij) jazyk," in Jazyki i pis''mennosť narodov Severa, III (1934), 188 ff. 6 As for the division of multilateral oppositions into heterogeneous and homogeneous oppositions, and the division of homogeneous oppositions into linear and nonlinear oppositions, they are ultimately also based on the same principles. 7 In this connection, see under c. IV PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 1 PRELIMINARY REMARKS So far we have considered the various types of distinctive oppositions thus: (a) from the point of view of their relation to other oppositions in the same system; (b) from the point of view of the logical relations between the opposition members themselves; and (c) from the point of view of the extent of their distinctive force. These three viewpoints resulted in three different classifications: (a) bilateral and multilateral, proportional and isolated oppositions; (b) privative, gradual, and equipollent oppositions; and (c) neutralizable and constant oppositions. All these standpoints and principles of classification are valid not only for phonological systems but for any other system of oppositions as well. They contain nothing that is specifically phonological. In order to be applied successfully in the analysis of concrete phonological opposition systems, they must still be supplemented by specifically phonological principles of classification. The specific character of a phonological opposition consists in the latter's being a distinctive opposition of sound. "Distinctiveness" in the phonological sense, that is, the capacity of differentiating meaning, is something that requires no further classification. Phonological oppositions can, nevertheless, from this point of view be divided into oppositions differentiating words (Lxical oppositions) and oppositions differentiating sentences (syntactic oppositions). For the meanings that can be distinguished by phonological oppositions are either the meanings of words, 90 including the meanings of individual grammatical word forms, or the meanings of sentences. This division is certainly of importance for the phonemic systems of the individual languages. It is less important for the general classification of distinctive oppositions, for all distinctive oppositions that appear with a syntactic function in one language may occur with a lexical function in another language. There are actually no specific phonological oppositions for differentiating sentences: an opposition that in one language serves to differentiate sentences in another serves to differentiate words. Much more important for the general classification of phonological oppositions is the fact that these oppositions are phonic oppositions. Neither gesticulations with one's hands nor flag signals, but specific phonic properties, are placed in opposition with each other in phonological oppositions. It is presumed common knowledge that the purpose of contrasting sounds with each other is to differentiate meaning. The problem as to how phonic properties are placed in opposition with each other, that is, what types of opposition result, was discussed in Chapter III. The question now is to examine what phonic properties form phonological (distinctive) oppositions in the various languages of the world. In Chapter HI we operated with purely logical concepts. We must now combine these logical concepts with acoustic and articulatory, that is, with phonetic, concepts. For no other discipline except phonetics can teach us about individual sound properties. But we must not forget what has been said in the Introduction about the relationship of phonology to phonetics. Already in view of the fact that they are made part of the system of opposition categories that were discussed in Chapter III, the phonetic concepts with which the phonologist operates appear or necessity somewhat 91 92 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions schematized and simplified. Thus actually very little remains of phonetics in the following exposition. But this should not disappoint the phonetician. The object of the present chapter is not a classification of the sounds that can be produced by the human vocal apparatus, but a systematic survey of the phonic properties that are in effect utilized for the differentiation of, meaning in the various languages of the world. It is therefore also rather unimportant for the phonologist wither he uses acoustic or sound-physiological phonetic terminology. Important is only the unambiguous designation of phonic properties, which in phonetic literature are studied and investigated from various points of view, and which all phoneticians, despite existing differences of opinion, should be familiar with at least as objects of study. In modern instrumental phonetics, especially by means of sound film and X rays, it has become increasingly evident that the same sound effects can be produced by quite different movements of the vocal organs (Paul Menzerath, G. Oscar Russel). Such terms as "front vowel" or "occlusive" are therefore rejected from the standpoint of the modern methods. However, these terms have the advantage of being understood correctly by anyone familiar with traditional phonetics. Even the most accurate phonetician, provided he is not pedantic, can accept such terms, for lack of better and more accurate ones, as conventional designations for familiar objects of study. Acoustic terminology unfortunately is still very sparse. Consequently it is unavoidable in most cases to use physiological terms coined by traditional phonetics, although modern phonetics ascribes more consistency and uniformity to the acoustic effect than to the articulatory movements producing it. The phonologist, who is for the most part only interested in making reference to generally known phonetic concepts, is able to overcome these terminological difficulties. 2 CLASSIFICATION OF DISTINCTIVE PHONIC PROPERTIES The phonic properties that form distinctive oppositions in the various languages can be divided into three classes: vocalic, consonantal, and prosodic. Vowel phonemes consist of distinctive vocalic properties, consonantal phonemes of distinctive consonantal properties; but there are no phonemes that consist exclusively of prosodic properties. Depending on the language, prosodic properties may combine with a single vowel phoneme, a single consonantal phoneme, or an entire sequence of phonemes. Before defining the various classes of distinctive phonic properties, the term "vowel" and "consonant" must therefore be examined. phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 93 L. Hjclmslev attempted to define these terms without reference to any phonetic concepts: vowels are to be those phonemes, or in Hjelmslev's terminology, "cenemes" or "cenematemes," wliich have the faculty of forming a notional unit or a word by themselves, while all other phonemes or "cenemes" or "cenematemes" respectively are consonants.1 Hjelmslev subsequently refined this definition which obviously restricted the application of the vowel concept too much. (For example, in German only three vowel phonemes would remain: Oh! AuUnd Ei!) He added the following supplement: "Nous comprenons par voyelle un ceneme susceptible de constituer a lui scul un enonce .. . ou bien admettant a I'intcricur d'une syllabe les memes combinaisons qu'un tel ceneme."2 But even in this second, expanded version the definition is untenable. Again, in German, of the pure vowels only the o and of the diphthongs only xe and ao are used as interjections, only Au and Ei as words. These three vowel phonemes, among others, can also occur word-finally ("froh," "Frau," "frei" [glad, woman, free]), but they are not found before //. Short vowels, on the other hand, cannot occur in final position, but some, namely /', it, it, a, and e, occur before ij. If one regards interjections such as Oh! Ai! and Au! as independent notional units (enonces), one must also recognize as such the interjection ssh! (a plea for silence). In keeping with Hjelmslev's definition, German short u, u, i, a, and e would accordingly have to be regarded as consonants, while German s and all phonemes participating in the same combinations, that is, practically all consonants, would have to be regarded as vowels. The untenability of the definition proposed by Hjelmslev is even more apparent in other languages. In addition to the interjection s! Russian also has the interjections s! and c! In certain other languages the number of isolated "syllabic consonants" used as interjection ■ or command words for animals is even more numerous.3 On the other hand, there are many languages in which vowels cannot occur initially, and where it is consequently impossible to have words consisting of a single vowel. The untenability of the definition given by Hjclmslev is not an accident. "Vowel" and "consonant" are phonic or acousti terms, and can only be defined as such. Any attempt to eliminate or circumvent acoustic-articula-tory concepts in the definition of vowels and consonants must necessarily fail. The process of phonation of human speech can West be illustrated by the following scheme: somebody whistles or sings ; iciody into the mouthpiece of a tube and alternately opens and covers the other end of that tube with his hand. It is clear that three types of elements can be distinguished acoustically in the course of this process: first, the segments between 94 PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 95 closing and opening the orifice; second, the segments between opening and closing it; and third, the segments of the melody whistled or sung into the tube. Elements of the first type correspond to consonants, elements of the second type to vowels, and those of the third type to prosodic units. Important for a consonant is, in the words of Paul Menzerath, "a closure-aperture movement, with an articulatory maximum between these two points," for a vowel "a movement of aperture-closure, with an articulatory minimum in the interspace."4 In other words, what characterizes a consonant is the production of an obstruction and the overcoming of such an obstruction. A vowel, on the other hand, is characterized by the absence of any obstruction.5 It follows from what has been said that properties that are specifically consonantal can refer only to various types of obstructions or to the ways of overcoming these obstructions. They may therefore be called properties based on the manner of overcoming an obstruction {Ueberwindungsarteigen-schaflen). Properties that are specifically vocalic, on the other hand, can only refer to the various types of absence of an obstruction, that is, practically speaking, to the various degrees of aperture. They may therefore be called properties based on degree of aperture (Oeffnungsgradeeigen-schaften). In addition to these properties, which are specifically consonantal or vocalic, consonantal and vowel phonemes may have certain other properties. Let us suppose that in the presented scheme of the phonation process the length of the tube changes continually, or the position of its orifice varies continually. It follows that with respect to the consonants the different types of obstruction or the different modes of overcoming these obstructions, and with respect to the vowels, the different degrees of aperture, must be localized in different positions. As a result, special properties of localization are produced for the consonants as well as for the vowels. These form, so to speak, a second coordinate to consonant or vowel quality, respectively. For some vowel and consonant phonemes still a third quality coordinate can be established. To stay with our phonation scheme, let us suppose that our tube is connected with another resonator, and during phonation this connection is alternately established and disrupted. This, of course, must affect the character of the sound produced. The specific acoustic properties that the phonation of consonants and vowels produces by means of establishing and disrupting the connection with the second resonator can be termed properties of resonance. A distinctive property exists only by virtue of being a member of a distinctive opposition. German d, when placed in opposition with / ("Seidc"/"Scite" [silk/side]), has the property "lenis" based on the manner of overcoming an obstruction; when placed in opposition with /; ("dir"/"Bier" [you/beer]) or with g ("dir"/"Gicr" [you/greed]), the property of localization "dental" or "apical"; and the property of resonance "nonnasal" when placed in opposition with n ("doch"/"noch" [yet/ still]). Similarly, French o has a specific property based on degree of aperture in opposition with u ("dos"/"doux"), a specific property of localization in opposition with o ("dos"/"deux"), and a specific property of resonance in opposition with o ("dos"/"don"). In contrast, German o does not have any property of resonance because a distinctive opposition between nasalized and nonnasalized or between pharyngeali/ed and nonpharyngealized vowels is alien to standard German. The "three coordinates" to vowel or consonantal quality need not, therefore, be present in every vowel or consonant phoneme. But each one of the properties that make up a vowel or a consonant phoneme must belong to one of the "three coordinates" mentioned. As regards the prosodic units, our phonation scheme shows that they are rhythmic-melodic units-—"musical" in the broadest sense of the word. Even from a purely phonetic point of view, the "syllable" is basically something quite different from a combination of vowels and consonants.6 The phonological prosodic unit is, of course, not simply identical with the "syllable" (in the phonetic sense). However, it always relates to the syllable because, depending on the language, it is either a specific segment of the syllable or an entire sequence of syllables. It is quite clear that its properties cannot be identical with the vocalic and consonantal properties discussed above. Since the prosodic unit must be conceived of as "musical" (rhythmic-melodic), or better, as a segment of a "musical" unit, it follows that "prosodic properties" refer either to the specific marks of each constituent segment of a melody (intensity, tone) or to the type of segmentation of the melody in the phonation process of human speech. The former type of properties effectuates the rhythmic-melodic differentiation of prosodic units. The latter characterizes the contact of a given prosodic unit with an irhmediatety adjacent unit. Prosodic properties can therefore be divided into properties based on type of differentiation and properties based on type of contact. 3 VOCALIC PROPERTIES A Terminology As already discussed, vocalic properties are divided into properties based on degree of aperture, properties of localization, and properties of resonance. The first two types of properties are much more closely related 96 PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS to each other than to the properties of resonance, so that they may be combined into a special group or bundle.7 Among all speech sounds, vowels can most easily be analyzed acoustically. The degrees of aperture correspond acoustically to "degrees of saturation or sonority." In principle the more the lower jaw is lowered, that is, the wider the mouth is opened, the higher the degree of saturation. But this principle appears to be fully valid only in the case of isolated vowels when they are sung. In spontaneous connected speech the same acoustic .effects can also be achieved with the articulating organs in a different position. The parallelism between degree of saturation of the vowel and degree of lowering of the lower jaw (vertical movement) is therefore not always complete.8 Since the linguist, after all, is ultimately interested in the acoustic effect, it would perhaps be advisable to replace the term properties based on degree of aperture, by properties based on degree of sonority or properties based on degree of saturation. The properties of localization correspond acoustically to various gaps in the series of partial tones: the "front vowels" show an increase of the higher and a suppression of the lower partial tones; conversely the higher partial tones are the ones that are suppressed in the case of the "back vowels." In general, the stronger or higher the partial tones, the shorter the "front resonator," that is, in terms of the human vocal apparatus, the shorter is the distance between the rims of the lips and the highest point of the mass of the tongue. But since the same acoustic effect can also be achieved by other positions of the vocal organs, the parallelism between tongue and lip movement ("horizontal movement") is not always present in this case either. The term properties of localization with reference to the vowels may therefore be replaced by properties of timbre. In the following discussion the "inexact" terms "properties pertaining to degree of aperture" and "properties of localization" are used in addition to the acoustic terms. Languages having only one vowel phoneme do not seem to exist in the world. If such a "one-vowel" language should ever have existed, it must have permitted numerous consonant combinations. For only under this condition would a single vowel phoneme be able to exist at all, since it could be placed in opposition with the absence of a vowel (zero vowel) between the members of a consonant combination or after consonants in final position. A "one-vowel" language without consonant combinations, on the other hand, would be vowellcss from a phonological point of view, since the obligatory vowel after every consonant would have to be evaluated as a matter-of-fact component in the realization of the consonant and would not have any distinctive force.9 The languages with which we are familiar have several vowel phonemes which form specific rowel systems. k PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 9V B From the point of view of degrees of aperture (degrees of sonority) and W. vocalic localization series (classes of timbre), three basic types of vowel If system can be set up:10 (a) linear systems, in which the vowel phonemes M possess specific degrees of sonority but no distinctively relevant properties A of timbre (properties of vocalic localization); (b) quadrangular systems, in X which all vowel phonemes not only possess distinctive properties based on S degree of sonority but also distinctive properties of timbre; and (c) tri- X angular systems, in which all vowel phonemes possess distinctive properties M based on degree of sonority. Distinctive properties of timbre are found with • all vowels except the maximally open vowel phoneme. The latter phoneme ■ is outside the oppositions of localization. Within these basic types, subtypes B can be set up depending on how many degrees of sonority and classes of 2 localization there are, and depending on the relations of logical opposition 9> between the individual types of distinctive property. fln B Properties of Localization or Timbre M There are languages in which these vowel properties are not distinctive, &* because they are automatically conditioned by the phonic environment. » This is the case in Adyghe, where three vowel ph nemes are distinguished: V the maximally close "a" which is realized as u in the neighborhood of ft labialized velars, as ii between two labials and after labialized sibilants, as S ui after nonlabialized back velars, as / after palatals, and in all other posi- •Br" tions as a close indeterminate vowel ,?; mid open "e" which is realized S after labialized velars as o, after labialized sibilants and between labials S as d, after laryngeals and nonlabialized back velars as a, in the remaining jBL positions as e or as indeterminate open vowel e\ and the maximally open w "a" which is realized between two labials as slightly rounded, between W two palatals as it, and elsewhere as a long a. The duration of these vowels M: is in accord with their sonority: "a" is the long \ "e" somewhat shorter IK (after laryngeals and nonlabialized back velars v is difference in quantity is clearly noticeable), 'V the shortest, with a tendency to be reduced, ■f Long, ii, 6, e, and / do occur, but only as optional variants of diphthongs Wt ("eir," "air," "ej," "dj"). Similar conditions '. old true for Abkhas, but J§- there the realization of the mid-open vowel is more uniform: it occurs as an e only in the vicinity of j, as an o only before a ir in closed syllable, elsewhere always as a which is distinguished from the maximally sonorous 'ft vowel mainly by its shorter duration. The vowel system of Ubyk is in all W probability based on the same principle. Vowel phonemes with a phono- ft logically specific degree of sonority and phonologically irrelevant timbre • would thus appear to be a peculiarity of the ' Caucasian languages. M Whether such "linear" vowel systems also occu, elsewhere is hard to say 98 PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVH OPPOSITIONS PHONOLOGICAL C'LASSI S Ol DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS at the present state of phonological studies in the world. As far as we know, linear systems do occur in certain languages as partial systems, in particular in certain Finno-Ugric and Turkic languages, in which the vocalism of the first syllable is richer than the vocalism of all other syllables. (On this point see further below.) In the overwhelming majority of languages, the properties of timbre of the vowel phonemes are distinctive. The only difference between triangular systems and quadrangular systems is that in the former distinctive oppositions of timbre exist only with respect to the vowels of nonmaximal degree of aperture, while in the latter type they are found in vowel phonemes of all degrees of aperture. Actually there are only two oppositions of timbre: one opposition between rounded and unrounded vowels (opposition of lip rounding), and another between back and front vowels (opposition of tongue position).11 These oppositions can occur with distinctive force either independently or in combination, thus producing different classes of timbre. The following eight classes of timbre are conceivable: rounded, unrounded, front, back, front rounded, back rounded, front unrounded, back unrounded. All eight of these classes do in fact occur in different languages. But in a single system only four classes of timbre can exist at the most. The triangular and quadrangular systems can accordingly be divided into two-class, three-class, and four-class systems. Acoustically, the rounded vowels are darker than the unrounded, and the front vowels clearer than the back vowels. Every multiclass vowel system must therefore have a maximally dark and a maximally clear class of timbre, which may be designated as extreme classes since there may be one or two medial classes between them. Three possibilities exist for two-class systems: either the opposition of tongue position alone is distinctive, or the opposition of lip rounding alone is distinctive, or both oppositions occur in combination. In the first case the back and front vowels are placed in opposition with each other, and lip participation is phonologically irrelevant. In the second case rounded and unrounded vowels are opposed to each other, and the position of the tongue is phonologically irrelevant. Finally, the third case involves a distinctive opposition between back rounded and front unrounded vowels. In this case the properties of timbre of the vowel phonemes cannot be divided. Thus one should actually not speak of back rounded and front unrounded vowels, but only of maximally dark and maximally clear vowels. It is evident that the first and second cases involve logically privative oppositions, while the third case involves a logically equipollent opposition. In quadrangular two-class systems, cases one and two are the ones usually found, that is, the correlation of tongue position or the correlation of lip rounding is found in its pure form. It all depends here on the makeup of the two vowel phonemes with the maximal degree of aperture. If both are unrounded, one must be a back vowel, the other a front vowel. As a result, the opposition of tongue position also becomes a bilateral proportional opposition in the other vowel pairs of the same system. On the other hand, the fact that back vowels of nonmaximal degree of aperture are rounded is nonsignificant from the point of view of the total system. As an example of such a two-class quadrangular system, the vowel system of those archaic Montenegran dialects may be cited in which the Proto-Skivic "semivowels" did not develop into an a, as they did in Serbo-Croatian, but into a particularly open a- sound (a "sound intermediary between a and c>"):]2 a ie o e If, however, the "dark" vowel of" maximal degree of aperture is rounded, and its "partner" is an unrounded nonfront vowel, the lip position alone is phonologically relevant for such a \owel pair. As a result, the opposition of lip rounding becomes exclusively distinctive for all other vowels of the same system as well, while the frontal character of the unrounded vowels is considered merely an insignificant secondary phenomenon. The vowel system of the Plaza dialect of Polish (in Western Little Poland) will serve as an example of such a quadrangular system: a a Quadrangular two-class systems, in which the maximally open vowel pair is represented by a back rounded \owcl and a front unrounded vowel phoneme, arc extremely rare. In systems of this kind individual properties of localization cannot be isolated: the vowels are divided into two classes of timbre, a maximally dark and a maximally clear class. These stand in a relationship of logically equipollent opposition to each other. The vowel system of the Uzbek dialect of Tashkent may serve as an example:14 u i 100 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions This relation of logically equipollent opposition between the two classes of timbre, which as indicated is extremely rare in quadrangular systems, predominates in triangular two-class systems. In systems of the latter type back rounded (maximally dark) vowels are contrasted with front unrounded (maximally clear) vowels as "polar" members of an equipollent opposition; and the maximally open vowel phoneme a, which stands outside this opposition, is a back unrounded vowel, that is, it .belongs to neither of the two classes of timbre as the remaining phonemes of the respective vowel system. The well-known vowel system of Latin may serve as a classical example: Similar triangular systems, only sometimes with a different number of degrees of aperture, occur in the most diverse languages in all parts of the world. Only rarely does it happen that the correlation of lip rounding or the correlation of tongue position alone has distinctive force in a two-class triangular system, so that the relation between the two classes of timbre would be logically privative. This may be deduced either from the realization of the vowel phonemes or from the circumstances surrounding the neutralization of the various oppositions. The vowel systems of Russian, Artshi, and Ostyak may be cited as examples of such triangular two-class systems in which only the correlation of lip rounding is distinctive. In Russian the front or back position of the tongue is contextually conditioned in the realization of the vowel phonemes: between two palatalized consonants "m," "a" "," and "/'" are pronounced as front vowels (.7, ii, , and /'). u is also fronted in that position, though not as much as the other vowels. On the other hand, after nonpalatalized (phonetically velarized) consonants "m," "o," and "a" are realized as vowels of the back series, "/'" as a vowel of the back-central series (ui). "," too, is pronounced by some Russians as a vowel of the central series in this position. Back or front position of the tongue is therefore phonologically irrelevant for Russian vowels: the correlation of lip rounding of the vowel phonemes alone has distinctive force.15 Artshi, an East Caucasian language of Central Daghestan, has a "correlation of consonantal rounding," that is, certain consonants are divided into a rounded and an unrounded variety. This correlation is neutralized before and after the rounded vowels u and o.16 As a result, these vowels arc placed in opposition with the remaining vowels phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 101 of the Artshi system, namely, with unrounded a, e, and ;'. This means that all vowels are divided into rounded and unnunded vowels, while the back or front position of the tongue proves irre ant for the classification of vowel phonemes, and consequently also for lie phonemic content of these phonemes.17 Ostyak, or more precisely, the Kasym dialect of Northern Ostyak, now elevated to the rank of a standard written language, has a two-class triangular system in word-initial syllables: Only unrounded vowels (/', e, e, and a) occur in all other syllables.18 In other words, the correlation of timbre is here neutralized in noninitial syllables, the unrounded vowels representing the archiphoncmcs of the corresponding oppositions (u-i, o-e, 3-e). Since the choice of the representative of the archiphoneme in this case is obviously internally conditioned, unrounded /, e, and e in the pairs u-i, o-e, a;. J d-e must be considered the unmarked opposition members. Lip rounding must therefore be regarded as the phonologically relevant correlation mark. As an example of such two-class triangular systems, in which only the correlation of tongue position is distinctive, the Japanese vowel system may be cited. In this system, the correlation of palatalization of consonants, that is, the opposition between palatalized and nonpalatalized consonants, is neutralized before the front vowels e and /', but retained before the back vowels u, o, and a. As a result, / and c arc put in opposition with the remaining vowels, that is, all vowels are divided into front and back vowels, lip rounding being irrelevant for the classification of the vowel phonemes, and hence for their phonemic content.19 The vowel system of Japanese and that of Artshi (w, o, a, e, i), already referred to, are therefore quite different phonologically, despite their apparent similarity. The correlation of tongue position alone is the phonological basis of the one, the correlation of lip rounding of the other. Jn addition to the two "extreme" classc of timbre, three-class vowel systems further contain a "medial" class nhich is phonetically realized either by unrounded back or central vowels, or by rounded front or central vowels. The medial class of timbre is most frequently represented by front lounded vowels. The relationship of the medial class of timbre to the 102 I PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF D1STINCT1VL OPPOSITIONS I PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 103 extreme classes is not identical in all languages. The presence of the medial class of timbre partly facilitates and partly complicates the analysis of complexes of properties that occur in the extreme classes. In a three-class vowel system a medial class of timbre that consists of front rounded vowels may be related more closely to the one or the other extreme class of timbre of the same vowel system, depending on the language involved. The closeness of the relationship is primarily expressed in the neutralization of the corresponding oppositions. Thus, for example, the oppositions y( = ii)-u, o-o, and ci-a are neutralizable in Finnish. y, 6, and a cannot occur after a syllable containing it, o, or a. Conversely it, o, and a cannot occur after a syllable containing y, d, and a. The oppositions u-i, y-i, o-e, and o-e, on the other hand, cannot be neutralized. In other words, only oppositions between front and back vowels (of the same degree of aperture) are neutralizable, while oppositions between rounded and unrounded vowels (of the same degree of aperture) are constant. After a syllable containing u, y, o, 6, a, or ii, therefore, only five vowels are possible in each case; that is, after u, o, and a, the vowels o e u i and after y, ii, and ci, the vowels a Quite a different distribution of classes of timbre is seen in a three-class vowel system, such as that of Polabian.20 In Polabian the correlation of palatalization was present in consonants. However, it was neutralized before all front vowels and before the maximally open vowel a which stood outside the classes of timbre. As a result, the back vowels u, o, and a acquired a special position in the system. The oppositions between the back and front vowels of the same degree of aperture were constant (non-neutralizable), while the oppositions between rounded and unrounded vowels of the same degree of aperture (ii-i, o-e) were neutralizable after /> and /, the archiphoneme being represented by unrounded / and e. As a result, the medial class of timbre was more closely linked to the front class. A certain hierarchy existed with respect to the correlation of tongue position and the correlation of lip rounding: (rounded unrounded The properties of lip participation were phonologically irrelevant for the back vowels.21 Graphically this may be represented as follows: o o, e u ii, i It seems that three-class vowel systems are comparatively rare, such as those of Finnish and Polabian, in which the medial class of timbre is more closely related to one of the extreme classes, thereby creating a certain hierarchy between the correlations of tongue positions and lip rounding. In most three-class systems that have front rounded vowels in the medial class of timbre, it is not possible to establish a closer relationship between that class of timbre and one of the extreme classes. For example, in standard German, Dutch, French, Norsvegian, Swedish, and Danish the three classes of timbre are opposed to each other as equidistant opposition members. As far as we know, there is also no reason to assume a closer relationship between the medial class of timbre and one of the extreme classes in Northern Albanian, Estonian, Ziryene,22 and Annamese.23 In K'uri, now Lezghian, in which the oppositions a-e and u-i are not neutralizable, while both the oppositions ii-u and ii-i are neutralized in certain positions (stressed ii cannot occur in a syllable after it or /, and stressed it and / in turn cannot occur in a syllable containing ii), the medial class of timbre is also equally closely related to both extreme classes.24 In the three-class vowel systems discussed so far, the medial class of timbre was represented by front rounded vowels. Systems in which the medial class of timbre contains back (or central) unrounded vowels are found much more rarely. As examples Romanian, Siamese,25 and Votyak ("Udmurt")26 may be mentioned. In systems of this type, too, there is sometimes a closer relationship between the medial class and one of the extreme classes of timbre. For example, in the East Sorbian (East Lusatian-Wendic) dialect of Muskau,27 described by Scerba, the opposition between vowels of the medial and front class of timbre is neutralized after non-guttural lingual consonants, that is, after dentals, palatals, sibilants, and 104 PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 105 r and / sounds: the unrounded central vowels ((Scerba's e')and e(Scerba's x) occur after d, t, n, I, r, s,z,c; the front vowels / and e, on the other hand, occur after 3', c\ z\ s', n, I, r, and j (while, for example, after labials, i and e as well as / and e may occur with distinctive function). The vowels of the medial class of timbre in this case are therefore more closely related to the vowels of the front (maximally clear) class. As for four-class vowel systems, they are found much more rarely than three-class systems. The vowel system found in many Turkic languages may be cited as an example: u ui u 1 In those Turkic languages in which the so-called vowel harmony is carried through consistently, the vowel system cited exists only in word-initial syllable in the above form, that is, with full phonological validity. In all other syllables the oppositions of timbre are neutralized. The realization of the vocalic properties of timbre in noninitial syllables is conditioned by the vowel of the preceding syllable. In the four-class vowel systems such a system as that of Eastern Cheremis28 must also be counted, in which the vowels with the minimal degree of aperture have four, with the mid-degree of aperture three, and with the maximal degree of aperture two, classes of timbre, so that the entire system contains nine vowel phonemes. The correlation of tongue position is neutralizable in all vowel pairs, while the correlation of lip rounding is neutralizable only in the vowels with the minimal degree of aperture.29 The particular vowel system could therefore be represented in about the following diagram (using the transcription as it appeared in Anthropos): But there are also those four-class vowel systems in which the oppositions of timbre cannot be neutralized at all, so that all four classes of timbre coexist fully autonomously and with full equality. The vowel system of Ostyak-Samoyed (now Selkup),30 in which not a single opposition is neutralizable, appears to belong to this type: a o 3 x C Properties Based on Degree of Aperture or Sonority Above we discussed the so-called linear vowel systems whose members only possessed properties based on degree of aperture but no properties of localization or timbre. The question now is whether there are also vowel systems whose members, conversely, possess only properties of timbre but no properties based on degree of aperture. J. van Ginneken believes that he can answer this question in the affirmative. As an example he cites the vowel systems of Lak, an East Caucasian language of Central Daghestan, and of "Assyro-Babylonian31 of the Achaemenid inscriptions." With respect to the latter, no opinion can be ventured at all since it is an extinct language. With respect to Lak, it can be positively shown that the vowel phonemes of this language contain not only properties of timbre but also properties based on degree of aperture. It is tnu. of course, that the three vowels of Lak are realized as 11, a, and / in mos. phonic positions. It is this circumstance that leads van Ginneken to assume that the u involved a "back rounded vowel in general," the / a "front unrounded vowel in general," and the a a "back unrounded vowei in general," so that the degree of aperture of these three vowels was phonologically irrelevant. However, in the neighborhood of strongly palatalized consonants the realization of all three Lak vowel phonemes changes: "w" in this position is realized as 0, "/" as e, and "a" as d.i2 Thus no opposition of timbre, but an opposition of degree of aperture, exists in this position between "/"" and "a." A comparison of the two variants of each vowel phoneme of Lak shows that with respect to "a" the maximal degree of aperture alone is important, while for "«" and "/," first, the minimal degree of aperture, and, second, a specific property of timbre, that is, for "«" the property of being rounded, for "/'" the property of being unrounded, are phonologically relevant. Lak can therefore by no means be used as proof for the existence of vowel systems without properties based on degrees of aperture. The same is also true of other languages with three-member vowel systems of the type "«," "a," "Z."33 In Arabic a clear opposition based on degree of aperture exists between "1" and "a" since "a" is mostly realized as a front vowel (unless it occurs in the vicinity o! emphatic consonants"). But after emphatic consonants the "a" sounds "dark," so that in that position it rather stands in opposition to "«" with respect to degree of aperture. Arabic "a" before "emphatic consonants" is realized as a back or back-central vowel (like the "a" in English "father"). But short "/" in this position is also pronounced as back-central /'. Therefore, in this case too, an opposition based on degree of aperture is found between "a" and ";."34 The same phonological properties as for the three vowel phonemes 106 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 107 of Lak discussed above must therefore also be assumed for Arabic "w," "a," and "/." In Modern Persian long "a" is normally pronounced with rounding, while short "a" has changed to "//." Long a in this case thus stands in an opposition based on degree of aperture with the corresponding maximally dark vowel (//), and short a with the corresponding maximally clear vowel (e).35 In other languages with only one "maximally dark," one "maximally clear," and, with respect to timbre, one "neutral," yowel phoneme the "neutral" vowel phoneme is also much more open than the other two. While there actually exists only an opposition of timbre between the "maximally dark" and the "maximally clear" vowel, both vowels stand in an opposition based on degree of aperture with the "neutral" vowel phoneme. In certain phonic positions this is particularly apparent. Accordingly there are no vowel systems without distinctive oppositions based on degree of aperture. This is of course only true with respect to "total systems": in "partial systems," that is, in those systems that exist only in a specific phonic position in a given language, it happens that oppositions based on degree of aperture are excluded. For example, in Russian only two vowel phonemes, that is, ; and u, occur in pretonic syllables after palatalized consonants as well as after tY, s, and f. The phonemic content of these vowel phonemes in this particular position consists only of their properties of timbre (i unrounded, u rounded). But this partial system does not have an independent existence. It exists only in connection with the partial system of the remaining unaccented syllables (u, a, and I) and with the partial system of the accented syllables (u, o, a, e, and /) which have oppositions based not only on classes of timbre but also on degrees of aperture. Every language has thus a vowel system with oppositions based on degrees of aperture. And just as all vowel phonemes with the same property of timbre form a "class of timbre" within a given vowel system, all vowel phonemes with the same degree of aperture (= degree of sonority) can be comprised under one "degree of sonority" within the same system. Vowel systems can accordingly be divided not only into "one-class" ( = linear), "two-class," "three-class," and "four-class" systems, but also into "two-degree," "three-degree," "four-degree" systems, etc. Two-degree vowel systems are not rare. The systems of Lak, Arabic, and Modern Persian have already been cited above. These are two-degree (and two-class) triangular systems. Schematically: Certain other vowel systems also belong to the same type, for example, the system of Tlingit and Haida (in North America)36 and Old Persian. But there are also two-degree quadrangular systems, for example, the vowel system of Tonkawa, in Texas,37 which has a back and a front class of timbre, the vowels of the back class being realized more openly than the corresponding front vowels. Thus there is no symmetry from a phonetic point of view: A two-degree, three-class quadrangular system, for example, is present in K'uri (Lezghian):38 As an example of a four-class, two-degree quadrangular system, the aforementioned vowel system of many Turkic languages can be cited: o a 6 a u in u i It is clear that in all two-degree vowel systems the opposition based on degree of aperture can be conceived of as a logically privative opposition—■ "low"/"nonlow" or "high"/"nonhigh." But since, as far as we know, the opposition based on degree of aperture does not seem to be neutralizable, it has not become an actually privative opposition anywhere.39 By far the majority of languages has three-degree vowel systems. A two-class, three-degree triangular system is found with varying realization in numerous languages in all parts of the world: for Europe, let us mention Modern Greek, Serbo-Croatian, Czech, (standard) Polish; for the Soviet Union, (standard) Russian, Erza-Mordvin, Georgian, Avar, Andi, Artshi, Tavgy-Samoyed ("Ngasan"); for Asia, Japanese and Tamil; for Africa, Lamba, Shona, Zulu, Ganda, and Chichewa; for America, Maya, etc.: u i u i 108 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 109 But three-class, three-degree triangular systems are not rare. Of the three-degree quadrangular systems the already-mentioned vowel system of the Montenegran dialects may be cited: a a In all three-degree vowel systems the individual degrees of sonority stand in a relation of gradual opposition with each other. The neutraliz-ability of a phonic opposition within such a system conforms to the rules that govern the neutralization of gradual oppositions, that is, either the "extreme" opposition member functions as the representative of the archi-phoneme or its choice is conditioned externally. Considerably rarer than three-degree vowel systems are four-degree systems. Still, they do occur in quite a number of languages in various parts of the world. As examples, the triangular system of Italian: and the above-mentioned quadrangular system of many Polish dialects may be cited: a a u y u i (in the traditional transcription of Polish dialectologists). Here, as in all vowel systems of more than two degrees, the individual oppositions based on degree of aperture are gradual oppositions. Special relations result where some of these oppositions are neutralizable. For, if the opposition between the two medial degrees of sonority is neutralizable, this opposition loses its gradual character and becomes a privative opposition. The opposition "mark" is then either the "closeness" or "openness," depending on which opposition member represents the archiphoneme. For example, in the Scottish dialect of Barra Island (Hebrides)40 a four-degree vowel system exists, but only in word-initial syllable. The medial oppositions o-o and e-x are neutralized in the remaining syllables, so that in that position there occur only the open vowels o and x. These vowels may thus be considered the unmarked opposition members. The correlation o-o, e-x must then be designated as a "correlation of c! >seness." However, where the neutralizable opposition of sonority contacts one of the "extreme" degrees of sonority, that is, either the maximal or minimal degree, the gradual character of the opposition is not chant ~d. In Danish the oppositions M-o, y-o, and i-e are neutralized before a preconsonantal nasal (and before rj). There is also a clear tendency to neutralize the same oppositions before r.41 Nevertheless, Danish o, 6, and e cannot be considered open u, y, i. The situation is somewhat different where this type of neutralization affects the entire vowel system. This is the case in Ibo, Southern Nigeria.42 This language has a two-class, four-degree vowc! system, in which, on the one hand, the oppositions based on degree of aperture between the vowels of the first and second degree of sonority are neutralizable. On the other hand, the vowels of the third and fourth degree of sonority are neutralizable in such a way that there exists a proportion "1:2 = 3 :4." A word can only contain vowels of the first and third degree or of the second and fourth degree of aperture. All affixes (prefixes and sullixes) follow the vocalism of the stem in this regard. Accordingly all oppositions based on the degree of aperture are equipollent in this system :43 f open.....o-a 4th degree of aperture 0W \ close.....o-e 3rd degree of aperture ,. , f open.....u-e .... 2nd degree of aperture l° \ close.....u-i .... 1st degree of aperture One can divide the words, .or stems, or roots of this language into an "open vowel" category and a "close vowel" category, and the affixes into a "low vowel" category and a "high vowel" category. But none of these classes can be considered unmarked or marked. As already mentioned, four-degree vowel systems are much rarer than three-degree systems. • Five-degree vowel systems may be considered special rarities. In Europe such systems exist in Switzerland, for example, in the Kerenz dialect of the Canton Glarus.44 In Africa, Fante on the Gold Coast seems to have a five-degree (two-class) triangular system: u, u, o, j, a, e, e, i, i.4S Gweabo. in Liberia, seems to have a six-degree (two-class) triangular system, if the opposition between aright" and "muffled" vowels of this system can be evaluated as an opposition based on the degree of sonority.46 In Gweabo there exists a type of "vowel harmony" 110 PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS that presupposes the neutralizability of the oppositions between the first and second, the third and fourth, and the fifth and sixth, degrees of sonority. The rules for vowel harmony are here much more complicated than in Ibo. In any event they presuppose the following division of the entire system (we leave E. Sapir's transcription unchanged): low mid high "bright" "muffled" "bright" "muffled" "bright" "muffled" . a . 6th degree of aperture O - E . . . 5th degree of aperture e . 4th degree of.aperture e 3rd degree of aperture e . 2nd degree of aperture / . I st degree of aperture In every vowel system the maximally dark and the maximally clear class of timbre always contains the same number of degrees of sonority. This is valid without reservation for quadrangular systems. In triangular systems the vowel with the maximal degree of sonority, which is outside the classes of timbre, is to be added. A four-degree quadrangular system, for example, must therefore contain four vowels of the maximally dark, and four vowels of the maximally clear, class of timbre, while a four-degree triangular system contains only three dark and three clear vowels and in addition a maximally open vowel. In quadrangular systems individual oppositions based on degree of aperture are usually neutralized in both the maximally dark and the maximally clear class of timbre. The result of such neutralization is then always another "quadrangular" partial system (with fewer degrees of sonority). In triangular systems the neutralization of a particular degree of aperture may take place in the two "extreme" classes of timbre, which again creates a "triangular" partial system; or it may take place only in one of the two extreme classes of timbre, in which case the partial system is quadrangular. For example, in certain dialects of Modern Greek the opposition between the first and second degree of sonority of a three-degree, two-class triangular system is neutralized in unstressed syllables,47 resulting in a two-degree triangular system in that position: stressed unstressed In the North Great Russian dialects, on the other hand, where stressed syllables also possess a three-degree, two-class triangular system, the opposition a-e is neutralized in unstressed syllables. The representative of PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 111 the archiphoneme is conditioned externally (e after palatalized consonants, a after nonpalatalized consonants). This gives rise to a two-degree quadrangular system:48 a stressed o e u i unstressed u i Examples can easily be multiplied. In three-class vowel systems the medial class of timbre cannot contain more vowel phonemes than either of the extreme classes. An equal number of vowels in all three classes is found primarily in triangular systems, for example, in the Mongolian system:49 In three-class quadrangular systems, on the other hand, the medial class of timbre almost always contains fewer vowel phonemes than either of the extreme classes. (See, for example, the above-cited vowel systems of Finnish and K'iiri or Lezghian.) The same relation is not rare for triangular systems either. See, for example, the Norwegian vowel system:50 as well as the analogously structured, but differently realized, vowel systems of Polabian, Annamese, the Scottish dialect of Barra Island, and (with an unrounded central series) the East Sorbian dialect of Muskau, referred to above and described by Scerba. When the medial class of timbre contains fewer degrees of sonority than either of the extreme classes, it usually lacks the equivalent to the most sonorous vowels of the extreme classes of timbre. At any rate, the minimal degree of sonority always appears to be fully represented in three-class systems, that is, by three vowel phonemes. It follows from what has just been said that the medial class of timbre of a three-class system can sometimes also be represented by a single vowel phoneme. I n this case such a phoneme must have the same degree of aperture 112 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 113 as the minimally sonorous vowels of the extreme classes of timbre. Examples of this type are indeed not lacking. The vowel system of K'iiri (Lezghian), which was already mentioned, may be cited: u u Middle Greek had a three-degree triangular system, in which the medial series was represented by ii alone: In Tubatulabal, an Indian language of the Shoshonean group of the Uto-Aztecan family, an analogous system still exists today, with the difference that an unrounded i occurs instead of an u.51 These are by no means the only examples of this type. Neutralization of individual oppositions based on degree of aperture in the three-class vowel systems follows the same rules as in the two-class systems, but the medial class of timbre may not contain any more phonemes in the partial system than in either of the extreme classes of timbre. Since oppositions of timbre are sometimes also neutralizable, it often happens that a two-class (or even a linear), two-degree partial system exists besides a three-class, multidegree total system. For example, the already-cited three-class, three-degree vowel system of Mongolian occurs only as such in first syllables: In noninitial syllables following a syllable containing an /, the opposition ii-o is neutralized, and the following partial system results: Finally, after a syllable with any other vowel (except /) the oppositions of timbre u-ii, 0-6, o-e, and o-e, on the one hand, and the oppositions based on degree of aperture o-a, d-a, e-a, on the other, are neutralized. As a result, the following partial system arises: u 152 All this is valid only for the long vowels of Mongolian. In short vowels all oppositions of timbre are neutralized after a syllable containing an /, so that a three-degree linear system results: After a syllabic containing any other vowel the system shrinks even more, and only two short vowels, "/" and "e," remain. The latter takes on the quality of the vowel of the preceding syllable. The statement was made above that where the medial class of timbre of a three-class vowel system was represented by a single vowel phoneme, this phoneme had the minimal degree of sonority, and in this respect formed a group with the minimally sonorous vowels of the extreme series of timbre u and /. This rule applies without exception where a rounded front vowel is involved: if the vowel system contains only one such phoneme, it is always u, never 0. But there are cases where in addition to the vowels of the extreme classes of timbre a multidegree vowel system contains still another, unrounded vowel phoneme which does not belong to any of these classes of timbre, and which has neither the maximal nor the minimal degree of sonority. Since such a vowel phoneme can therefore be characterized only negatively, it may be designated as an " indeterminate vowel."53 This phoneme must not be confused with the sole representative of the (unrounded) medial class of timbre: the latter stands in a relation of pure (isolated bilateral) opposition of timbre with u and /', while the "indeterminate vowel" does not stand in a bilateral opposition relation with any other phoneme of the vowel system. In any event, it does not participate in any pure opposition of timbre. . Many languages in various parts of the world have an indeterminate vowel in the above-defined sense in stressed as well as in unstressed syllables. The vowel may be long or short: the vowel in the English word bird (in standard Southern English) may be reg.- ■-■d as a long indeterminate vowel. But in many languages the indeterminate vowel only appears in partial systems in those phonic positions where several oppositions based on degree of aperture and oppositions of timbre are neutralized. PHONOLOGICAL CI ASM S Ol DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 115 It follows from what has been said that the indeterminate vowel must be considered not as the sole representative of a specific medial class of timbre but as a vowel phoneme that lies outside any class of timbre. As a result thereof the indeterminate vowel may enter into a special relation to that vowel of maximal degree of sonority which characterizes triangular systems and which also lies outside any class of timbre. Under certain circumstances an "indeterminate" vowel in a triangular system can thus become a "specific" \owcl by entering into a relation of bilateral opposition with "«." Such a case is present, for example, in-Bulgarian. The Bulgarian indeterminate vowel has approximately the same degree of aperture as o and e, but it is neither rounded nor palatal. It would hardly be possible to assume a pure opposition of timbre between Bulgarian d and o, or between Bulgarian a and e. But the proportions o:a = u:j,e:a =/':,?, and the proportion u:o = i:e = jut deduced therefrom may well be established. The conditions in unstressed syllables (at least in a part of the local types of pronunciation) are proof that this proportion corresponds to a reality. For in these syllables o, e, and a are not permitted, only u, i, and 3 are. In other words, the oppositions based on degree of aperture u-o, i-e, and s-a are neutralized, while the triangular character of the vowel system is preserved. Graphically, this may be represented as follows: stressed a o e unstressed u i The Bulgarian vocalism is thus a three-class triangular system, in which the medial class of timbre is characterized by its neutral character and by an increase in the degree of aperture of its members.54 The Bulgarian vowel system appears to be a rather rare case, fn the other languages with which we are familiar no special bilateral relation can be ascertained between this vowel and the "a." There is accordingly no reason to include the indeterminate vowel and the "a" in a special medial class of timbre. Not much can be said about the number of degrees of sonority in four-class vowel systems since vowel systems of this type are in general extremely rare. According to our knowledge, in such systems none of the medial classes of timbre can contain more degrees of sonority than either of the extreme classes. (The total number of vowel phonemes of the two medial if I' classes of timbre cannot therefore exceed the total number of vowel phonemes of the two extreme classes.) In the above-cited vowel system of Eastern Cheremis the lowest degree of sonority is represented in all four classes of timbre. This system is proof that the two medial classes of timbre of a four-class system need not necessarily contain the same number of degrees of sonority. Also very closely related to the study of the properties based on degree of aperture is the difficult problem of the position in the vowel system of monophonematically evaluated diphthongs. The situation is simplest in such cases as the vowel systems of the archaic Great Russian and North Ukrainian dialects, where the phonemes that are represented by u> and e in Russian dialectology are realized as diphthongs of movement with an increasing degree of aperture (approximately like ub, ie). These diphthongs begin somewhat lower than the maximally high vowels of the same system, but they do not end with as great a degree of aperture as that found in undiphthongized o and e of the same system. The position of these phonemes in the vowel system can therefore evoke no doubts: such vowel systems are four-degree triangular systems in which "to" and "e" represent the second degree of sonority (u, w, o, a, e, e, and /'). The oppositions co-o and e-e are neutralizable in the dialects in question. In unstressed syllables where these oppositions are neutralized, the respective archi-phonemes are represented by o and e. (This is true, at least, of the North Great Russian and the North Ukrainian dialects which have an "o>" and an "".) It follows that in this case diphthongization or, more precisely, the decrease in vowel height must be regarded as the correlation mark. Equally clear is the position of the diphthongs "oa" and "ea" in Daco-Romanian, where they obviously stand between o and e, on the one hand, and a, on the other:55 ea In the Slovenian dialect of Carinthia, north of the Drau (in the dialect of the so-called Drauci), the diphthongs uo and ia, which end less high than they start, are obviously to be classed between u and ;, and o and e, whereas oa and ea evidently lie between o, e, and the maximally open a, and a. Accordingly there exists a five-degree quadrangular system:56 116 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 117 a a oa ea o e w3 id u i More difficult is the classification of those monophonematically evaluated diphthongs in which one part is more open and the other higher than the neighboring vowels of mid-degree of aperture. German and Dutch present a case of this type. The three German diphthongs "au," "eu" and "ei" can be grouped into the three classes of timbre of the German vowel system, but it is impossible to accommodate them within the system constituted by degrees of sonority. The fluctuation and indeterminacy of the degree of aperture of these phonemes, which is due to flexibility in articulation, may well be considered their specific mark. It distinguishes them from all other long (i.e., unchecked) vowel phonemes of the German language. The long vowel phonemes must therefore first be divided into two categories: those with a "stable" and those with a "movable" degree of aperture. Further classification according to the three classes of timbre can then be carried out in both categories. Classification according to the three degrees of sonority, on the other hand, can only be carried out in the category of vowels with a stable degree of aperture.57 The diphthongs of English present particularly complicated problems, even if one limits oneself to the form of the modern language as codified by Daniel Jones.58 Recently several attempts have been made to interpret the vowel system of this form of English phonemically; in chronological sequence these were by Josef Vachek (1933),59 Bohumil Trnka (1935),«" A. C. Lawrenson (1935),61 and Kemp Malone (1936).62 The so-called short vowels appear to offer no difficulties: all four investigators agree that in technical terminology these vowels form "a two-class, three-degree quadrangular system." In English the opposition mark seems to be tongue position rather than lip rounding. Difficulties appear in regard to the so-called long vowels and diphthongs (or triphthongs). However, these difficulties seem to have arisen primarily because the English vowel system had been treated without considering the peculiarities of the English prosodic system. In English "quantity" is a prosodic opposition based on type of contact. A vowel is "short" if its pronunciation is interrupted^ the beginning of the following consonant, "long" if its pronunciation is undisturbed and displays its full extent. The description by Daniel Jones indicates that of the "unchecked" vowel phonemes in English only a: and a: have no diphthongal variants. All remaining unchecked vowel phonemes have diphthongal variants, in other words, they have variants that are characterized by a movable degree of aperture. Such variants are only optional for e: and o:, and they are used much more rarely than in the case of /': and «:. Still, they do exist, and this is sufficient. In the variety of modern English described by Jones no fundamental difference can be recognize-- between the "true" diphthongs and the "long monophthongs" (with the exception of a: and a:). Both are unchecked vowel phonemes with a movable degree of aperture. The only unchecked vowel phonemes with a stable degree of aperture are maximally open a: and indeterminate a:, that is, those unchecked vowel phonemes that stand outside the classes of timbre. The flexibility in degree of aperture in the variety of English studied is thus related to "uncheckedness" as well as to membership in a specific class of timbre. On the basis of the preceding, a principle of classification can be established for the vowel phonemes with a movable degree of aperture, namely, the direction of articulatory movement {Ablaufsrichtung). Some unchecked vowels have a centripetal, others a centrifugal, direction of articulatory movement. I.e., some move back to a (neutral) center position63 from a point characterized by the marks of a specific class of timbre, while others move in the direction of the extreme representative of the specific class of timbre. In German we can designate the former as vowels with an articulatory movement toward the center (hineinablaufende Vokale), the latter as vowels with an articulatory movement away from the center (hinausablau-fende Vokale). It is significant that a: and rich stand outside the system of timbre, in the center so to speak, have an immovable degree of aperture. In the remaining unchecked phonemes in English it is possible to determine the relative degree of aperture of the starting point. Both classes of timbre64 have three degrees of sonority. In the case of the vowel phonemes with an articulatory movement away from the center, these are uw (= u:), ou, and au in the dark class of timbre, and /',' -= /':), ei, and ai in the clear class of timbre. In the case of the vowel phonemes with an articulatory movement toward the center, the first degree of sonority is evidently represented by ua and id. To the second we assign a: and e:, which actually also have the optional variants 3d and ed and which, on the basis of their phonological content, should rather be considered the realization of the vowels moving toward a neutral center ('Vj. The phonemes that Jones designates as triphthongs aua and aid, and for which he lists the optional variants ad, aa and a:, a: respectively, should probably be considered as having the third degree of sonority.65 Since the maximally open a stands 118 I PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS Jfr PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 119 outside the classes of timbre, the entire English system of unchecked vowel phonemes must be regarded as a "four-degree, two-class triangular system containing an indeterminate vowel." However, due to the fact that the two directions of articulatory movement are distinguished in every class of timbre, the total number of unchecked vowel phonemes is not eight but fourteen: a: aid ai Oil 3d e? ei As to the diphthong oi, it is considered a phoneme combination by all the above investigators, except A. C. Lawrenson. But the positive arguments Lawrenson raises in favor of his monophonematic interpretation do not seem to carry much weight (see Kemp Malone, op. cit., p. 160, no. 4).66 While in standard German and Dutch there are only few unchecked vowels that have a movable degree of aperture and are always "articulated away from the center," most unchecked vowel phonemes in English are characterized by the movability of their degree of aperture and, in addition, present an opposition based on the direction of articulatory movement. It is possible that similar conditions also exist in certain other languages or dialects, in particular in those languages that have a prosodic structure based on the same principle as that of English. In any event, all languages that have a large number of diphthongs of movement must always be examined as to the question whether differences in direction of articulatory movement similar to those found in English are not significant for them. D Resonance Properties While the properties of localization and the properties based on degree of aperture of the vowels are so closely linked with each other as to form a kind of "bundle," the resonance properties belong on quite a different plane. By the term "oppositions of resonance" we understand all "distinctive oppositions" between "pure" vowels and vowels that are somehow "impure." a. The correlation of nasalization The correlation of nasalization is the most common.67 In many languages it exists for all vowels. The nasalized vowels, of course, need not be fully identical with the corresponding nonnasalized vowels with respect to tongue, lip, and jaw positions. Only sameness of position in the system is important. For example, in Burmese the long nasalized vowel phonemes with the second and third degree of sonority are realized as diphthongs, while the corresponding nonnasalized vowels are realized as monophthongs:68 nonnasalized nasalized c dii an ai ei i u In many languages the correlation of nasalization extends only to part of the vowel system. Often one of the mid degrees of sonority remains unaffected by this correlation. This is true, for example, of the variety of Scottish spoken on Barra Island:69 nonnasalized a o x o o e u y i or of Northern Albanian:70 a s nonnasalized o o e nasalized it y i Sometimes not the mid, but the maximally high, vowels are exempted from nasalization, as, for example, in French: a a nasalized 5 & ii y i a e it y i nonnasalized d E o o e v nasalized In all of these cases all classes of timbre are represented in the nasalized vowels. There are languages with two-class vowel systems which have only two nasalized vowels. This is the case, for example, in the Jauntal dialect of Slovenian spoken in Carinthia (nasalized vowels d and a),71 and in the 120 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions Kashub dialects (o and a).72 In other languages no specilic degrees of aperture, but certain classes of timbre, are excluded from nasalization. In the Central Chinese dialect of Siang-tang (Honan Province) only unrounded vowels are nasalized: nonnasalized nasalized e ,"73 In the dialect of Marchfeld the vowels of the medial class of timbre as well as the vowels of the second mid degree of aperture are exempted from nasalization:74 a a au dii di au ai nonnasalized o q e nasalized o e ode u i u ii i The number of nasalized vowels is thus never greater than the number of nonnasalized vowels. It may happen that a language contains only a single "nasalized vowel." For such a vowel neither a particular class of timbre nor a particular degree of aperture is relevant. These could only become relevant in contrast with other nasalized vowels. The coloration of such a single nasalized vowel is therefore determined by its consonantal environment alone. Its aperture is not present at all. In other words, such an "indeterminate" nasal vowel is nothing but a syllabic nasal that is assimilated to the following consonant. In sketches on African languages where such phonemes occur, they are usually represented by the letters m, n, tj, etc. But it is very questionable whether this phoneme can really be identified with m, n, etc. It must be kept in mind that in most such languages consonant combinations do not occur at all (or that only the combinations "obstruent + liquid" are permitted). The phonemes in question can therefore only form distinctive oppositions with the vowel phonemes, while hi, /i, etc., stand in a relation of direct, distinctive opposition only to the other consonants. Furthermore, the "syllabic nasal" in the particular African languages shows the same distinctive differences of tone (differences of tonal register) as the vowels. All this favors the view that in cases such as lbo "mbe" (bisyllabic, high-tone m, low-tone e [turtle]) the "syllabic nasal" may be considered an "indeterminate nasalized vowel." phonological classes of distinctive o. cositions 121 However, even with this interpretation certain difficulties remain. For in languages such as lbo, Efik, Lamba, Ganc.i, etc., which do not have any nasalized vowels nor a nonnasalized indeterminate vowel, the "syllabic nasal" stands in a relation of distinctive opposition to the vowels only, but this relation is always multilateral. In such a case, the "syllabic nasal" can probably be regarded as an "indeterminate vowel in general." Its nasalization, however, is a purely phonetic, phonologically irrelevant property. In languages such as Ewe, Yoruba, Fante, etc., on the other hand, where the correlation of nasalization comprises the entire vowel system, this "syllabic nasal" would have to be grouped with the category of nasalized vowels. A curious situation would result: the system of nasalized vowels would then contain one phoneme more than the system of nonnasalized vowels, which would contradict everything we know about the correlation of nasalization. b. The correlation of muffling The correlation of nasalization is probably the most common, but by no means the only, correlation of resonance. Whether there exists only one or several other additional types of oppositions of resonance is very difficult to say at the present stage of investigation. The languages in which distinctive oppositions between "pure" and somewhat "muffled" vowels exist are "exotic" languages. The notations that one finds about them by observers, usually better trained and more interested in ethnology than in linguistics, are, for the most part, rather unclear.75 Subject to this reservation, we continue to use the term "correlation of muffling" (or opposition of muffling), without taking up the question of whether this always involves the same or different correlations in the various languages. Recently the phonetic aspect of the problem has been considerably advanced, at least for Africa. Dr. A. N. Tucker, who had studied and mastered the pronunciation of the "pure" and "muffled" vowels in the Nilotic languages, was himself subjected to an instrumental phonetic study by Panconcelli-Calzia in Hamburg. The results showed that in the case of the "squeezed" vowels the faucal pillars are compressed and the velum is lowered, without, however, enabling the flow of air to escape through the nose. In the case of the "breathy" vowels the velum is raised, the fauces retracted, and the larynx clearly lowered, so that quite a large cavity is formed behind the oral cavity proper. The glottis appears to be in the position of whispering.76 Dr. Ida C. Ward made the same observations for the Abua language of Southern Nigeria: this language also involves the opposition of vowels with a compressed pharynx and vowels with a wide-open pharynx, resulting in a "flat" sound.'7 It seems that in certain 122 PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS dialects of Modern Indie the same phonetic basis for the "correlation of muffling" can be determined. In any event, J. R. Firth here also speaks of an opposition between "tight" and "breathy phonation."7S The phonetic nature of vowel muffling in certain Fast Caucasian languages, on the other hand, remains unclear from the description by A. Dirr. Of the corresponding vowels of Tabarasan it is claimed that they have laryngeal friction and that, in comparison with other vowels, they have an energetic expiration.7'' Of the corresponding vowels of Aghul it is said that the larynx is compressed in their articulation. This produces a slight noise of laryngeal friction.s,) The "correlation of muffling," like the correlation of nasalization, also extends either to the entire vowel system or only to a specific part thereof. The former seems to be the case in Nuer, a Nilotic language of the Egyptian Sudan.81 possibly also in other Nilotic languages, while in Abua, according to Ida C. Ward, the correlation of muffling is present only in the vowels e and o; in Tabarasan, according to A. Dirr, only in u and a, and probably also in Aghul, where "muffled" u is realized as a type of o. (Unmuffled o does not occur as an independent phoneme in words of native origin.) The above-mentioned languages with two nasalized vowels can be compared with these cases. In all types of "oppositions of resonance " the rules for monophonematic and polvphonematic interpretation must be observed with particular stringency. Phonetically nasalized vowels are very often only the realization of the phoneme sequence "vowel + nasal," and the vowels accompanied by a noise of laryngeal friction are only the realization of a combination of a vowel phoneme with a laryngeal consonantal phoneme. 4 CONSONANTAL PROPERTIES A Properties of Localization (l.okalisierungseigenscltaftcn) There is no language in which the properties of localization of the consonantal phonemes would be phonologically irrelevant. There are, of course, individual consonantal phonemes without distinctive properties of localization in every language. But these always lake some special position in the system because they deviate from the "norm." Several consonantal phonemes of a given language may be equivalent to each other with regard to the instinctive properties of localization (and distinguish themselves from each other only by the properties based on the manner of overcoming an obstruction or the properties of resonance). By aeries of localization is meant the s'im of all consonants with the same distinctive properties of localization, regardless of whether such a series consists of several consonants or only of a single one. Within a system of consonants PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS 123 the individual classes of localization stand in various relations of opposition to each other. a. The basic series. Those consonantal series of localization that stand in a relation of heterogeneous multilateral opposition to each other, we call "basic series." Some of these basic series occur in almost all languages of the world. They are the gutturals ("dorsals"), the apicals ("dentals"), and the labials. We do not know of any languages that do not have apicals. Gutturals do not occur, for example, in some Slovenian dialects of Carinthia. Labials arc absent in Tlingit (Alaska). But these are extremely rare cases. Except for these, the three mentioned series of localization occur in all languages of the world. This certainly cannot be an accident. It must have some basis in the makeup of these three series. It is probably easiest to seek an explanation in the fact that the lips, the tip of the tongue, and dorsum of the tongue arc movable organs that are best suited for obstructing the oral cavity. Thus for the labial series the bringing together of the lips is relevant; for the apical series the participation of the tip of the tongue, the tongue itself being extended, and a frontal position of articulation; and, finally, for the guttural series the participation of the back of the tongue, the tongue being contracted, and a back position of articulation.82 These three positions of the vocal organs may be considered the "most natural," but by no means in the sense of being "innate." It is well known that children must first acquire these positions laboriously. The sounds that are spontaneously produced by children in the babbling stage for the most part only remotely resemble labials, apicals, and gutturals. The three types of consonants mentioned are "natural" only in the sense that they solve most easily and naturally, with the aid of the movable parts of the oral cavity, the task of producing different sounds that have their own individual character and that are clearly discriminated from each other. This may also explain their universal (or near universal) presence in the world. Just as universal as the labials, apicals, and gutturals are the sibilants. The only language known to this author in which an "s" is almost completely absent is Eastern Nuer in the Egyptian Sudan. The grooved shape of the tongue surface distinguishes sibilants from the apicals which are produced with the tongue tintly extended, and from the gutturals which are produced with the tongue arched and contracted. The grooved shape of the tongue surface gives special direction to the airstream, creating a specific acoustic effect. But since the upper and back portion of the resonating cavity is approximately the same for sibilants and apicals, these two series of localization show a certain relatedness, and in some languages they unite into a single series under certain circumstances. 124 phonological classes of distinctive oppositions phonological classes of distinctive oppositions 125 In addition to the four series of localization commonly found and referred to above, there are some languages that have still other basic series. One of these is particularly the lateral series, which occurs as a special series of localization in many North American and some African languages (Zulu, Pedi, Herero, Sandawe, etc.).83 A type of intermediary series of localization between the guttural and the labial series is represented by the series of localization that is usually called "labiovelar." As far as we know, it exists in the above form only in the so-called Sudan languages, and, it seems, in certain Japanese dialects. This scries is characterized by simultaneous labial and guttural occlusion. We would prefer to call it "gutturo-labial." A type of intermediary series between the guttural and the apical is represented by the palatal series of localization, which occurs in very many languages in all parts of the world. In many languages it can be considered a basic scries, but in some languages it enters into a bilateral relation with the guttural or the apical scries. The phonetic realization of the palatal series, too, differs from language to language.84 Lastly, the laryngeal series of localization must be regarded as a basic series on a par with the others, at least for a part of the many languages in which it occurs. In addition to the four universal (or near-universal) basic series, that is, the labial, guttural, apical, and sibilant, there thus exist four less common series, namely, the lateral, the gutturolabial (= labiovelar), the palatal, and the laryngeal. However, the phonological concept of series of localization must not be confused with the phonetic one of position of articulation. For example, in Czech a relation of neutralizable opposition exists between voiced laryngeal h and voiceless guttural a- ("ch"), which is fully analogous to the relation "voiced"/"voiceless." x and Ar, however, stand in a bilateral proportional relation to each other {x:k = s:c = s:c). The h in Czech thus does not belong to a special laryngeal series, which does not even exist in that language. It belongs to the guttural series, for which, from the standpoint of the Czech phonological system, only the fact that lips and tip of tongue do not participate is relevant.85 In Grcenlandic Eskimo86 all spirants have occlusives as "partners." These belong to the same series of localization: s-c, x-k, .x-q, f-p. The lateral spirant A alone has no "occlusive partner." Since, however, there is no closer spirantal equivalent of the apical occlusive the t is evaluated as the "occlusive partner" of A, that is, the lateral egress of air in A is irrelevant for Greenlandic. Its apical articulation alone is relevant. Examples of this type can easily be multiplied. One can speak of a particular lateral, palatal, or laryngeal scries in the phonological sense only if the phonemes in question do not stand in a relation of proportional bilateral opposition to any phoneme of another localization series. In cases where, as in the ex mples cited above, there exists a bilateral opposition between consonants of different positions of articulation, and that opposition is proportional to analogous relations between phonemes of the same scries of localization (Czech and Slovak A-.v = z-s = z-s, Greenlandic /-A = p-f' = A-.v = ;-.v = c-s), both members of the opposition in question must be assigned to one series of localization. Cases in which two series of localizaL n stand in a relation of bilateral opposition to each other are not to be confused with the above. b. Equipollent related series. Each of the above basic series stands in a relation of multilateral opposition to the other basic scries. In certain languages, however, two series occur for some of these basic series, and these stand in a relation of bilateral equipollent opposition to each other. Instead of a single labial series, characterized by the par icipation of the lower lip, a labial and a labiodental series may occur. Both are labial, but at the same time they remain distinct from each other. This is the case, for example, in standard German, where the labial series is represented by b, p, and in, the labiodental series by v,f, and /). It is even more pronounced in Shona, a language spoken in Rhodesia, where the occlusives p and b are contrasted with the spirant j3 in the bilabial series, and the occlusives (affricates) p and b with the spirant v in the labiodental scries.87 Many languages have two apical series, one characterized by the tip of the tongue pointed upward, the other by the tip of the tongue pointed downward, instead of a single series characterized by the participation of the tip of the tongue. Depending on the language, this relation can be expressed as opposition between "retroflex" and "plain" apicals,88 or as opposition between "alveolars" and "interdentals,"89 or, finally, as opposition between "dentals" and "prepalatals."90 The relation itself remains identical in all cases: the tip of the tongue is always relatively higher in the realization of the one series than in the realization of the other. Instead of a single "guttural" series, characterized by participation of the dorsum, many languages have two distinct dorsal series: a postdorsal series and a pre-dorsal series. This is the case in many North American languages, for example, in Kwakiutl, Tlingit, and Haida; in Eskimo and Aleut; also in the so-called Paleo-Asiatic languages (Chukch.. Koryak, Kamchadal, Gilyak, Kettic); and in all Caucasian idioms. Or ounded and unrounded gutturals stand in opposition to each other, as in Tigre, in Ethiopia.91 Instead of a single sibilant series, an 5 and an .v series occur. Such a "split" in the sibilant series is very common among European languages: English, French, German, Italian, Hungarian, Albanian Romanian, all Slavic languages, and Lithuanian and Latvian. This pi omenon is rather common in other parts of the world also. Finally, the laryngeal series, which is phonological classes of distinciivl oppositions characterized by the passivity of all mouth organs, may also be replaced by two series: a purely laryngeal series and a pharyngeal series, as found, for example, in Somali, in the Semitic languages, and in some North Caucasian languages. As for the palatal series, in some systems it stands in a relationship of bilateral opposition to the apical or the dorsal series and must then be evaluated either as "a series with the tip of the tongue lowered," or as a "predorsal series." The bilateral nature of an opposition is proved objectively by its neutralizability. In Czech, Slovak, Hungarian, and Serbo-Croatian, where the opposition between dentals and palatals is ncutralizable, these two series of phonemes can be considered "a split" in the apical series. In the Central Chinese dialect of Siang-tang (Honan Province), where the opposition between velar and palatal consonants is ncutralizable in certain positions (before u, a, /, d, and i),'n these two series must be considered "splits" in the dorsal series. All cases discussed above thus involve a "split" of a basic scries into two "related series" which stand in a relation of bilateral opposition to each other, but in a relation of multilateral opposition to all other series of localization in the same system. It must be stressed, however, that there can be a question of such a split in a basic series only if the context of the entire system requires it. Spirants frequently do not have the same position of articulation as occlusives. For example, in Modern Greek there exist, on the one hand, bilabial, postdcntal, dorsal, and sibilant occlusives (tr, r, k, tct), on the other, labiodental, interdental, dorsal, and rill spirants (
r, and u toward 6. A foreign observer tends to notice these concomitant phenomena only: he hears the hoarse laryngeal glide after the consonant as well as the hoarse, clearer, and more open pronunciation of the surrounding vowels. But these concomitant phenomena are irrelevant for the phonemic system of the particular language. Only the specific consonantal coloring is important, which a foreign observer learns to notice only after prolonged practice.
Just as the palatal series cannot exist as an autonomous series of localization in languages with simple palatalization, because it is inevitably interpreted as a "palatalized apical" or "palatalized guttural" series, so the "glottal" (or "pharyngeal") series must be interpreted as a "palatalized laryngeal" series in languages with a correlation of emphatic palatalization.
From the correlation of emphatic palatalization it is necessary to distinguish the correlation of emphatic relarization that plays an important role in the Semitic languages, especially in Arabic. The Arabic "emphatic" consonants are characterized by a thickening of the root of the tongue, which at the same time causes a shift of the larynx. The opposition between "emphatic" and "nonemphatic" consonants is found in the apical, guttural, sibilant, and laryngeal series. It is accompanied in all series by specific shifts in the position of articulation: the "emphatic" apicals are not only velarized (in the above-defined sense), but arc also alveolar in contrast with the postdental nonemphatic apicals. Likewise in the case of the emphatic sibilants, the tip of the tongue is raised higher than in their non-emphatic equivalents. The emphatic gutturals are postdorsal or even uvular, while nonemphatic k is predorsal or palatal. In certain dialects of the Egyptian Sudan the voiced equivalent of nonemphatic A is almost marginally palatal. Finally, the emphatic laryngeals are closer to being pharyngeal, while the nonemphatic laryngeals are pure laryngeal sounds.105 However, these concomitant differences in the position of articulation
132
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
133
must be ignored. For in the phonemic system of Arabic the emphatic velarized consonants form a closed category, which is placed in opposition to the category of the nonemphatic consonants. What makes the correlation of emphatic velarization in Arabic somewhat opaque is the fact that it docs not comprise all consonants of the respective series, and further that it cannot be neutralized:
nonemphatic / d 8 b n k g -
? h - b f in r I
emphatic
Sa -
Consequently, whether the phonemes q, y, and x are to be interpreted as "emphatic gutturals" or as a special postvelar (uvular) series, and whether h and tj are "emphatic laryngeals" or whether they form a special pharyngeal series, is subject to debate. But since similar questions do not arise with regard to the apicals and sibilants, one may probably assume the correlation of emphatic velarization in the case of the gutturals and laryngeals as well, and accordingly designate ,v, q, y, h, tj, as x", ka, g", h", and fya. In languages that have a correlation of consonantal timbre, all bilateral oppositions between series of localization which permit such an interpretation are considered privative with respect to the particular correlation of timbre.
The case is much simpler and clearer for the correlation of rounding or labialization. It occurs as the sole correlation of timbre in some languages of the Northern Caucasus (Kabardian, Ch'ak'ur, Rutulian, Lezghian, Aghul, Artshi, and Kubachi), in Kwakiutl (North America),106 and possibly also in some African (in particular Bantu) languages. In Kwakiutl this correlation extends only to the two guttural series. In the languages of the Northern Caucasus, in which this correlation is found, it occurs also mainly with respect to front and back gutturals but is not limited to this series. In Kabardian and Lezghian it includes also the apical series; in Ch'ak'ur, Rutulian, and Aghul, the apical series and both sibilant series; and in Artshi the lateral series as well.
The various correlations of timbre tend to combine into bundles. We are only familiar with bundles that are produced by the combination of the correlation of palatalization with the correlation of rounding. They are found in Circassian. Ubyk, Abkhas, Dungan Chinese, Korean, and Burmese. The bundles do not occur in all series. For example, in Adyghe, the 5 series alone has three types of timbre (s, s', s°), the s series has only the
correlation of palatalization, the two guttural series and the apical series only the correlation of rounding. (The labial, lateral, and laryngeal series do not have any differences of timbre.)107 In standard Abkhas three types of timbre occur in both guttural series and in the s series, while the 5 series occurs only with the correlation of palatalization, the apical and the laryngeal series only with the correlation of labialization, and the labial series with no differences of timbre at all.108 In Burmese the labial series alone has three series of timbre (p, p , and p°), while the remaining series, that is, the two apical, the guttural, the sibilant, and the palatal ones, have only the correlation of labialization.109 But in Korean all series of localization appear to participate in both correlations of timbre. The transparency of the system is increased here by the fact that the entire correlation bundle can be neutralized.110 In all cases discussed so far, the combination of the correlation of palatalization and labialization results at the most in three-member bundles. In the Bsyb dialect of Abkhas, however, the s sounds indicate four classes of timbre (neutral, simple-palatalized, simple-labialized, and "i/'-colored " palatalized-rounded). A similar case seems to be on hand in Kinyarwanda, a Bantu language described by P. P. Schumacher (Anthropos, XXVI): four classes of timbre are distinguished in the bilabial series and, it seems, also in the s series (only three in the apical and in the s series; only two, i.e., f-f° and v-v° in the labiodental series).111
A different type of correlational bundle of timbre should probably be posited for Sanskrit. Since any reduction in the front resonator results acoustically in a reinforcement of the higher partial tones, and consequently in a clearer timbre, it is evident that the timbre of the "dental" occlusives and sibilants of Sanskrit must have been higher than that of the "cerebrals" and lower than that of the "palatals." However, not only the opposition between "dentals" and "palatals" but also the opposition between "dentals" and "cerebrals" was neutralizable and consequently bilateral. It is therefore possible that in this case a correlation bundle existed. The opposition between "dental" and "palatal" occlusives (t-c, th-ch, d-j, dh-jli) and between s and c could then be interpreted as a correlation of palatalization (similarly as in Ukrainian or Mordvin, for example). The opposition between "dental" and "cerebral" occlusives (/-/, th-th, d-d, dh-dli), nasals («-«), and s-s, on the oth r hand, would have to be considered a special "correlation of rein xion." The characteristic feature of the "cerebral" phonemes would tonsist of the elongation of the front resonator of the mouth (i.e., of the space between the highest point of the tongue and the orifice of the mouth), resulting from the retraction and retroflexion of the tongue, and the corresponding lowering of timbre of the respective consonants. The entire bundle has, of course,
134
phonological classes of distinctive oppositions
a certain gradual character. The question as to what extent the bundle of timbre which existed in Sanskrit can also be posited for other languages, must remain unanswered for the present. Much depends on whether the opposition between "dentals" and "palatals" is bilateral in the language in question. This can be proven objectively only by its neutralizability.
p) The click correlation is geographically much more limited in scope. Even in those areas where it is found it extends only to a few languages. It occurs only in a few Southern Bantu languages. Of these, Zulu is the most important. It is found further in the genetically isolated Hottentot and Bushman languages, which are also spoken in South Africa; and, finally, it occurs in Sandawe in the KJIimatinde district of East Africa, which again is geographically and genetically isolated.
The phonetic aspect of click sounds is well studied at the present. Good instrumental phonetic recordings and detailed descriptions are available. Recently a whole monograph appeared in which the "click problem" was discussed from various points of view.112 Roman Stopa, the author, discusses the phonetic nature of the click sounds in detail. He develops hypotheses as to the origin of these sounds and the origin of language in general. But he does not even raise the question of the position of the click phonemes in the phonemic system of the respective languages. The small essay by P. de V. Pienaar is very valuable.11-' Although it does not clarify the phonemic problem, it at least contributes new reliable and essential phonetic material. A recently published study by D. M. Beach,114 in which the phonetic, and in part also the phonemic, character of the clicks is placed in a new light, is very commendable. Thanks to this excellent study, we now have at our disposal an absolutely reliable description of Hottentot, that is, of all its main dialects: Nama, Damara, Griqua, and Korana. Of the other languages concerned, Zulu has been studied most completely from the point of view of phonetics. The basic work on the sounds of this language by Clement M. Doke,115 though itself not phonological in our sense, nevertheless makes it possible to work out the phonemic system without great difficulties. For Sandawe, too, the phonemic system can be worked out in its general outline (at least with respect to the consonants).116 The same may also be said for the description by A. N. Tucker of the phonetics of the Suto-Chuana group.117 The situation is somewhat more difficult with respect to Bushman, which is generally regarded as "the click language par excellence." The copious notations by Wilhelm Heinrich Bleek118 are an extremely important source for the study of Bushman. But the fluctuating and inconsistent transcription of the sounds of the Bushman language makes it extremely difficult, if not impossible, to infer its phonemic system, at least without the commentary of the collaborator
phonological classes of distinctive oppositions
135
of this deserving Bushman scholar. Although P. Meriggi succeeded in bringing a certain order to this confusion,"'1 complete clarity has by no means been achieved.
The problem the phonologist encounters with regard to the click sounds of the African languages is the following: Is the opposition between click phonemes and nonclick phonemes in these languages an opposition of localization or an opposition based on the manner of overcoming an obstruction? Phoneticians who studied the physiological nature of click sounds have interpreted and treated the specific properties of these sounds as properties based on type of articulation. The "clicking" (avulsive) type of articulation of these sounds was compared with other types of articulation (ingressive, implosive, ejective, etc.). The comparison was a general one, without regard to the consonantal system of a particular language. The phonologist, however, must study the position of the click phonemes in the consonantal system of individual languages. Such a study leads to the following results. In Zulu, which has apical, palatal, and lateral clicks, there also exist nonclick apicals, palatals, and laterals. Disregarding the clicks for a moment, one will find that in every series of localization, including the apical, palatal, and lateral, there exists a voiced consonant, a recursive occlusive, a voiceless aspirated occlusive, and a nasal.120 Mutatis mutandis, the same oppositions also exist with respect to the three "click" series: each of these has a click with a voiced (soft) vowel onset, another in which the vowel has a "hard" onset (glottal plosion), a third in which the vowel has an aspirated onset, and, finally, a nasalized click. All oppositions between these various types of clicks are distinctive. Accordingly the apical clicks, the palatal clicks, and the lateral clicks in Zulu form a special series, which represents a parallel with the respective nonclick series. In Bushman, where the same four types of clicks occur (i.e., with a voiced soft, a voiceless hard onset, an aspirated vowel onset, and nasalization), the same four types of articulation are found for the corresponding nonclick consonants as well. Accordingly here too a relation of parallel series exists between the apical and palatal clicks and nonclicks. A similar relationship can also be shown for Sandawe, as will be demonstrated further below. The relation of the "click series" to the "nonclick series," which was noted for Zulu, therefore seems to be characteristic also for "click" languages in general. If the distinction between "click" and "nonclick" articulation would consist only in the relation between ingress and egress of air, it would, of course, be impossible to classify this distinction according to oppositions of localization. But more recent phonetic studies have shown that "click sounds" always require a specific shape of the tongue. In addition to the basic closure that is formed either
136
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
137
by the lips or by the anterior parts of the tongue, and which produces the various types of clicks (labials, dentals, retroflexes, palatals, and laterals), each click has still another, so-called supplemental closure which is always velar (i.e., it is produced by raising the posterior part of the dorsum against the soft palate). The presence of two closures, of which one must be velar and the other somewhere in the anterior part of the oral cavity, is part of the nature of click sounds. A suction act rarefies the air in the space between these two closures. Upon release of the anterior closure, outside air rushes into this air-starved space. But immediately thereafter the posterior velar closure is released. From a phonetic point of view, all these properties of the click sounds are equally important. However, from a phonological point of view, the presence of the velar occlusion, in addition to another closure (labial, apical, palatal, etc.), and the resultant specific modification of the shape of the tongue, and hence the configuration of the entire oral resonating cavity, are most important. This circumstance makes it possible to interpret the difference between click and nonclick articulation as an opposition of localization, more specifically, as an opposition between basic and secondary scries. Since this opposition is logically privative and occurs in several series of localization of the same system, it may be designated as "click correlation."
The presence of a velar "supplemental closure" quite naturally produces a shift in the position of articulation of the front part of the tongue. It is therefore at times very difficult to pair a click series with a particular nonclick series. In Bushman nonclick consonants have a labial, an apical, a dorsal, a palatal, a sibilant, and a laryngeal series. The click consonants, on the other hand, have a plain-apical, a "cerebral," a palatal, and a lateral series. A click correlation can therefore be determined here at first glance only for the apical and the palatal series. However, what has been said by D. M. Beach (op. cit., pp. 81 ff.) about the corresponding phonemes of Hottentot, namely, that retroflexion of the tip of the tongue is optional and not essential, can probably be repeated for the "cerebral" clicks of Bushman. Important for cerebral clicks is solely that in comparison with the "dentals" and "palatals" they are shifted further backward, so that a comparatively large "empty" space, that is, a space not filled by the tongue, is formed in the front part of the mouth. The relationship that thus exists between "dental" and "cerebral" clicks may be compared with the relationship between apical and guttural nonclicks. The "cerebral" clicks may be considered a secondary series of the guttural series. The system of clicks in Hottentot, as described by Beach, pages 75 to 82, can be summarized as follows: There arc two series of plosive clicks. In one, which is the "dentialveolar series," according to Beach, and the "palatal series,"
according to earlier observers, the tongue fills the anterior part of the oral cavity up to the teeth. In the other, which is the "alveolar series," according to Beach, and the "cerebral series," according lo earlier observers, an empty cavity remains in the anterior part of the moiuh. In addition to these two "plosive" series, two "affricate" series are found which stand in exactly the same relationship to each other as the "plosives." In other words, in the one, namely, the "dental" series, the front part of the oral cavity is filled by the tongue, while in the other, namely, the lateral scries, it is not. In releasing the anterior closure in the i.ase of the "plosive" series, the tongue is simply torn away from the palate, while in the case of the "affricate" series it permits the air to penetrate gradually: from the front in the "dental" series, from the sides in the lateral series. It is clear that the opposition between "affricate" and "plosive" series is not an opposition of localization. Accordingly there are actually only two click series of localization in Hottentot, one characterized by a completely filled-out anterior part of the mouth (by the tongue), the other by an empty anterior part of the mouth. The nonclick consonants of Hottentot are divided into labials, apicals (including sibilants), gutturals, and laryngeals. The labials and laryngeals obviously stand outside the click correlation. In the remaining series the apical nonclicks correspond to the "clicks with a filled-out anterior cavity," and the guttural nonclicks to the "clicks with an empty anterior cavity." Thus there exists in Hottentot a correlative relation between the click and nonclick series of localization as well.
In connection with the click correlation, a type of secondary series must still be discussed, namely, the "correlation of full gutturalization"* and the "correlation of labiovclarization." These correlations are found in certain Bantu languages, namely, in the Shona group, and in the neighboring Venda.121 A correlation of full or pure gutturalization exists in the opposition between nonvelarized consonants and consonants in which, in addition to the basic articulation, a secondary guttural articulation takes place, which consists of raising the dorsum of the tongue against the soft palate. The tongue can be raised high enough so as to practically form a velar closure. (This is usually the case in the Zc/uru dialect of Central Shona.) Or it may be raised somewhat lower so that it results only in a velar stricture. (This is characteristic for the other dialects of Eastern and Central Shona, especially for the Karanga subgroup.) In the Zezuru dialect this correlation is found in the bilabials and palatals. The correlation of
* Translator's note: The term gutturalization instead of veiarization is used here in view of N. S. Trubetzkoy's apparent preference for this term.
138
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE: OPPOSITIONS
139
labiovelarization is a combination of the correlation of full gutturalization and the correlation of labialization. It occurs independently of the correlation of full gutturalization in all dialects of Eastern and Central Shona, in the apicals, palatals, and the two sibilant series. The acoustic impression of the fully gutturalized and labiovelarized consonants on a foreign observer is that of combinations of consonants (pk, ck, tku; cku; or p.x, c.x, t.xw; c.xw respectively). They must nevertheless be evaluated as mono-phonematic since the languages in which they occur do not permit any other consonantal clusters. If one compares the clicks and the fully gutturalized (or labiovelarized) consonants, one arrives at the conclusion that the difference is only phonetic, not phonological. The suction element, which, at first glance, seems to be characteristic of clicks, is only a special way of releasing the anterior oral closure. For the position of the click sounds in the phonemic system it is much less important than the presence of the velar "supplemental closure." But the latter is also present in the pure gutturalized and labiovelarized consonants of Zezuru and the other dialects of Eastern and Central Shona, though perhaps not in quite as energetic a form.
In summary, it can be said that the localization properties may form rather complicated systems of oppositions. The basic series stand in a relation of multilateral (heterogeneous) opposition. But in many languages some of these basic series are split into two related series each. These stand in a relation of bilateral equipollent opposition to each other, and in a relation of multilateral opposition to the other (basic or related) series of the same system. Finally, each series of localization can be split into series that stand in a relation of (actually or logically) privative opposition to each other. If such a split comprises several series of localization in the same consonantal system, it represents a correlation, which may be either a correlation of consonantal timbre or a click correlation.
d Consonantal phonemes outside the series of localization. In many, and possibly in most, languages there are consonantal phonemes that stand outside the localization series (or at least outside the noncorrelative series of localization). The "liquids" and "/;" are usually among these consonantal phonemes. But one should not generalize this statement. The liquids and the h can sometimes also be incorporated into the series of localization. Above we have already mentioned Gilyak, where the r must be considered a voiced continuant of the apical series.122 In Eskimo, where the r is always realized as a uvula1- and without a trill, it takes the same position in the postdorsal series as lhe ir takes in the labial scries and the y in the predorsal series. In the apical series this position is taken by /, which has a voiceless spirant A as its counterpart, so that the following system results:125
9
/
k .x
V
q c .V s r
('") (") ('/) ('/)
In languages that have only one liquid and that have a palatal series of localization, the ir can be interpreted as the labial, the y as the palatal, and the sole liquid as the apical sonorant. But whether such an interpretation is correct can be established only where it is substantiated by the way the system functions or by grammatical alternation. For example, in Mende (Sierra Leone), where the / is the only liquid, / and / are in grammatical alternation. This alternation takes place under the same conditions as the alternation p - ir. Accordingly a proportion /:/ = />:u- may be set up.124 In Chichcwa, where the only liquid is realized sometimes as an r and sometimes as an /, it becomes d following the prefix in or /;. Under the same conditions v is replaced byy and ir by />.125 In these instances there exists objective proof that the "sole liquid" belongs to the apical series. But in cases where similar proof does not exist the classification of single liquids into a particular localization series is always subject to doubt. In languages that have more than two liquids, it is not uncommon that at least one or two liquids belong to specific scries of localization. For example, in Serbo-Croatian (Stokavian) the relationship between / and / is obviously analogous to the relationship between n:h, f.c. and d:d. This justifies grouping / with the "dental," and / with the "palatal" series. Thus only the r remains outside the series of localization. The case of Tamil will be discussed later (pp. 141 f.).
Most languages of the world have only two liquids. Only in extremely rare cases can these be grouped with any localization series.126 They generally stand outside these series. They form a bilateral opposition which can be interpreted as logically privative. The relation /•-/can then be considered either "trilled"/ "untrilled" or "nonlateral"/ "lateral." In a language such as Italian, where the r is always a trilled vibrant, the first interpretation is probably the more suitable one, while in German, where the "untrilled" varieties of the /' sound arc very frequently realizations of the r phoneme, the second interpretation would be the only one possible. But insofar as the opposition ;•-/ cannot be neutralized in a given language, it remains only logically privative. The opposition between /• and / is then at any rate not an opposition of localization but an opposition based on the manner of overcoming an obstruction. This is true even for languages such as German, where r is the "nonlateral" and / the "lateral" liquid. From a phonological point of view, the lateral articulation can be
Hi If
140
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
PHONOLOGICAL CLASSES OF DISTINCTIVE OPPOSITIONS
141
considered a localization property only if it is shared by several phonemes whose remaining distinctive marks are the same as the properties based on the manner of overcoming an obstruction of the phonemes of other basic (or related) series of the same system (as is the case, for example, in Pedi, Sandawe, Tlingit, Chinook, Adyghe, Avar, etc.)- However, in languages that have only a single lateral phoneme, and in which that phoneme stands in a relation of bilateral opposition to the r that lies outside the localization series, the lateral articulation (i.e., the unimpeded'frictionless passage of egressing air between the side of the tongue and the "side wall" of the oral cavity) must be considered a special manner of overcoming an obstruction. The ambiguous character of lateral articulation, which causes such difficulties in phonetic systemization, is something that can quite easily be resolved in phonological systemization, the more so since the important thing here is only to establish to which other phoneme the particular "lateral" phoneme stands in a relation of opposition, and to determine the nature of such an oppositive relationship.
As for h, in many languages it is the "indeterminate consonantal phoneme in general." In many others, however, it is grouped with a particular series of localization. This may be either the "guttural series," which in such a case is characterized by the fact that the tip of the tongue and the lips are not involved in articulation, or it may be a particular laryngeal series. The latter is the case primarily where the same system contains a laryngeal plosive (glottal stop) that stands in a relation of bilateral opposition to /;. In Danish, where /i occurs only in those phonic positions in which (voiceless) unaspirated lenes b, d, and g are in opposition with the aspirated fortes p, t, and k, h obviously stands in the same oppositive relationship to the unaspirated vowel onset as t, and k stand to b, d, and g.l2y A laryngeal series could, therefore, be posited here, in which h would be the "aspirate" (or "fortis"). In German, on the other hand, where the relationship between h and unaspirated vowel onset is not paralleled by the relationship between p, t, k and /', d, g, h must be considered an "indeterminate" phoneme which stands outside any series of localization (/; is voiced intervocalically, while p, t, and k are voiceless in that position; h does not occur in final position, while p, t, and A- in that position represent the archiphonemes of the neutralized opr