DOI: 10.1126/science.1247003 , 424 (2014);344Science Daniel Baldauf and Robert Desimone Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based Attention This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only. clicking here.colleagues, clients, or customers by , you can order high-quality copies for yourIf you wish to distribute this article to others here.following the guidelines can be obtained byPermission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles ):April 28, 2014www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of The following resources related to this article are available online at http://www.sciencemag.org/content/344/6182/424.full.html version of this article at: including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services, http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2014/04/09/science.1247003.DC1.html can be found at:Supporting Online Material http://www.sciencemag.org/content/344/6182/424.full.html#ref-list-1 , 20 of which can be accessed free:cites 73 articlesThis article http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/neuroscience Neuroscience subject collections:This article appears in the following registered trademark of AAAS. is aScience2014 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by theScience onApril28,2014www.sciencemag.orgDownloadedfromonApril28,2014www.sciencemag.orgDownloadedfromonApril28,2014www.sciencemag.orgDownloadedfromonApril28,2014www.sciencemag.orgDownloadedfromonApril28,2014www.sciencemag.orgDownloadedfrom 11. E. S. Boyden, F. Zhang, E. Bamberg, G. Nagel, K. Deisseroth, Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1263–1268 (2005). 12. T. Ishizuka, M. Kakuda, R. Araki, H. Yawo, Neurosci. Res. 54, 85–94 (2006). 13. X. Li et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 17816–17821 (2005). 14. A. R. Adamantidis, F. Zhang, A. M. Aravanis, K. Deisseroth, L. de Lecea, Nature 450, 420–424 (2007). 15. A. M. Aravanis et al., J. Neural Eng. 4, S143–S156 (2007). 16. A. Berndt, M. Prigge, D. Gradmann, P. Hegemann, Biophys. J. 98, 753–761 (2010). 17. G. Nagel et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 13940–13945 (2003). 18. J. Mattis et al., Nat. Methods 9, 159–172 (2012). 19. S. Geibel et al., Biophys. J. 81, 2059–2068 (2001). 20. A. Seki et al., Biophys. J. 92, 2559–2569 (2007). 21. A. Berndt, O. Yizhar, L. A. Gunaydin, P. Hegemann, K. Deisseroth, Nat. Neurosci. 12, 229–234 (2009). 22. C. Bamann, R. Gueta, S. Kleinlogel, G. Nagel, E. Bamberg, Biochemistry 49, 267–278 (2010). 23. O. Yizhar et al., Nature 477, 171–178 (2011). 24. H. E. Kato et al., Nature 482, 369–374 (2012). 25. S. Kleinlogel et al., Nat. Neurosci. 14, 513–518 (2011). 26. D. B. Sauer, W. Zeng, S. Raghunathan, Y. Jiang, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 16634–16639 (2011). 27. B. Roux, R. MacKinnon, Science 285, 100–102 (1999). 28. G. Yellen, Nature 419, 35–42 (2002). 29. M. Köhler et al., Science 273, 1709–1714 (1996). 30. C. M. Nimigean, J. S. Chappie, C. Miller, Biochemistry 42, 9263–9268 (2003). 31. D. Bichet, M. Grabe, Y. N. Jan, L. Y. Jan, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 14355–14360 (2006). 32. A. Berndt et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 7595–7600 (2011). 33. D. E. Clapham, C. Montell, G. Schultz, D. Julius; International Union of Pharmacology, Pharmacol. Rev. 55, 591–596 (2003). 34. Single-letter abbreviations for the amino acid residues are as follows: A, Ala; C, Cys; D, Asp; E, Glu; F, Phe; G, Gly; H, His; I, Ile; K, Lys; L, Leu; M, Met; N, Asn; P, Pro; Q, Gln; R, Arg; S, Ser; T, Thr; V, Val; W, Trp; and Y, Tyr. In the mutants, other amino acids were substituted at certain locations; for example, E136R indicates that glutamic acid at position 136 was replaced by arginine. 35. Materials and methods are available as supplementary materials on Science Online. 36. H. Liske, X. Qian, P. Anikeeva, K. Deisseroth, S. Delp, Sci. Rep. 3, 3110 (2013). 37. A. M. Herman, L. Huang, D. K. Murphey, I. Garcia, B. R. Arenkiel, eLife 3, e01481 (2014). 38. Y. Andrews-Zwilling et al., PLOS ONE 7, e40555 (2012). 39. J. Y. Lin, P. M. Knutsen, A. Muller, D. Kleinfeld, R. Y. Tsien, Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1499–1508 (2013). 40. R. Prakash et al., Nat. Methods 9, 1171–1179 (2012). 41. J. L. Galzi et al., Nature 359, 500–505 (1992). 42. M. J. Gunthorpe, S. C. Lummis, J. Biol. Chem. 276, 10977–10983 (2001). 43. H. Yang et al., Cell 151, 111–122 (2012). Acknowledgments: We thank C. Perry and H. Swanson for technical assistance, the entire Deisseroth laboratory for helpful discussions, and T. Jardetzky for use of a Biotek Synergy4 plate reader. K.D. is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency, the Gatsby Charitable Foundation, and the Wiegers Family Fund. A.B. received support from the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD), and S.Y.L. received support from the Fidelity Foundation. Optogenetic tools and methods reported in this paper are distributed and supported freely (www.optogenetics.org). Supplementary Materials www.sciencemag.org/content/344/6182/420/suppl/DC1 Materials and Methods Figs. S1 to S3 18 February 2014; accepted 19 March 2014 10.1126/science.1252367 Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based Attention Daniel Baldauf* and Robert Desimone How we attend to objects and their features that cannot be separated by location is not understood. We presented two temporally and spatially overlapping streams of objects, faces versus houses, and used magnetoencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging to separate neuronal responses to attended and unattended objects. Attention to faces versus houses enhanced the sensory responses in the fusiform face area (FFA) and parahippocampal place area (PPA), respectively. The increases in sensory responses were accompanied by induced gamma synchrony between the inferior frontal junction, IFJ, and either FFA or PPA, depending on which object was attended. The IFJ appeared to be the driver of the synchrony, as gamma phases were advanced by 20 ms in IFJ compared to FFA or PPA. Thus, the IFJ may direct the flow of visual processing during object-based attention, at least in part through coupled oscillations with specialized areas such as FFA and PPA. W hen covertly attending to a location in theperiphery,visualprocessingisbiased toward the attended location, and the sources of top-down signals include the frontal eye fields (FEF) (1, 2) and parietal cortex (PC). FEF may modulate visual processing through a combination of firing rates and gamma frequency synchrony with visual cortex (2). For nonspatial attention, the mechanisms of top-down attention are much less clear. When people attend to a feature, such as a particular color (3–5), or to one of several objects at the same location (6–8), activity in the extrastriate areas representing properties of the attended object is enhanced. But where do the attentional biases (9) come from, and how do they enhance object processing when the distractors are not spatially separate? We combined magnetoencephalography (MEG), supplemented by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging to optimize both spatial and temporal resolution. In the MEG experiment, two spatially overlapping streams of objects (faces and houses) were tagged at different presentation frequencies (1.5 and 2.0 Hz) (Fig. 1, A and B) (5, 10–12). The stimuli went in and out of “phase coherence,” so that they were modulated in visibility over time but did not change in luminance or flash on and off. When subjects were cued to attend to one of the streams and to detect occasional targets within the cued stream, frequency analyses allowed identifying brain regions that followed the stimulus oscillations. Using MEG data only (13), the strongest activity evoked by the face tag was in the right fusiform gyrus, whereas the activity evoked by the house tag was more medially in the inferiortemporal cortex (IT) (Fig. 1C; figs. S1 and 2 for individual subjects and alternative source reconstruction approaches). These areas were roughly consistent with the locations of fusiform face area (FFA) and parahippocampal place area (PPA) determined previously in fMRI (14–16). To increase the accuracy of localization in each subject, we added high-resolution fMRI localizers for FFA and PPA (Fig. 2, B and D, and fig. S3A), which were focused at the expected spots (Fig. 2F). To identify other areas important for nonspatial attention, we contrasted the brain state when attending to one of the two superimposed object classes with a similarly demanding state that did not require attending to either object class. The attention-related fMRI localizers revealed consistent activation in the inferior frontal junction (IFJ) at the intersection of the inferiorfrontal and precentral sulcus (17–19) (Fig. 2, A, C, and E), with weaker and less-consistent signals in posterior-parietal and in inferior-temporal cortex (fig. S3C). A control experiment confirmed that IFJ's activation was indeed related to nonspatial attention, rather than simply memory (fig. S4). Each subject's individual fMRI localizers were then used as regions of interest (ROIs) to guide the analysis of the MEG signals (see supplementary material for a description of the coregistration of fMRI and MEG). The modulation of sensory responses by attention in the taggingfrequency range is shown in Fig. 2G (fig. S5B for individual subjects). FFA and PPA had the strongest responses, with FFA more responsive to the attended face tag (t test, P < 0.001) and PPA more responsive to the attended house tag (t test, P < 0.01). Thus, object-specific attention modulates the sensory responses in FFA and PPA. Weaker sensory responses were found in region V1. Although weaker in amplitude, sensory responses were also found in IFJ, and the attention effects were much stronger—there was a tagging frequency response only to the attended object (both t test, P < 0.001). Control regions in the FEF (localized in separate fMRI runs, fig. S3D), PC (localized in the attention-related fMRI experiment in some participants) and the frontal pole (anatomically defined) showed only minor and less consistent responses. The general pattern of McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, 02139 MA, USA. *Corresponding author. E-mail: baldauf@mit.edu 25 APRIL 2014 VOL 344 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org424 REPORTS results did not depend on the specific tagging frequency assignment to faces or houses (fig. S5). In temporal cortex, both MEG and fMRI results showed a moderate tendency of lateralization: FFA totherightandPPAtothelefthemisphere(Fig.2H). The attentional effects in IFJ were slightly lateralized to the right. We used Fourier transformations to extract the phase relation between the frequency-tag response and the stimulus on the screen, i.e., the latency of the sensory responses (Fig. 2I). The phase lag of IFJ (corresponding to 208 ms) was shifted by about 25 ms in comparison to FFA and PPA (188 and 171 ms) (Fig. 2J and fig. S5) (20), which likely accounts for transmissiontime andsynapticdelaysbetweenareas. Frequency [Hz] Lateralization FFA/PPA phase lag [ms] 0 1 2 V1 0 1 2 IFJ 0 2 FFA 0 2 PPA attend Face attend House 2 2 100 200 100 200 FFAPPA IFJV1 200 250 150 100 FFA PPA A B C D E F G H I J Fig. 2. Measures of attention localization. (A) Average of the fMRI localizers for attention-related and (B) object-related ROIs (blue: FFA, red: PPA, P < 0.001, FW errorcorrected). (C and D) All subjects' individual ROIs superimposed on a standardized brain surface (red: middle frontal gyrus; blue: inferior frontal gyrus; dashed line: BA44) and (E) on an inflated brain. (F) Comparison of the average MEG-based (filled) and fMRI-based (outlines) localization of face- (blue) and house-related (red) activity. (G) Spectral power in the participants' individual ROIs when attending houses (1.5 Hz, red) or faces (2.0 Hz, blue). (H) Lateralization of the attentional effects. (I) Phase lags of the neural activity to the physical stimuli on screen. (J) Systematic phase advancement from FFA/PPA to IFJ. CA B attend FACE (2.0Hz) attend HOUSE (1.5Hz) 2.0 1.0 5.0 H H H F F F F 1cm Power (2.0Hz) Power (1.5Hz) Face F F F F F F F Fig. 1. Stimuliandattention.(A) Stimuli used in the MEG experiment (see online methods). (B) Sequence of stimuli consisting of an overlay of two streams of objects (faces and houses), fading in and out of a phase-scrambled noise mask at different frequencies (1.5 and 2.0 Hz). Subjects had to attend the cued stream and report occasional 1-back repeats. (C) Fourier-transform of the minimum norm estimate when attending to faces (2.0 Hz) or houses (1.5 Hz, P < 0.05, FDR-corrected). www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 344 25 APRIL 2014 425 REPORTS To test for functional interactions among the areas, we analyzed coherence between the frontal and temporal ROIs (Fig. 3C) across a wide frequency spectrum, including frequency bands that were not time-locked to the stimuli (see timefrequency power spectra and an analysis of frequency nesting in fig. S6). The baseline-corrected coherence between IFJ-FFA (top) and IFJ-PPA (bottom) in the tagging-frequency range is shown in Fig. 3A. When attending into an area's preferred stimulus domain, that area became functionally connected with IFJ at the respective tagging frequency (both t test, P < 0.001), as responses in both areas were phase-locked to the attended stimulus but with different phase lags. Coherence at frequencies higher than the tagging frequency was dominated by shared background coherence, as typical in MEG. To reduce the influence of background coherence, we analyzed patterns of domain-specific coherence by computing an attention index, the AIC = (attend preferred – attend unpreferred)/ (attend preferred + attend unpreferred) which directly contrasts both attentional conditions and, therefore, is more sensitive to subtle attentional effects on coherence (Fig. 3B). When attending to faces (top, blue) coherence between IFJ and FFA increased not only at the tagging frequency (2.0 Hz) but also in a high-frequency band (70 to 100 Hz, both t test, P < 0.05). Similarly, when attending to the house stimuli (red), IFJ and PPA exhibited increased coherence, both at the tagging frequency (1.5 Hz) and in a high-frequency band (60 to 90 Hz, both t test, P < 0.01). In this high-frequency gamma range, the individual subjects varied considerably in their respective peak modulation frequency. As a check for whether the coherence in the gamma range resulted from common stimulus-locked onsets, we reran the analysis in a control data set, with shuffled trial order within each ROI (fig. S7C), which completely eliminated gamma coherence. Attentional modulations of coherence between IT and PC were weaker and nonsignificant (fig. S7D). To test the directionality of the gamma-band coherence between IFJ and FFA/PPA, we analyzed the instantaneous phase lags between the two areas. Because portions of the signal in both sites are shared background coherence (due to electromagnetic field spread) or random noise, we first baseline-corrected the phase lag distributions to dissociate shared background coherence (which is simultaneous) and noise (which is uniformly distributed) from phase coherence that results from axonally transmitted synchronization (see supplementary methods and figs. S8 and S9). We then compared the residual phase lag distribution across a range of frequency bands around the subject's frequency of maximal coherence (peak T10 Hz). In most subjects (9 out of 12), the baseline-corrected phase lags systematically increased as a function of frequency, consistent with IFJ leading FFA/PPA with a constant time lag of about 20 ms (SE = 6 ms) (Fig. 3D and figs. S10 and S11). The three other subjects seemed to have stronger bottom-up or balanced coherence (see supplementary materials). To determine whether IFJ is anatomically connected with FFA or PPA, we computed maps of probabilistic connectivities (21) to the seed regions in FFA and PPA. When normalizing to the site of maximal activity within frontal cortex, both FFAand PPA-connectomes revealed areas around IFJ to have the highest connection probabilities (see Fig. 3E and fig. S12). The neural mechanism that enables attention to an object or feature seems intuitively more complex than spatial attention, which may only require a spatial-biasing signal that targets a relevant location. Yet the present study reveals some striking parallels in neural mechanisms: Prefrontal cortex seems to be a common source of top-down biasing signals, with FEF supplying signals for spatial attention and IFJ supplying signals for object or feature attention. With spatial attention, cells in FEF and visual cortex begin to oscillate together in the gamma frequency range, with FEF the “driver” in these oscillations (2). Here, we find that IJF—although it has delayed sensory responses—is also the “driver” in coupled gamma oscillations with FFA/PPA. In primates, coherent gamma oscillations in FEF are phase-shifted by about 10 ms compared with oscillations in area V4, which has been argued to account for the axonal conductance time and synaptic delays between the two areas (2). With the phase shift, spikes of FEF cells presumably affect cells in V4 at a time of maximum depolarization, which increases their impact. Here, a phase shift of 25 ms may allow for longer transmission times from IFJ to FFA and PPA in humans. Thus, spikes originating from IFJ may arrive in FFA and PPA respectively, and vice versa, at a time of maximum depolarization in the receiving area, magnifying their impact. The directing of IFJ signals to the FFA versus PPA may not be inherently more BA C D π 2π φ∆ E 1 0 Fig. 3. Coherence measures of attention. (A) Cross-area coherence spectra. (B) Attention indices, converted into changes of coherence. When attending to the preferred stimulus (faces for FFA, houses for PPA), coherence between IFJ and the respective temporal area increased at the respective tagging frequency and in a high-frequency band (70 to 100 Hz). Dots represent subjects' peaks of attentional modulations. (C) Schematic of the fronto-temporal connectivity. (D) Directionality measure of gamma phase-lags between IFJ and FFA/PPA in polar (right) and Cartesian (left) coordinates. In 9 of 12 subjects the phase-lag of FFA/PPA to IFJ increased linearly with increasing frequencies around the subject's peak of gamma coherence, consistent with IFJ cycles leading over FFA/PPA cycles. (E) Parcellation-based probability maps of frontal connectivity to FFA/PPA. 25 APRIL 2014 VOL 344 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org426 REPORTS complex than shifting FEF signals between different locations in the visual field. IFJ may include areas that function as general executive modules (22, 23). Also, IFJ is close to areas Ba45 and Ba46, homologs of which have been described in nonhuman primate recordings to encode information about object-categories in delayed match-to-sample tasks (23, 24). Indeed, the “attentional template” that specifies the relevant location or object in spatial or feature attention is hardly distinguishable from working memory for these qualities (9), which is known to involve prefrontal cortex (24). Coupled interactions between prefrontal areas and visual areas (25–31) could underlie many cognitive phenomena in vision, with shared neural mechanisms but variations in the site of origin and the site of termination. References and Notes 1. T. Moore, K. M. Armstrong, Nature 421, 370–373 (2003). 2. G. G. Gregoriou, S. J. Gotts, H. Zhou, R. Desimone, Science 324, 1207–1210 (2009). 3. S. Treue, J. C. Martínez Trujillo, Nature 399, 575–579 (1999). 4. J. T. Serences, G. M. Boynton, Neuron 55, 301–312 (2007). 5. M. A. Schoenfeld, J.-M. Hopf, C. Merkel, H.-J. Heinze, S. A. Hillyard, Nat. Neurosci. 17, 619–624 (2014). 6. K. M. O’Craven, P. E. Downing, N. Kanwisher, Nature 401, 584–587 (1999). 7. J. Duncan, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501–517 (1984). 8. V. M. Ciaramitaro, J. F. Mitchell, G. R. Stoner, J. H. Reynolds, G. M. Boynton, J. Neurophysiol. 105, 1258–1265 (2011). 9. R. Desimone, J. Duncan, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222 (1995). 10. R. Hari, M. Hämäläinen, S.-L. Joutsiniemi, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 86, 1033–1039 (1989). 11. P. Lakatos, G. Karmos, A. D. Mehta, I. Ulbert, C. E. Schroeder, Science 320, 110–113 (2008). 12. L. Parkkonen, J. Andersson, M. Hämäläinen, R. Hari, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 20500–20504 (2008). 13. M. S. Hämäläinen, R. J. Ilmoniemi, Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 32, 35–42 (1994). 14. N. Kanwisher, J. McDermott, M. M. Chun, J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311 (1997). 15. D. Y. Tsao, S. Moeller, W. A. Freiwald, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 19514–19519 (2008). 16. R. Epstein, N. Kanwisher, Nature 392, 598–601 (1998). 17. M. Brass, J. Derrfuss, B. Forstmann, D. Y. von Cramon, Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 314–316 (2005). 18. T. P. Zanto, M. T. Rubens, A. Thangavel, A. Gazzaley, Nat. Neurosci. 14, 656–661 (2011). 19. A. Gazzaley, A. C. Nobre, Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 129–135 (2012). 20. J. Liu, A. Harris, N. Kanwisher, Nat. Neurosci. 5, 910–916 (2002). 21. Z. M. Saygin et al., Nat. Neurosci. 15, 321–327 (2011). 22. J. Duncan, Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 172–179 (2010). 23. E. Fedorenko, J. Duncan, N. Kanwisher, Curr. Biol. 22, 2059–2062 (2012). 24. G. Rainer, W. F. Asaad, E. K. Miller, Nature 393, 577–579 (1998). 25. F. Barceló, S. Suwazono, R. T. Knight, Nat. Neurosci. 3, 399–403 (2000). 26. P. Fries, Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480 (2005). 27. O. Jensen, J. Kaiser, J.-P. Lachaux, Trends Neurosci. 30, 317–324 (2007). 28. A. K. Engel, P. Fries, W. Singer, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716 (2001). 29. T. Womelsdorf et al., Science 316, 1609–1612 (2007). 30. M. Siegel, T. H. Donner, R. Oostenveld, P. Fries, A. K. Engel, Neuron 60, 709–719 (2008). 31. R. T. Canolty et al., Science 313, 1626–1628 (2006). Acknowledgments: We thank J. Liang, D. Pantazis, M. Hämäläinen, D. Dilks, D. Osher, Y. Zhang, C. Triantafyllou, S. Shannon, S. Arnold, C. Jennings. Supported by NIH (P30EY2621) and NSF (CCF-1231216), both to R.D. Supplementary Materials www.sciencemag.org/content/344/6182/424/suppl/DC1 Materials and Methods Supplementary Text Figs. S1 to S13 References (32–78) 8 October 2013; accepted 1 April 2014 Published online 10 April 2014; 10.1126/science.1247003 A Chloroplast Retrograde Signal Regulates Nuclear Alternative Splicing Ezequiel Petrillo,1 * Micaela A. Godoy Herz,1 Armin Fuchs,2 Dominik Reifer,2 John Fuller,3 Marcelo J. Yanovsky,4 Craig Simpson,3 John W. S. Brown,3,5 Andrea Barta,2 Maria Kalyna,2 † Alberto R. Kornblihtt1 ‡ Light is a source of energy and also a regulator of plant physiological adaptations. We show here that light/dark conditions affect alternative splicing of a subset of Arabidopsis genes preferentially encoding proteins involved in RNA processing. The effect requires functional chloroplasts and is also observed in roots when the communication with the photosynthetic tissues is not interrupted, suggesting that a signaling molecule travels through the plant. Using photosynthetic electron transfer inhibitors with different mechanisms of action, we deduce that the reduced pool of plastoquinones initiates a chloroplast retrograde signaling that regulates nuclear alternative splicing and is necessary for proper plant responses to varying light conditions. L ight regulates about 20% of the transcriptome in Arabidopsis thaliana and rice (1, 2). Alternative splicing has been shown to modulate gene expression during plant development and in response to environmental cues (3). We observed that the alternative splicing of At-RS31 (Fig. 1A), encoding a Ser-Arg–rich splicing factor (4), changed in different light regimes, which led us to investigate how light regulates alternative splicing in plants. Seedlings were grown for a week in constant white light to minimize interference from the circadian clock and then transferred to light or dark conditions for different times (see the supplementary materials). We observed a two- and fourfold increase in the splicing index (SI)—defined as the abundance of the longest splicing isoform relative to the levels of all possible isoforms—of At-RS31[mRNA3/(mRNA1+mRNA2+mRNA3)] after 24 and 48 hours in the dark, respectively (Fig. 1B). This effect was rapidly reversed when seedlings were placed back in light, with total recovery of the original SI in about 3 hours (Fig. 1C), indicating that the kinetics of the splicing response is slower from light to dark than from dark to light. The light effect is gene specific (fig. S1) and is also observed in diurnal cycles under short-day conditions (Fig. 1D and fig. S2). Furthermore, three circadian clock mutants behaved like the wild type (WT) in the response of At-RS31 alternative splicing to light/dark (fig. S3). Changes in At-RS31 splicing are proportional to light intensity both under constant light and in short-day– grown seedlings (fig. S4). Both red (660 nm) and blue (470 nm) lights produced similar results as white light (Fig. 1E). Moreover, At-RS31 alternative splicing responses to light/dark are not affected in phytochrome and cryptochrome signaling mutants (5, 6), ruling out photosensory pathways in this light regulation (Fig. 1F and figs. S5 and S6). Light-triggered changes in At-RS31 mRNA patterns are not due to differential mRNA degradation. First, the light effect is not observed in the presence of the transcription inhibitor actinomycin D (Fig. 1G). Second, the effects are still observed in upf mutants, defective in the nonsense-mediated mRNA decay (NMD) pathway (7) (Fig. 1H and fig. S7). Third, overexpression of the constitutive splicing factor U2AF65 (8) in Arabidopsis protoplasts mimics the effects of light on At-RS31 alternative splicing (Fig. 1I). mRNA1 is the only isoform encoding a fulllength At-RS31 protein (9). mRNA3 and mRNA2 are almost fully retained in the nucleus (fig. S8). mRNA1 levels decrease considerably in dark without significant changes in the total amount of At-RS31 transcripts (Fig. 2A and fig. S9), which suggests that alternative splicing is instrumental 1 Laboratorio de Fisiología y Biología Molecular, Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, IFIBYNE-CONICET, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad Universitaria, Pabellón 2, C1428EHA Buenos Aires, Argentina. 2 Max F. Perutz Laboratories, Medical University of Vienna, A-1030 Vienna, Austria. 3 Cell and Molecular Sciences, The James Hutton Institute, Invergowrie, Dundee, Scotland. 4 Fundación Instituto Leloir, IIBBA-CONICET, C1405BWE Buenos Aires, Argentina. 5 Division of Plant Sciences, University of Dundee at The James Hutton Institute, Invergowrie, Dundee, Scotland. *Present address: Max F. Perutz Laboratories, Medical University of Vienna, A-1030 Vienna, Austria. †Present address: Department of Applied Genetics and Cell Biology, BOKU, University of Natural Resources and Life Sciences, Muthgasse 18, A-1190 Vienna, Austria. ‡Corresponding author. E-mail: ark@fbmc.fcen.uba.ar www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 344 25 APRIL 2014 427 REPORTS