Conversion or Commitment? A Reassessment of the Snow and Machalek Approach to the Study of Conversion Author(s): Clifford L. Staples and Armand L. Mauss Source: Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion , Jun., 1987, Vol. 26, No. 2 (Jun., 1987), pp. 133-147 Published by: Wiley on behalf of Society for the Scientific Study of Religion Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/1385790 JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms Wiley and Society for the Scientific Study of Religion are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Conversion or Commitment? A Reassessment of the Snow and Machalek Approach to the Study of Conversion* CLIFFORD L. STAPLESt ARMAND L. MAUSSt Snow and Machalek (1983; 1984) have pointed to the importance of language in underst the conversion process. In particular, they have identified four "rhetorical indicators" chara of the accounts converts give about their conversion experience. After a brief review o indicators, and of the Snow and Machalek approach more generally, we offer a critique alternative interpretation of the part played by language in the conversion process, and by th indicators in particular. We substantiate our position both theoretically and empirically, wit gathered from a small sample of Christian evangelicals. We find, happily, that the four indi characteristic of the Nichiren Shoshu believers studied by Snow and Machalek were to be fou among our Christians. However, only one of those four indicators, "biographical reconstr was unique to the Christians who claimed a conversion experience, as such. The rest of the ind were as common among lifelong Christians as among self-professed converts, indicating that are probably indicators of religious commitment more generally, rather than of conversion Of even greater theoretical significance, we believe, is our argument for understanding langu rhetorical devices as actively chosen tools and methods in converts' own efforts at self-transfo rather than as indicators of what has already happened to them. Recently, Snow and Machalek (1983; 1984) have suggested that an analysis of and reasoning of subjects can provide a fruitful approach to the study of conv propose that conversion be viewed as a change in one's "universe of discours certain "rhetorical indicators" in the talk of individuals should identify the co primarily on Snow's (1976) study of the Nichiren Shoshu Buddhist movement, t that converts may be identified by: 1) the adoption of a master attribution biographical reconstruction; 3) the suspension of analogical reasoning; embracement of a master role (Snow & Machalek, 1983: 266-78). The turn to a consideration of the talk and reasoning of the religious pe potentially rich source of data for the study of "everyday religion" has bee several researchers (see Beckford, 1978; Robertson, 1971; Robertson & Campb *An earlier version of this paper was presented at the American Sociological Association and for the Sociology of Religion joint ASA/ASR Session on Issues in Religious Socialization, Wash August, 1985. We are thankful to Carol A. B. Warren and Ruth Kornfield for comments on an ea of this paper. tClifford L. Staples is visiting assistant professor of sociology at the University of Massachus Massachusetts. Armand L. Mauss is professor of sociology at Washington State Universit Washington. © Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 1987, 26 (2): 133-147 133 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION Taylor, 1976; 1978), but Snow and Machalek have been the first to develop th any extent. As a first attempt to introduce a focus on language to the study of Snow and Machalek's formulation warrants serious theoretical and empirical a We agree that a focus on the language and rhetoric of the religious pe promising approach to the study of conversion; however, we believe that the Machalek approach, important as it is, stresses the wrong implications of conver In this paper we develop a theoretically and empirically based critique of the Machalek approach to the study of conversion. Our purpose is to present an a theory of conversion, focusing specifically on the role of language in the conversio We also hope to stimulate further theoretical and empirical work in this area Snow and Machalek's (1983) approach to the study of conversion grows constructive critique of much of the contemporary work in the area. They arg 159) that while researchers have explored the "causes and consequences" of co there has yet to be an extensive analysis of the nature of conversion itself. In f researchers have proceeded to generate theories of the process of conversion (e. 1977; Lofland, 1978; Lofland & Stark, 1965; Richardson & Stewart, 1978), the characteristics of the converts themselves are usually either ignored or assumed. They suggest that "this is a serious oversight, especially since an understanding of the conversion process presupposes the ability to identify the convert" (1983: 260). With these concerns in mind, Snow and Machalek (1983) proceed to develop a definition of the "convert as a social type," based on an analysis of the talk and reasoning of subjects and supported by their own underlying theory of conversion. We will argue in this paper that Snow and Machalek's theory of conversion is flawed because three of the four proposed "rhetorical indicators" of conversion fail to distinguish religious converts from people who, though not "converted," are religiously committed. That is, we believe that the Snow and Machalek formulation fails to distinguish between conversion and commitment as two distinct phenomena (Barker & Currie, 1985). Since distinguishing the convert from other religious types is the supposed goal of their framework, we believe that this flaw attenuates considerably the usefulness of the Snow and Machalek theory. We will provide both theoretical reasoning and empirical evidence to substantiate this claim. In the first section of the paper we briefly present the essentials of the Snow and Machalek (1983; 1984) theory of conversion. In the second section we develop our own alternative ideas on conversion, present some data derived from interviews with a sample of evangelical Christians, and attempt to show why we think that the Snow and Machalek approach to conversion needs to be reconsidered. A SUMMARY OF THE SNOW AND MACHALEK APPROACH TO CONVERSION As Snow and Machalek (1983) observe, despite considerable difference of op the nature of conversion, there is one underlying assumption upon which most would seem to agree, and that is that "conversion involves a radical change in experience" (1983: 264). Whether fast or slow, complete or partial, radica something is assumed to underlie the experience of conversion. What Snow an 134 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? argue (1983: 264) is that it is one's "universe of discourse" which undergoes radical chan in the conversion experience. While they borrow the term from Mead (1934: 88-90), idea has been used in a number of different contexts, under a number of different nam For example, it is quite similar to Wittgenstein's (1958) notion of "language game" an Kuhn's (1962) concept of paradigm. "Universe of discourse" and related notions refer the idea that human beings orient themselves to the world from a set of taken-for-gran assumptions. These sources of "ultimate grounding," or "root reality," in Heirich's (1977 terms, are socially constructed frameworks of meaning imbedded in language and oth symbols that provide a system, or "sacred canopy" (Berger, 1967) for making sense o self and world. This approach postulates a theoretical connection between universe of discourse and consciousness that is based on language. It is thus a thoroughly cognitive approach to the study of coinversion, having its origins within the complementary assumptions of symbolic interactionism (Blumer, 1973) and the sociology of knowledge tradition, best exemplified by Berger and Luckmann (1967). In short, consciousness is the product of communication (i.e., symbolic interaction) within a shared universe of discourse. Since language is the primary "carrier" of meaning, we have access to the elusive entity of consciousness via language. It follows that if conversion involves a change in one's universe of discourse then it also involves a change in consciousness, and the evidence of both changes should be found in the language (i.e., talk and reasoning) of the convert. Or, as Snow and Machalek put it (1983: 279), "Inasmuch as language is practical consciousness, it stands to reason that transformations of consciousness necessitate transformations of language." The question remains, however, as to exactly what sort of rhetoric and reasoning we should expect to find characteristic of the convert. Snow and Machalek (1983: 266-78) propose four "rhetorical indicators" or "formal properties" of the convert. Of the four rhetorical indicators of conversion proposed by Snow and Machalek (1983: 266), biographical reconstruction is dearly the one with the most appeal, both theoretically and empirically. Biographical reconstruction refers to the idea that individuals who undergo the radical change of conversion reconstruct or reinterpret their past lives from the perspective of the present. In a very real sense, the past is created anew. This does not necessarily entail a wholesale fabrication or "distortion" of one's previous life, but rather a restructuring in which previously important events may be de-emphasized and less significant ones elevated to greater prominence. The concept of biographical reconstruction has considerable theoretical and empirical precedent. Perhaps the most cogent formulation of this notion is to be found in Mead's Philosophy of the Present (1932). For Mead, the focus of reality is the present, and it is through human action in the present that the past and future become meaningful. Thus, as argued by Mead, and illustrated by converts, the past is reconstructed in light of the new meanings which emerge from one's present status as a convert. Others, including Beckford (1978), Berger and Tuckmlann (1967), James (1958), Taylor (1976; 1978) and Travisano (1970) have also noted the phenomenon of biographical reconstruction accompanying conversion. While they have approached the idea from a number of perspectives, each has recognized the apparent importance of the phenomenon for an understanding of conversion. At least one issue, however, remains problematic. If we accept Mead's (1932) approach to the problem, then biographical reconstruction is actually ubiquitous: we reconstruct 135 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION our pasts constantly as we confront new experiences, and conversion is not qua different in this respect. Snow and Machalek (1983: 269) agree that the bi reconstruction involved in conversion is also present in everyday, non-c experience, but they suggest that in conversion, biographical reconstruction amplified and intensified. But if biographical reconstruction in conversi quantitatively (not qualitatively) different from "normal" identity reconstruct operationally how can we decide how much reconstruction is beyond "normal and Machalek (1983) provide no general procedure for making such a decision they indicate exactly what procedures they employed in deciding to assert that b reconstruction was quantitatively great enough in the converts they observed the "convert" identity. We will have more to say on this problem below. The second indicator of conversion proposed by Snow and Machalek (1983: the adoption of a master attribution scheme. Based on the assumption that in employ causal interpretations to make sense of themselves, others, and experien 1958; Kelly, 1967; 1971; Spilka et aL, 1985), attribution is a common and find cognitive process. What distinguishes the convert from others, though, is th of a master attribution scheme, one which informs all causal inferences (Snow & 1983: 270). While the average person might utilize a variety of interpretive s attribute cause (e.g., natural law; human nature; divine intervention; see Apos 1983), converts resort to a singular rhetoric of cause and effect. When asked t for the state of the world, self, or others and their actions, converts inevitably one attribution scheme. The third indicator of conversion proposed by Snow and Machalek (1983: 273) is perhaps the most original and interesting of the four. They propose that the talk and reasoning of converts exhibits a suspension of analogical reasoning. Arguing that one important means through which individuals relate and comprehend inchoate experiences is through the use of metaphor or analogy, Snow and Machalek suggest that converts avoid the use of analogy when discussing their beliefs. The reasoning behind this proposal is persuasive: Since the use of analogy is meant to indicate that "one thing is like another," it is contrary to the basic motive of religious belief to suggest that an equivalence exists between one's own beliefs and those of some other group. In Durkheim's terms (Durkheim, 1965; Snow & Machalek, 1983: 275), the suspension of analogical reasoning can be seen as a technique for affirming the sacredness of one's own beliefs over the profanity of all others, thereby insulating and protecting one's own beliefs and self from the contamination of association with alien world views and individuals. Finally, Snow and Machalek (1983: 276-78) propose that the convert can be identified by his embracing of a master role. This property of the convert should be the easiest to observe. In embracing a master role, the convert comes to see him- or herself almost totally in terms of the role as convert and member of a particular group. In contrast, modem life usually imposes role compartmentalization, where individuals are expected to enact a variety of roles depending upon the context in which they find themselves. In the embracing of a master role, the convert both subordinates other roles to the master role (or eliminates them entirely if they are in conflict with the master role), and attempts to apply the master role in all situations. In the first case, there is a clear prioritization of roles, or identities. For example, one is a Christian above all else, then a mother, wife, 136 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? and teacher. Thus, while other roles may be important to the convert, their impo derives not from their intrinsic qualities, but from their relation to the master It might not be unreasonable to say that the master role not only informs all o subordinate roles, but infuses them with a distinctive meaning. Thus, one becom "Christian mother," a "Christian wife," a "Christian teacher," and the like. In t the convert is able to continue to negotiate with the secular world but is able also the secular roles with a sacredness derived from the master role. Furthermore, relatedly, the adoption of the master role means that the convert views every as an opportunity to enact the master status. Despite the compartmentalization thrust upon the moder convert, there is thus a generalization of the master sta role to all situations, providing a psychological basis not only for proselyting a but also for a defense against the self-dividing forces of the modern secular wo CONVERSION AS A PROCESS OF SELF-TRANSFORMATION We agree with Snow and Machalek that conceptualizing conversion as fundamen a process of change is essential, that viewing a shift in universe of discourse as an imp element of this change process may also be useful. However, in their attempt to the process of change in some phenomenon "more fundamental than beliefs or ide ..." (1984: 170), Snow and Machalek's theory offers us a rather weak conceptualiz of the person who experiences conversion. In their scheme, the person is replace the term "consciousness," and this concept is never well developed. In contrast, we see conversion as involving primarily a change in self-consciou or, using the more conventional term, as a change in the self-concept (Mead Rosenberg, 1979). Thus, conversion is seen to involve a change in the way a person and feels about his or her self. One advantage of this approach is that it allows a plausible theoretical link between the individual and the universe of discourse via l (Mead, 1934; Schwalbe, 1983) than is possible with the concept of "consciousn addition, it also makes available to us the extensive theoretical and empirical lite on the self-concept (see Rosenberg & Kaplan, 1982; Gecas, 1982). To distinguish the self-concept change involved in conversion from other, more r changes in the self-concept (e.g., role changes, life-cycle changes), we use the term transformation. Self-transformation, as we define it here, refers to a change in, creation of, what Turner (1976) has refeiead to as the "real self." In making the dis between the "real self" and the "spurious self," Turner is acknowledging the fac not all of our self-conceptions are of equal importance to us and that we are gen aware of a distinction between who we are really versus who we might be in a par role, or under a particular set of circumstances. We capitalize on Turner's distin between the "real self" and the "spurious self" by viewing conversion as transformation - the creation of a new vision of who we really believe we are w our social roles and self-presentations are stripped away. Secondly, we think also that a focus on the language and rhetoric of convert useful direction to pursue, not so much because it allows us indirect access to consci but rather because it is through language that individuals transform themselves is, we take what is referred to as a "functionalist" approach to language (see Sch 137 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION 1983), which leads us to ask: what sort of activity does this rhetorical device a Thus, where Snow and Machalek view particular kinds of language and r observable indicators of some underlying change in consciousness, we view parti of language and rhetoric as methods used by subjects to achieve self-transfor Though these two uses of language are not mutually exclusive or incompatibl lead to somewhat different research questions. The central question moti research is: how do subjects use language and rhetoric to achieve self-transfo What are the implications of the differences between our approach to conv the approach developed by Snow and Machalek? In the first place, Snow and M view conversion in a way that focuses more on what happens to a person, whil conversion more as a process of self-transformation. That is, we view the su active participant in this reconstruction of the self (Richardson, 1985). Secon the issue of identifying the convert. For Snow and Machalek, the subject's selfas a convert or a non-convert is largely irrelevant to determining whether or not is a convert. Instead, they propose that the only basis necessary for the iden of the convert is the presence or absence of their four proposed rhetorical ind conversion. In effect, from Snow and Machalek's point of view, the researcher is better qualified to determine who is or is not a convert than are the subjects Because we view conversion as an inherently subjective phenomenon, we bel the subject, and only the subject, is qualified to tell us who he or she really is. Con we determine whether or not someone is a convert by asking the person the Are you a convert? Though Snow and Machalek (1983: 261-64) are justifiably in their interpretation of converts' accounts of their conversion experiences, we it is a mistake to ignore what our subjects tell us - particularly when they ar us who they are. Indeed, if we are unwilling to believe our subjects when they they are converts, then why should we believe them when they tell us that Christians, Jews, or Nichiren Shoshu Buddhists? Finally, if having the self-concept of a convert is all that is necessary to qu as a convert, is there any theoretical utility in Snow and Machalek's purported indicators" of conversion? Based on our "functionalist" approach to langu rhetorical devices can be theoretically useful only if it can be shown how sub them as methods in their efforts to achieve self-transformation. Or, alternativ be possible to show how these rhetorical devices are used to achieve some oth of religious life, such as religious commitment. The thoughts expressed above have evolved from thinking about the conversion, thinking about Snow and Machalek's approach to conversion, and some data we have gathered from a small sample of evangelical Christians. H far discussed ideas - theirs and ours - we would now like to present and disc of the data. If it appears that our data conform nicely to our ideas and not s the ideas of Snow and Machalek, that is because our ideas have been formed an with more or less continuous reference to our data. Thus, we do not present th analysis as any sort of definitive empirical "test" of the Snow and Machalek fo To do that would be unfair. Instead, we offer the analysis so that the reader some sense for the empirical evidence that has forced us to question the 138 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? Machalek approach and to formulate our own ideas on the role of language in conve SAMPLE AND METHODS We had initially become interested in the prospects of assessing the Machalek approach as a result of auditing taped interviews obtained in the e from members of a so-called "Jesus Freaks" organization located in the North & Petersen, 1974; Petersen & Mauss, 1973). Since the interviews with tho Christians could not have been informed by the much later work of Snow an we were interested and impressed by the frequency with which those inter listened to them more than a decade later, yielded the kinds of rhetoric Snow reported finding among their converts to Nichiren Shoshu. Since many of Freaks" were undoubtedly converts, it seemed quite possible that Snow and might be on to something. We decided, therefore, to try to obtain some sort of of contemporary Christians and subject them to interviews that could be mo informed by the Snow and Machalek formulation. Sample During the 1984-85 school year, we obtained access to a sample of young Christians who were active in Christian evangelical groups and fellowships on or near the campus of Washington State University. Our subjects constitute an "opportunity" sample obtained through a "snowball" method: Subjects referred us to others in their religious network, or related ones, and these, in turn, referred us to others, etc. Altogether, we were able to obtain interviews with 15 such subjects. The respondents were about evenly divided by gender and ranged in age from 19 to 28. They were all from distinctly middle-class backgrounds, though some had enjoyed more affluent upbringings than others. Many had been reared in mainline Christian churches, including Roman Catholic. Methods The main criterion used in deciding how to stnlcture the interviews was to provide the subject the opportunity to display the kinds of rhetorical properties Snow and Machalek suggested should characterize the convert. Consequently, the interviews were very open-ended; the respondents were asked to discuss themselves and their religious 1. We are grateful to an anonymous reviewer who contributed to the evolution of our thinking here by forcing us to reconsider an earlier form of our critique of Snow and Machalek. At first we had taken the more simplistic tack that we could partially replicate the Snow and Machalek study of Nichiren Shoshu converts by applying their four indicators to our own alternative sample of Christian Evangelicals and seeing if those indicators distinguished "converts" from "lifelong" Christians in our sample. Such a "replication," we reasoned, would allow us not only to check on the generalizability of the four indicators from one religion to another, but also to "validate" the indicators against the self-professions of our converts. While we still think that approach has some merit, we now concur with our anonymous critic that such an approach is not so much a "test" of the Snow and Machalek indicators as it is an attempt to supplant one definition of "convert" (theirs), which does not include the self-label criterion, with a different definition (ours) which does include that criterion. This realization, in turn, forced us to articulate more clearly (we hope), and more thoroughly, our rationale for preferring our definition, with its emphasis upon what happens to the self. 139 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION beliefs in interviews that ranged from 45 minutes to 2 hours in length. It was this approach would provide subjects with the time and the freedom to displ characteristic ways of talking and thinking about themselves and their beliefs interviews were conducted by the first author in his academic office on campu of the respondents also allowed us to make copies (with due protection for pr diaries that they had kept around the time of their conversion. The interviews recorded with the subject's consent, and later transcribed in written form for The first step in our analysis was to attempt to determine whether or no all of the four rhetorical indicators were present in the talk of our 15 subjects to do this, it was necessary to operationalize the four rhetorical indicators in so way. Unfortunately, Snow and Machalek (1983; 1984) do not provide any clues a they or anyone else might do this. As a practical matter, deciding whether or n engaged in "biographical reconstruction" or "suspended analogical reason somewhat difficult, and open to a substantial amount of interpretation. It w therefore, that each of us would examine the transcripts separately using an definition of the four rhetorical indicators. By comparing notes, we would the to see whether or not we agreed on which subjects did or did not exhibit which Below are listed the explicit definitions that were employed along with two ex each of the indicators as they were found in the transcripts. (Pseudonyms are, used throughout for all subjects.) Biographical Reconstruction: Where the subject actively reinterprets past ex or self-conceptions from the vantage point of the present in such a way as to meaning of the past for the subject (especially significant segments are emph the passages that follow). Example #1 Gina: So then during high school, I had a lot of friends that I did sports with, and everything, that were really Christians; and I thought they were fanatics. They just turned me off. When I look at it now, I respect them a lot more ... Example #2 Mark: I grew up in a family with high morals. I always considered myself to be a "good kid," so to speak. I was very involved in athletics and what not, but I guess now if I look back, I really ... I can look back on God's little architectural plan for me he kind of started; I can look all the way back to my sophomore year in high school and just kind of see how all these things kind of meld together as he led me towards himself Adoption of a Master Attribution Scheme: Any evidence that the subject attributes all outcomes or the reasons for all outcomes to one source (e.g., "God, "Jesus", "the Bible"). Example #1 Jason: How is my life different? I read the Bible today. I pray today. If I have any question, I'll go to the Bible. All the answers we have in this world are in that book. 140 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? Example #2 Frank: The Bible more or less teaches ... Romans, 13:1, says that all power and authority is from God and all earthly authorities are from God, and stuff. And that is how I feel about contemporary political situations: that power and authority is given to people on earth by God and what they do with it is a different story. Suspension of Analogical Reasoning: The willingness of the subject to equate his or her beliefs or ideas with the beliefs or ideas of other individuals or groups. Example #1 Interviewer: Is that the only way you can get that love? Frank: In Christ? Yes. That's the only way you can really find what true love is. Because true love only rests in God. It is only in God, and you need to go through Christ to get to God. Example #2 Pete: I also believe that it's the only true way to be happy on earth: To feel like you are important, to feel like you mean something. To feel fulfilled while you're on earth, you have to be a Christian. Embracing a Master Role: The subject sees a full interpenetration of the Christian identity with all other identities; the subordination of all other identities to the Christian identity; or the elimination of other identities incompatible with the Christian identity. Example #1 Pete: Example #2 Rod: ... but hopefully in all that I do people can see that maybe I'm different, not on a superficial level; maybe they can't see from just being in a class with them, but maybe they can a little bit; maybe you could see me at a party and I wasn't drinking and say, "He's a little different; he's got some different beliefs there." Since my religious life, of course, permeates everything I do, my behavior is a function of my faith in the living God. And that includes all my behavior, whether I am edifying myself or not; all that behavior is a function of what I believe in. FINDINGS Both of us agreed that each of our 15 Christian subjects displayed some or a rhetorical indicators proposed by Snow and Machalek, suggesting that these r properties are not exclusive to the Nichiren Shoshu Buddhist "universe of disc We also agreed, however, based on further analysis, that while all 15 of our s displayed evidence of suspending analogical reasoning, adopting a master attri scheme, and embracing a master role, four of the 15 subjects failed to engage in purported indicator of conversion - biographical reconstruction. As it turns o 141 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION four subjects also denied ever having had a conversion experience and consistent to refer to themselves as being "born again." In fact, two of these four subject declined to participate in the study because they had heard it was about "bor Christians, and they did not feel that they would be appropriate subjects bec considered themselves to be "lifelong" Christians. These four subjects all claim religious commitment, but no conversion experience as such. Take, for examp exchange with Rod, a 21 year old senior: Interviewer: Okay, we'll start out with the basic stuff, and I already kno a little bit of this, but it will get us going anyway. When did you become a Christian? Rod: It's difficult to say, since growing up in the church I never real had a conversion experience. I remember it... as early as I think five years old is when I remember making my first commitment. Since then, it's very much been a process. Interviewer: You basically consider yourself a lifelong Christian? Rod: So far, yes, and I expect that to carry on. In contrast, consider the exchange with Jonathan, a 26 year old graduate studen minister, and (we think) convert: Interviewer: First of all, when did you become a Christian? Jonathan: November 4, 1978. More of a theological reason, because in th way I believe and so forth there was a particular instant when you became a Christian, that you obeyed God's call A lot of othe people may think, "Well, somewhere along the line Jesus cam into my life," and stuff. Well I know, because I obeyed, and so that's how come I can say, "Yes, I know the time and the hour." The four subjects (including Rod) who denied being converts were the same who failed to display any propensity to engage in biographical reconstruction reasonable, therefore, to suspect that biographical reconstruction is unique to as Snow and Machalek suggest. Yet, where Snow and Machalek would inte finding primarily as evidence of an underlying change in consciousness among the we believe that converts exhibit this pattern of language use because it facili change we call "self-transformation." But why? And how? Briefly (and relying on Mead [1932]), knowing who we are in the present concept) is dependent on the knowledge we obtain about ourselves in symbolic with others. Such knowledge contains not only material for constructing ourse present, but also material for constructing our past and future selves. That is, on of the universe of discourse is to provide us with a methodology for the const our biographies. In self-transformation, we adopt a new universe of discourse it a new methodology for constructing the self. Consequently, self-transformati a rhetorical mechanism, or device, to assist people with the task of realignin biographies in a way that makes sense from the new perspective. We use 142 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? "biographical reconstruction" to label the device, but the specific rhetoric used instance is a feature of the particular universe of discourse in question. For examp Christian theology and Freudian psychoanalysis provide individuals with a rhet achieving biographical reconstruction, but the content of each rhetoric, and cons the meaning of the selves created, are very different. In the "born again" Christian sub-culture, biographical reconstruction ge involves the repudiation of a sinful past. Attendant to that repudiation is the c of a past self that is seen by the convert, from the converted vantage point, as a self," with his or her "real self" being the person now in communion with Jesus interviews with these young college students, the past self was often seen not necessarily as having been sinful, but lost and without direction. Consider the fo statement by Mark, a 20 year old sophomore: Mark: I... consider the building of one's character like building a pyramid I was bringing all these blocks, all these building blocks, and they were great, but then I couldn't really start building. I couldn't really go anywhere with these things; and then I was bringing all these things in and later I found a cornerstone, which was Jesus Christ; and then I was able to really start building. Whether or not Mark felt this way about himself before his conversion is almost irr What is relevant is that only by creating a past "spurious self" through biograp reconstruction is it possible for Mark to create his "real" Christian self in the Biographical reconstruction was absent from the rhetoric of our four "lifel Christians, probably because they saw little or no discontinuity in their "real sel Christian) over time. In short, no self-transformation is evident. This is not to s these subjects viewed themselves as unchanging - quite the contrary. But th more likely to refer to change as "growth," or "a process," and never as a clean with the past. And the growth they experienced always left fundamentally un their "real" Christian selves. While our four "lifelong" Christians failed to engage in biographical reconstruction, they did display, to varying degrees, the three remaining rhetorical properties argued by Snow and Machalek to be indicators of conversion. The fact that our four "lifelong" Christians were equally as likely as our 11 professed converts to suspend analogical reasoning, adopt a master attribution scheme, and embrace a master role, casts doubt, at least in our eyes, on Snow and Machalek's contention that these three rhetorical properties are unique to the convert. With respect to one of these (embracement of a master role) an alternative explanation more plausible to us, for there is some evidence in our data to support it, is this: The embracement of a master role was evident in the talk and reasoning of all 15 Christians because it especially facilitates high levels of religious commitment, a characteristic shared by all of our subjects. If "conversion" involves the attempt to change, or create, the "real self," then it seems reasonable to view "commitment" as the attempt to maintain a consistency in the "real self." Where biographical reconstruction assists in the creation of a new "real self," the embracing of a master role appears to assist in the maintenance of the "real self." Highly committed, "lifelong" Christians are no less likely to display such indicators than are 143 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION their "converted" comrades because they share with these new Christians the d necessity of maintaining a consistent image of their "real selves" as Chri illustrate, consider again the willingness of Rod (a "lifelong" Christian) to em master role of Christian by infusing all his other roles with his "real self": Rod: Since my religious life, of course, permeates everything I do, my behavior is a function of my faith in the living God. And that includ all my behavior, whether I am edifying myself or not; all that behavi is a function of what I believe in. By allowing his "religious life" to "permeate" all his interactions and roles, R to maintain his true Christian self. In a sense, Rod sustains his "real" Christia interaction with others by dissolving the distinction between his "real self" an "spurious selves." By expanding the terrain of interaction to encompass all his provides himself with ample opportunities to display, affirm, and maintain his Though we also discovered evidence of the suspension of analogical reasoni the adoption of a master attribution scheme in the rhetoric of all 15 subjects not yet formulated a theoretical link connecting these devices in any formal o way to conversion, commitment, or any other facet of religious life. However, both self-professed converts and avowed non-converts displayed these characte the least we do not think that they are directly involved in conversion, as sug Snow and Machalek. It seems reasonable to us that all three indicators (other than biographical reconstruction) are likely to be a product of religious socialization, and are also likely to play some role in the process of maintaining religious commitment. This seems particularly true of the embracement of a master role, as we have explained just above.2 Since we are fairly certain that biographical reconstruction plays an important role in conversion, it is helpful to explore this idea in more detail. If biographical reconstruction functions as we suggest, then we would expect to find the greatest need for this device immediately following one's "born again" experience, with a diminishing need for it as one achieves a more complete self-transformation. To test this hypothesis, we went back to the interviews with our 11 self-professed converts to see whether or not the frequency of instances of biographical reconstruction varied with the length of time since their "born again" experience. Our converts varied in what we might call their "spiritual age" (i.e., time elapsed since being "born again") from 8 to 72 months, with a mean of 35 months. Only three of our subjects had converted within the previous 12 months, however, and none had done so within the previous 6 months. Consequently, our sample had few newly bornagain Christians, with most of them having a spiritual age of about 18 months or more. Despite this skewness in our sample, an examination of the transcripts did reveal a 2. It is difficult for us to think of any theoretical basis on which one would expect these rhetorical indicators (other than biographical reconstruction) to be products of conversion but not of socialization. Would we not expect the children of (say) Christian fundamentalists to internalize these traits from their parents and ministers in growing up Christian? Would they not learn also how to make use of a master role, a master attribution scheme, and non-analogical reasoning in defense of their faith? 144 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? tendency for the newer converts to engage in relatively more biographical reco than their "older" counterparts. For example, the four converts less than 18 m averaged seven instances of biographical reconstruction per interview, while th seven "older" Christians averaged just slightly more than two instances of b reconstruction per interview. Differences in the length of interview time betw groups could not account for this difference. This bit of evidence, though cer conclusive, tends to support our view of the role that biographical reconstru in conversion.3 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Our research on religious conversion has been inspired by the recent work o and Machalek (1983; 1984). Their approach has encouraged us to think about con as involving a change in one's universe of discourse and has focused our attenti the role of language and rhetoric in conversion. However, based on our own ide conversion, on the role of language and rhetoric in conversion, and on some da a small sample of evangelical Christians, we have arrived at an alternative conversion to that offered by Snow and Machalek. Our primary difficulty with the Snow and Machalek formulation is that it do seem to recognize sufficiently the essentially subjective nature of conversion. Wh and Machalek focus on "universe of discourse" and "consciousness," we focus on t concept; where Snow and Machalek use rhetorical indicators to identify the con rely on the person's self-professed status as a convert. Where Snow and Machal particular kinds of language and rhetoric to be a reflection of some underlying in consciousness, we view particular kinds of language and rhetoric as tools ind use to achieve a transformation of the self. Finally, where Snow and Machalek view conversion as something that "happens to" a person, we view the person as participant in the creation of a new "real self." In our attempt to assess the merits of the Snow and Machalek formulat conducted interviews with 15 evangelical Christians. We found clear evidence of and Machalek rhetorical indicators of conversion in our sample, suggesting that indicators are generalizable to groups other than the Nichiren Shoshu Buddhist by Snow (1976). However, we also found that only one of the four proposed ind (biographical reconstruction) was capable of differentiating self-professed conver self-professed non-convert believers. This finding, in our opinion, attenuates the of the Snow and Machalek formulation as it now stands. We suggest instead tha biographical reconstruction does seem to be involved in the process of conversi remaining three indicators do not. We think that these three remaining in (particularly the adoption of a master role) may facilitate religious commitmen is why we found evidence of these indicators in the rhetoric of all 15 of our Chr At this time, however, we have not undertaken to explain exactly how these in 3. A possibly interesting parallel occurs to us here between typical religious evolution at the persona institutional levels. That is, the declining significance of biographical reconstruction (and perhaps ot elements) in the convert is in some ways analogous to the declining importance of the charismat in a new "sect" as it evolves toward a "church." 145 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION facilitate religious commitment, leaving us uncertain about the exact nature o relationship.4 It is our view that the study of religious conversion can greatly benefit fr meshing of two trends we see developing in the literature. On the one hand is t and Machalek (1983) approach, with its emphasis on language, rhetoric, and un discourse. On the other hand are studies of conversion, or self-transformation, t on the self-concept (see Ebaugh, 1984; Gordon, 1974; 1984; Harrison, 1974; Stap Straus, 1979; 1981; Travisano, 1970). Our own efforts have constituted an atte capitalize on the benefits of each approach. We propose that conversion be vie process; that this process is fundamentally one of self-transformation; t transformation is achieved primarily through language (Mead, 1934; Schwalbe, 1 that the convert plays an active role in his or her own self-transformation. We viewing the conversion experience in this way is consistent with contemporary in social psychology and opens up numerous directions for research capable of d our understanding of conversion. REFERENCES Apostle, Richard A., Charles Y. Glock, Thomas Piazza, and Marijean Suelzle 1983 The Anatomy of Racial Attitudes. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Barker, Irwin R. and Raymond F. Currie 1985 "Do converts always make the most committed Christians?" Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 24: 305-13. Beckford, James A. 1978 "Accounting for conversion." British Journal of Sociology 29: 249-62. Berger, Peter L. 1967 The Sacred Canopy. New York: Doubleday. Berger, Peter L. and Thomas Luckmann 1967 The Social Construction ofReality. Garden City, New York: Doubleday-Anchor. Blumer, Herbert 1973 Symbolic Interactionism: Perspective and Method. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. Durkheim, Emile 1965 The Elementary Forms of Religious Life. New York: Free Press. (Originally published, 1915). Ebaugh, Helen Rose Fuchs 1984 "Leaving the convent: The experience of role exit and self-transformation." Pp. 156-76 in J. A. Kotarba and A. Fontana, The Existential Self in Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gecas, Viktor 1982 "The self-concept." Annual Review of Sociology 8: 1-33. Gordon, David F. 1974 "The Jesus People: An identity synthesis." Urban Life and Culture 3: 159-78. 1984 "Dying to self: Self-control through selfabandonment." Sociological Analysis 45: 41-56. Harrison, Michael I. 1974 "Preparation for life in the spirit: The process of initial commitment to a religious movement." Urban Life and Culture 2: 387-414. Heider, Fritz 1958 The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley. Heirich, Max 1977 "Change of heart: A test of some widely 4. Nor are we attempting at this time to deal with certain other discoveries from our data, which may prove interesting in subsequent research. For example, two additional traits were widespread (if not universal) in the accounts of the converts in our sample: 1) social "bridge-burning" (renunciation of formerly significant others like family members, spouses, lovers, or important friends); and 2) an especially powerful urge to witness (proselyte) about the "good news" of the newly found faith. Insofar as these traits are acted out, they are more behavioral than rhetorical, of course. However, they have a rhetorical function as well when included as part of the convert's "account." They may be simply subsumable as elements in biographical reconstruction, especially in the functional process of redefining the "self" in terms of new relationships and new responsibilities. 146 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms CONVERSION OR COMMITMENT? held theories about religious conversion." American Journal of Sociology 50: 353-59. James, William 1958 The Varieties of Religious Experience. New [1902] York: New American Library. Kelley, Harold H. 1967 "Attribution theory in social psychology." Pp. 192-238 in D. Levin (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 15. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. 1971 Attribution in Social Interaction. Morristown, New Jersey: General Learning Press. Kuhn, Thomas 1962 The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lofland, John 1978 "Becoming a world-saver revisited." Pp. 10-23 in James Richardson (Ed.), Conversion Careers. Beverly Hills, California: Sage. Lofland, John and Rodney Stark 1965 "Becoming a world-saver: A theory of religious conversion." American Sociological Review 30: 862-74. Mauss, Armand L. and Donald W. Petersen 1974 "Prodigals as preachers: The Jesus Freaks and the return to respectability." Social Compass 21: 3. Mead, George H. 1932 The Philosophy of the Present. A. Murphy (Ed.). Lasalle, Illinois: Open Court. 1934 Mind, Self, and Society. C. Morris (Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Petersen, Donald W. and Armand L. Mauss 1973 "The cross and the commune: An interpretation of the Jesus People." Pp. 261-79 in C. Y. Glock (Ed.), Religion in Sociological Perspective. Belmont, California: Wadsworth. Richardson, James T. 1985 "The active vs. passive convert: Paradigm conflict in conversion/recruitment research." Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 24: 119-236. Richardson, James T. and Mary W. Stewart 1978 "Conversion process models and the Jesus Movement." Pp. 24-42 in J. Richardson (Ed.), Conversion Careers. Beverly Hills, California: Sage. Robertson, Roland 1971 "The sociology of religion: Problems and desiderata." Religion 1: 109-26. Robertson, Roland and C. Campbell 1972 "Religion and Britain: The need for new research strategies." Social Compass 19: 185-97. Rosenberg, Morris 1979 Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books. Rosenberg, Morris and Howard Kaplan 1982 The Social Psychology of the Self-Concept. Arlington Heights, Illinois: Harland Davidson. Schwalbe, Michael L. 1983 "Language and the self: An expanded view from a symbolic interactionist perspective." Symbolic Interaction 6: 291-306. Snow, David 1976 "The Nichiren Shoshu movement in America: A sociological examination of its value orientation, recruitment efforts, and spread." Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles. Snow, David and Richard Machalek 1983 "The convert as a social type." Pp. 259-89 in R. Collins (Ed.), Sociological Theory. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1984 "The sociology of conversion." Annual Review of Sociology 10: 167-90. Spilka, Bernard, Phillip Shaver, and Lee A. Kirk- patrick 1985 "A general attribution theory for the psychology of religion." Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 24: 1-20. Staples, Clifford L. 1985 "Self-transformation among born-again Christians: Preliminary observations on the motivational importance of existential meaning." Unpublished paper presented at the Annual Meetings of the American Sociological Association, Washington, D.C., August, 1985. Straus, Roger A. 1979 "Religious conversion as a personal and collective accomplishment." Sociological Analysis 40: 158-65. 1981 "The social-psychology of religious experience: A naturalistic approach." Sociological Analysis 42: 57-67. Taylor, Brian 1976 "Conversion and cognition: An area for empirical study in the microsociology of religious knowledge." Social Compass 23: 5-22. 1978 "Recollection and membership: Converts' talk and the ratiocination of commonality." Sociology 12: 316-24. Travisano, Richard V. 1970 "Alternation and conversion as qualitatively different transformations." Pp. 594-606 in G. P. Stone and H. A. Faberman (Eds.), Social Psychology Through Symbolic Interaction. Waltham, Massachusetts: Ginn-Blaisdell. Turner, Ralph 1976 "The real self: From institution to impulse." American Journal of Sociology 81: 989-1016. Wittgenstein, Ludwig 1958 Philosophical Investigations. Translated by G.E.M. Moore, London: Macmillan, 3rd Edition. 147 This content downloaded from 176.74.157.145 on Sat, 03 Oct 2020 22:39:53 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms