Editor: STEPHEN PRINCE ric juu 11 id i for Movies and Mind Bergbahn Journals SSUE 2 • WINTER 2012 Editor: STEPHEN PRINCE The Journal for Movies and Mind Published in association with Society for Cognitive Studies of the Moving Image, and Forum for Movies and Mind projections The Journal for Movies and Mind VOLUME 6 ISSUE 2 • WINTER 2012 FROM THE EDITOR ARTICLES THE NATURE OF FILM COMEDY, OR WHY \SSHAUN OF THE DEAD FUNNY? □irk Eitzen DON, PEGGY, AND OTHER FICTIONAL FRIENDS? ENGAGING WITH CHARACTERS IN TELEVISION SERIES Robert Blanchet and Margrethe Bruun Vaage FILM EVALUATION AND THE ENJOYMENT OF DATED FILMS Robert R. Clewis WHERE DOES THE BEGINNING END? COGNITION, FORM, AND CLASSICAL NARRATIVE BEGINNINGS Jason Gendler EMBODIED VISUAL MEANING: IMAGES SCHEMAS IN FILM Maarten Coegnarts and Peter Kravanja THE ARCHIVE EFFECT: ARCHIVAL FOOTAGE AS AN EXPERIENCE OF RECEPTION Jaimie Baron BOOK REVIEWS SG Berghahn Journals NEW YORK ■ OXFORD www.journals.berghahnbooks.com/proj Projections: The Journal for Movies and Mind is an interdisciplinary, peer-reviewed journal that explores the ways in which recent advancements in fields such as psychoanalysis, cognitive psychology, neuroscience, genetics and evolution help to increase our understanding of film, and how film itself facilitates investigations into the nature and function of the mind. The journal also incorporates articles on the visual arts and new technologies related to film. The aims of the journal are to explore these subjects, facilitate a dialogue between people in the sciences and the humanities, and bring the study of film to the forefront of contemporary intellectual debate. Projections is published in association with Society for Cognitive Studies of the Moving Image (SCSMI) and Forum for Movies and Mind (FMM). Copyright © 2012 Berghahn Books I55N: 1934-9688 (Print) • ISSN: 1934-9696 (Online) All rights reserved. Reproduction, storage, or transmission of this work in any form or by any means beyond that permitted by Sections 107 and 108 of the U.S. Copyright Law is unlawful without prior written permission from the publisher, or in accordance with the terms of licenses issued by the Copyright Clearance Center (CCC) and other organizations authorized by the publisher to administer reproduction rights. Subscriptions (non-membership) 2011 Subscription Rates • Volume 6/2012, 2 issues pa. Institutional Rate (Print and Online): $i8o.oo/£iio.oo/€i29.oo Institutional Rate (Online Only); $i62.oo/fg9.oo/€n6.oo Individual Rate (without membership): $70.oo/£44.oo/£50.oo Membership (with subscription) SCSMI Membership and Subscription: $65.oo/f44.oo/€45.oo SCSMI Student Membership and Subscription; $35.oo/£22.oo/€25.oo (please provide proof of enrollment) FMM Membership and Subscription: $65.oo/£44.oo/€45.oo FMM Student Membership and Subscription: $35.oo/£22.oo/€25.oo (please provide proof of enrollment) For membership in the Society for Cognitive Studies of the Moving Image (SCSMI), go to: http://scsmi-online.org/membership All other inquiries concerning subscriptions and backfiles should be directed to your nearest Berghahn Journals distribution office: UK/Europe: Berghahn Journals, Ltd., c/oTurpin Distribution, Pegasus Drive, Stratton Business Park, Biggleswade, Beds. SGi8 8T0, UK US/Rest of World: Berghahn Journals, Inc, c/oTurpin North America, 143 West Street, New Milford, CT 06776, USA Email: turpinna@turpin-distribution.com Online Projections is available online at journals.berghahnbooks.com/proj, where you can browse online tables of contents and abstracts, purchase individual articles, order a sample copy, or recommend the journal to your library. Also visit the Projections Web site for more details on the journal, including full contact information for the editors and instructions for contributors. Advertising All inquiries concerning advertisements should be addressed to: advertising@journals.berghahnbooks.com projections The Journal for Movies and Mind EDITOR Stephen Prince Theatre and Cinema, Virginia Tech ASSOCIATE EDITORS Carl Plantinga Communication Arts and Sciences, Calvin College Uri Hasson Psychology Department, Princeton University EDITORIAL BOARD Richard Allen, Cinema Studies, New York University Andreas Bartels, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Germany David Bord well, Emeritus Communication Arts, University of Wisconsin Yadin Oudai, Weitzmann Institute, Israel Cynthia Freeland, Philosophy, University of Houston Krin Cabbard, Comparative Literature, State University of New York at Stony Brook Torben Crodal, Film and Media Studies, University of Copenhagen Tom Cunning, Art History, University of Chicago Adrienne Harris, Postdoctoral Program in Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy, New York University Patrick Colm Hogan, English, University of Connecticut Norman Holland, English, University of Florida Bonnie S. Kaufman, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University Vicky Lebeau, English, University of Sussex, England Daniel T. Levin, Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University Leon Levin, University of Maryland School of Medicine Paisley Livingston, Philosophy, Lingan University, Hong Kong Gilbert J. Rose, Program in Psychoanalysis and the Humanities, Yale University Andrea Sabbadini, Institute of Psychoanalysis, London Steven Shaviro, English, Wayne State Bruce Sklarew, Forum for Movies and Mind Greg Smith, Communication, Georgia State University Murray Smith, Film Studies, University of Kent Vivian Sobchack, School of Theater, Film, and Television, UCLA Jeffrey M. Zacks, Psychology, Washington University in St. Louis Founding Editor: Ira Königsberg Professor Emeritus of Film, University of Michigan Bruce Sklarew Co-founder of The Forum for Movies and Mind VOLUME 6 • ISSUE 2 • WINTER -2 01 CONTENTS FROM THE EDITOR Articles THE NATURE OF FILM COMEDY, OR WHY IS SHAUN OF THE DEAD FUNNY? Dirk Eitzen DON, PEGGY, AND OTHER FICTIONAL FRIENDS? ENGAGING WITH CHARACTERS IN TELEVISION SERIES Robert Blanchet and Margrethe Bruun Vaage FILM EVALUATION AND THE ENJOYMENT OF DATED FILMS Robert R. Clewis WHERE DOES THE BEGINNING END? COGNITION, FORM, AND CLASSICAL NARRATIVE BEGINNINGS Jason Cendler EMBODIED VISUAL MEANING: IMAGE SCHEMAS IN FILM Maarten Coegnarts and Peter Kravanja THE ARCHIVE EFFECT; ARCHIVAL FOOTAGE AS AN EXPERIENCE OF RECEPTION Jaimie Baron Book Reviews Thomas Deane Tucker and Stuart Kendall, eds., TERRENCEMALIK: FILM AND PHILOSOPHY Jason Wesley Alvis Jason Horsley, THE SECRET LIFE OF MOVIES: SCHIZOPHRENIC AND SHAMANIC JOURNEYS IN AMERICAN CINEMA Robert N. Matuozzi Dean K. Simonton, GREAT FLICKS: SCIENTIFIC STUDIES OF CINEMATIC CREATIVITY AND AESTHETICS Daniel T. Levin From the Editor As it developed, cinema gained in popularity by offering pleasures that viewers found easy to experience and understand. Faced with an uncommonly vivid and accessible medium, moviegoers responded enthusiastically to comedies and dramas, and filmmakers learned to craft stories and characters and to design images and image transitions that made intuitive sense and that sustained the fictional worlds on screen and the pleasures engendered by an immersive visual experience. While viewers find most movies easy to understand, scholars, appropriately, do not. The intersection of movies and mind is complex and multi-factoral, encompassing social, psychological, and biological domains. This issue of Projections highlights this complexity by focusing on the psychological, social, and physiological constituents of meaning and emotion in cinema. Dirk Eitzen explains why zombies can be funny; his analysis ofShaun of the Dead integrates brain imaging studies with a humanistic understanding of the social and psychological underpinnings of humor, with results that clarify not just this film but also some fruitful connections between the sciences and the humanities. DVDs renewed the popularity and appeal of television series because they made long-form viewing available in a concentrated form. Viewers could spend hours at a time watching multiple episodes of favorite series. A serial format, arguably, makes possible a different kind of parasocial relationship viewers and characters than does cinema, where individual feature films are the normative format. Robert Blanchet and Margrethe Bruun Vaage illuminate the pleasures of long-form televisual narratives, in particular the manner in which relationships between viewers and characters assume the characteristics of a friendship. Movie advertising emphasizes current attractions, but movie lovers understand that dated films—movies that contain archaic elements—can be uniquely enjoyable. Robert Clewis examines the ways that dated films can evoke reactions of nostalgia, boredom, or comic amusement from viewers. Influencing aesthetic and affective meanings, datedness is engendered when one or more attributes of a film fails to achieve their ostensible aims because of the passage of time. Projections Volume 6, Issue 2, Winter 2012: v-vi © Berghahn Journals doi: io.3l67/proj.2012.060201 ISSN 1934-9688 (Print), ISSN 1934-9696 (Online) embodied sual meaning / 85 ied Visual leaning: Image Schemas in Film Maarten Coegnarts and Peter Kravanja Abstract: This article examines embodied visual meaning in film, the ways that film makes use of recurring dynamic patterns of our shared bodily interactions with the world (image schemas) to communicate abstract meaning to the viewer. Following the lead of recent discoveries in the field of neuro-science, the article argues that this metaphorical transference of abstract thought by means of image schemas is possible via the activation of embodied mirroring mechanisms in the observer. This empathetic and physical encounter of the viewer with the representational content and form of the work is crucial to the understanding of abstract conceptual thought in film. Keywords: aesthetic experience, conceptual metaphor, embodied mind, embodied aesthetics, filmic metaphor, image schemas, mirror neurons In the perception of shape lies the beginnings of concept formation. —Rudolf Arnheim 0g6g) Proponents of conceptual metaphor theory (CMT) claim that knowledge of abstract phenomena is only possible by linking these to concrete experiences. The conceptual metaphor mind is body is at the core of this theory (La koff and Johnson 1999).1 According to Lakoff and Johnson, thought is fundamentally embodied, that is human beings use physical experiences to metaphorically structure thoughts about abstract phenomena. For example, we speak about time in terms of space ("the deadline is ahead of us," "time flies by," "we are nearingthe end of the year") or we describe emotions in terms of forces ("she fought against her tears," "he suppressed his fears"). In order to further emphasize their claim of an "embodied mind," both Johnson (1987) and Lakoff (1987) point to image schemas. These recurring patterns of our sensomotoric experiences (for example in-out, front-behind, and part-whole) play a crucial role in answering the following question: How is abstract, philosophical thinking possible? Conceptual metaphor theory needs to address the following problem: the existence of image schemas is based nearly exclusively on b Projections doi: io.3i67/proj.2012.060206 Volume 6, Issue 2, Winter 2012: 84-101 Subject Figure 5. Dualistic model of aesthetic experience Freedberg and Gallese's theory is, however, limited in one important way. Challenging the primacy of cognition in responses to art, Gallese and Freedberg define aesthetic experience almost exclusively in terms of a physical relation. Little attention is given to the abstract and cognitive dimension of aesthetic experience. Their dualistic approach mostly focuses on the physical connectivity between the work and the viewer, while in fact this physical relation could in turn form a bridge to the construction of meaning on a higher, more conceptual level. For example, in Professione: Reporter, the aesthetic feelings are not limited to a physical relation between the viewer and the work (based on the SPG schema). The conformity between the form of the film, on the one hand, and the subject's body, on the other hand, constitutes an opening to the conveyance of abstract meaning (the exposition over time) where the body functions as the source domain for the metaphoric clarification of the abstract target domain. The image schema that is expressed in the form (a camera movement, a concrete expression of the work) and that metaphorically refers to the concrete repertoire of the viewer's experiences (physical actions such as swimming or walking), is then again applied metaphorically in order to convey an abstract phenomenon. In other words, the aesthetic experience that is the result of a physical intertwining of the subject and the object (as seen in Freedberg and Gallese) can trigger a further cognitive application of the aesthetic experience. Note that this opening to a deeper, conceptual level is entirely determined by the body. The abstract content cannot be formed without the physical mediation between object and subject. Through the image schema, the abstract always refers to the physical, so that the former cannot be seen separately from the latter. The cognitive aspect of the aesthetic experience always implies a sen- The image schema that is expressed in the form (a camera movement, a concrete expression of the work) and that metaphorically refers to the concrete repertoire of the viewer's experiences (physical actions such as swimming, walking, etc.), is then again applied metaphorically in order to convey an abstract phenomenon. sory aspect. Taking these observations into account, Freedberg and Gallese's dualistic model can be extended to a three-part model (see Figure 6): Artwork (content - form) MNS Subject Image schema Image schema Image schema Abstract conceptual thought Figure 6. Three-part model of aesthetic experience Conclusion In his 1873 essay "Uber das optische Formgefuhl: Ein Betrag zur Asthetik," Robert Vischer distinguishes between Sehen and Schauen. The former he describes as the mere passive and unconscious experience of physiological stimuli where the stimuli received have not yet been transformed and ordered into a meaningful, living whole. It is the task of the process of Schauen to achieve that transformation and order. Vischer typifies Schauen as follows: Scanning fSchauenJ is more conscious than mere seeing (Sehen), for it sets out to analyze the forms dialectically (by separating and reconnecting the elements) and to bring them into a mechanical relationship. Scanning alone makes a complete artistic presentation possible, for its movement.. .is accompanied by an impelling animation of the dead phenomenon, a rhythmic enlivening and revitalization of it.... Once I have accomplished the process of scanning, the impression of seeing is repeated on a higher level. What I have seemingly separated I have reassembled into an ordered and restful unity. Again I have an enclosed, complete image, but one developed and filled with emotion. To chaotic "Being"I called "Become!" (1994:94) This article has focused on the second form of viewing. We have shown how filmic images are anchored in bodily experience via image schemas. This thesis is biologically and neurologically justified by the recent discovery of mirror neurons, which may serve as an explanatory model for the empathic exchanges between object and subject. The result of this process is a kind of harmony between the viewer as a physical subject and the filmic images. Our IT 98 / PROJECTIONS EMBODIED VISUAL MEANING / 9 9 analysis of specific filmic images has allowed us to conclude that this conformity between the two can be applied metaphorically to convey abstract meaning. Embodied image schemas such as container, SPG schema, center-periphery, , verticality, and balance are applied metaphorically in order to express abstract phenomena such as power struggles, psychological conditions, and the movement of time. Therefore, our analysis allows us to appreciate both the cognitive and the physical dimension of aesthetic experience. In this way, our article offers a nuanced middle road between the purely cognitive and the purely physical. Let us conclude by quoting Warren Buckland (2000: 51): "Perception is not a process that involves a relation between the eye and the mind (whether conscious or unconscious); more fundamentally, it involves the metaphorical projection of the body on screen and in frame." Maarten Coegnarts has an MA in Film Studies and Visual Culture from the $ University of Antwerp and MA in Sociology from the University of Antwerp. His research primarily focuses on metaphor in film and embodied visual meaning. He also has a special interest in film analysis and in the relation between film and philosophy. I Peter Kravanja holds MS and PhD in Mathematical Engineering and Computer 1 Science from the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, MA in Cinema Studies from 1 the Universite de la Sorbonne Nouvelle—Paris 3, and BA in Philosophy from ~» the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven. His research interests include analytic phi- m losophy of art applied to cinema, filmic metaphors, questions concerning m analysis, interpretation, and form, as well as the relation between film and the a other arts. m Notes 1 In cognitive orconceptual metaphor theory it is common to use small capital letters to indicate that these particular wordings are not a matter of language, but of concepts, belonging to the realm of human thought. These concepts are underlying the.very nature of our daily metaphorical expressions (linguistic or otherwise). 7 According to Kant ([1781/1787] 1999), the schema—which he described as "an art hidden in the depths of the human soul"—was the third instance allowing the application of purely cognitive concepts (categories) on sensory experiences. ■ 3 We refer to the distinction made by German philosopher Martin Heidegger between Vorhandensein and Zuhandensein. The former marks the relation between man and thingas a conscious and theoretical one. The latter has its meaning flowing forth from the way we relate to all things in our daily existence. 4 For a similar reflection on this scene—albeit not from a metaphorical angle—«e Giin-ther (2004).; s These reflections are partly inspired by the insights of French filmmaker Claude Chabrol who analyzes a few scenes from the film in the short documentary Fritz Lang par Claude Chabrol (2003), which can be viewed as an extra feature on the Region 2 DVD of You Only Live Once (EAN: 5050582501704). 6 We refer to the observations on art theory made by Aby Warburg and Maurice Mer-leau-Ponty. See Rampley (1997) for a discussion by Aby Warburg relating to Robert Vischer. References Arnheim, Rudolph. 1969, Visual Thinking. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. Blumenberg, Hans. 1998. Paradigmen zu einerMetaphorologie. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp. Bordwell, David, 1985. Narration in the Fiction Film. London: Routledge. Bordwell,:David. 2008. Poetics of Cinema. New York: Routledge. - Branigan, Edward. 2003. "How Frame Lines (and Film Theory) Figure." Pp. 59^86 in Film Style and Story: A Tribute to Torben Crodai, eds. Lennard Hojbjerg and Peter Schepelern. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press. Branigan, Edward. 2006. Projecting a Camera: Language-Games in Film Theory. New York: Routledge. Buckland,,Warren. 2000. The Cognitive Semiotics of Film. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Collingwood, Robin George. 1938. The Principles of Art. London: Oxford University Press: Deane, Paul D. 1995. "Metaphors of Center and Periphery in Yeats' The SecondComing." Journal ofPragmatics24 (6): 627-642. Dodge, Ellen, and George Lakoff. 2005. "Image Schemas: From Linguistic Analysis toNeural Grounding." Pp. 57-91 in From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics, ed. Beate Hampe. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Forceville, Charles. 2009. "Non-verbal and Multimodal Metaphor in a Cognitivist Framework: Agendas for Research." Pp. 19-42 in Multimodal Metaphor, eds. Charles Forceville and Eduardo Urios-Aparisi. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Forceville, Charles. 2011. "The Journey Metaphor and the Source-Path-Goal Schema in Agnes Varda's Autobiographical Gleaning Documentaries." Pp. 281-297 >n Beyond Cognitive Metaphor Theory: Perspectives on Literary Metaphor, ed. Monika Fludernik. London: Routledge. 100 / PROJECTIONS EMBODIED S U A L MEANING Forceville, Charles, and Marloes Jeulink. 2011. 'The Flesh and Blood of Embodied Understanding: The Source-Path-Goal Schema in Animation Film." Pragmatics Si Cognition 19 (1): 37-59- Freedberg, David, and Vittorio Callese. 2007. "Motion, Emotion and Empathy in Aesthetic Experience." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 {5): 197-203. Gallese, Vittorio. 2010. "Mirror Neurons and Art." Pp. 441-449 in Art and the Senses,,, eds. Francesca Bacci and David Melober. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Gallese, Vittorio. 2011. "Seeing Art... Beyond Vision. Liberated Embodied Simulation in Aesthetic Experience." Pp. 62-65 'n Seeing with the Eyes Closed, eds. Alexander Abbushi, Ivana Franke and Ida Mommenejad. Berlin: Association of Neuroesthetics. Gallese, Vittorio-iuciano Fadiga, Leonardo Fogassi, and Giacomo Rizzolatti. 1996. "Action Recognition in the Premotor Cortex." Brain 119 (2): 593-609. Gallese, Vittorio, and George Lakoff. 2005. "The Brain's Concepts: The Role of the Sensory-Motor System in Conceptual Knowledge." Cognitive Neuropsychology 22 (3/4}: 455-47; Gentner, Dedre, and Mutsumi Imai. 1992;!'ls the Future Always Ahead? Evidence for System-:Mappings in Understanding Space-Time Metaphors," Pp. 510-515 in Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Bloomington, IN: Erlbaum. Gombrich, E, H. i960. Art and Illusion: A Study in the Psychology of Pictorial Representation. London: Phaidon.;: Gross, Charles,:and Marc Bornstein. 1978. "Left and Right in Science and Art." Leonardo 1 (1): 29-38. Günther, Winfried. 2004. "A Shot Conveys an Outlook: Some Aspects of Mise=en-scene in Spartacus." Pp. 57-65 in Kinematograph: Stanley Kubrick, eds. Bernd Eichometal, Frankfurt Am Maim Deutsches Filmmuseum,:; = Hampe, Beate. 2005. "Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics: Introduction." Pp. 1-12 in From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics, ed. Beate Hampe. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Johnson, Mark. 1987, The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Johnson, Mark. 1993. Moral imagination: Implications of Cognitive Science for Ethics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Johnson, Mark. 2005. "The Philosophical Significance of Image Schemas;" Pp. 15-33 in From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics, ed. Beate Hampe. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Johnson, Mark. 2007. The Meaning of the Body: Aesthetics of Human Understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Johnson, Mark. 2008, "Philosophy's Debt to Metaphor." Pp. 39-52 in The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, ed. Raymond W. Gihbs, Jr. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kant, Immanuel. [1781/1787] .1999.=Critique of Pure Reason. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Kappelhoff, Herman, and Cornelia Müller. 2011, "Embodied Meaning Construction: Multimodal Metaphor and Expressive-Movement in Speech, Gesture; and Feature Film." Metaphor and the Social World 1 (2}: 121-153. Kövecses, Zoltán: 2005. Metaphor in Culture: Universality and Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. lakoff, George. 1987. Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Our Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson:. [1980] 2003, Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its > Challenge to Western Thought. NewYork: Basic Books. Rampley, Matthew. 1997. "From Symbol to Allegory: Aby Warburg's Theory of Art." Art Bulletin 79 (1): 41-55. Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Luciano Fadiga, Vittorio Gallese, and Leonardo Fogassi. 1996. "Premotor Cortex and the Recognition of Motor Actions."' Cognitive Brain Research 3(2): 131-141. Rohdin, Maths.=20og. "Multimodal Metaphor in Classical Film Theory from the 1920s to the 1850s." Pp. 403^428 in Multimodal Metaphor, eds. Charles Forceville and Eduardo Urios-Aparasi. Berlin: Mouton deGruyter. Tomashevsky, Boris; 1965. "Thematics." Pp. 61-95 in Russian Formalist Criticism: Four Essays, eds.Lee Lemon and Marion Reis. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,: Vischer, Robert. 1994. "On the Optical Sense of Form." Pp. 89-123 in Empathy, Form and Space: Problems in German Aesthetics, 7873-1893, trans, and eds. Harry Francis Mallgrave and Eleftherios Ikonomou. Santa Monica: Getty Center for the History of Art and the Humanities. Westra, Adam. 2010. "Review of Hans Blumenberg'sParadigms for a Metaphorology." Trans. Robert Savage. Ithaque 7 (1): 119-129. Yu, Ning. 1998, The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor: A Perspective from Chinese. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins. Filmography Antonioni, Michelangelo. 1975. Professione: Reporter {The Passenger). Italy and Spain. Bergman, Ingmar. 1966. Persona. Sweden. Clert, Vassili. 2003. Fritz Lang par Claude Chabrol. France. De Palma, Brian. 1989. Casualties of War. USA. Dmytryk, Edward. 1944. Murder, My Sweet. USA. Khurana, Kireet. 1996."O". India. Kubrick, Stanley.i960. Spartacus. USA. Lang, Fritz. 1937. You Only Live Once. USA. Losey, Joseph. 1963. The Servant. UK....... -. 1967. Accident UK.