Regulation of T cell activation, anxiety, and male aggression by RGS2 Antonio J. Oliveira-dos-Santos*, Goichi Matsumoto*, Bryan E. Snow*, Donglin Bai† , Frank P. Houston‡ , Ian Q. Whishaw§ , Sanjeev Mariathasan¶ , Takehiko Sasaki*, Andrew Wakeham*, Pamela S. Ohashi¶ , John C. Roder† , Carol A. Barnes‡ , David P. Siderovskiʈ , and Josef M. Penninger*, ** *Amgen Institute and ¶Ontario Cancer Institute, Departments of Medical Biophysics and Immunology, University of Toronto, 620 University Avenue, Toronto, ON M5G 2C1 Canada; †Samuel Lunenfeld Research Institute, Mount Sinai Hospital, Toronto, ON M5S1A8 Canada; ‡Arizona Research Laboratories, Division of Neural Systems, Memory, and Aging, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85724; §Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, AB T1K3 M4 Canada; and ʈDepartment of Pharmacology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 Communicated by Alfred G. Gilman, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, August 29, 2000 (received for review June 20, 2000) Regulators of G protein signaling (RGS) proteins accelerate the GTPase activity of G␣ protein subunits in vitro, negatively regulating G protein-coupled receptor signaling. The physiological role of mammalian RGS proteins is largely unknown. The RGS family member rgs2 was cloned as an immediate early response gene up-regulated in T lymphocytes after activation. To investigate the role of RGS2 in vivo, we generated rgs2-deficient mice. We show that targeted mutation of rgs2 in mice leads to reduced T cell proliferation and IL-2 production, which translates in an impaired antiviral immunity in vivo. Interestingly, rgs2؊/؊ mice also display increased anxiety responses and decreased male aggression in the absence of cognitive or motor deficits. RGS2 also controls synaptic development and basal electrical activity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Thus, RGS2 plays an important role in T cell activation, synapse development in the hippocampus, and emotive behaviors. G protein-coupled receptors (GPCR) control fundamental cellular processes ranging from embryogenesis and neurotransmitter release to cell survival, proliferation, and differentiation. Heterotrimeric G proteins are composed of ␣, ␤, and ␥ subunits that relay GPCR stimulation to downstream signaling pathways (1). On ligand binding, GPCR undergoes conformation changes of its intracellular loops that promote the exchange of GDP for GTP on G␣ and the subsequent dissociation of G␣ and G␤␥. Both GTP-bound G␣ and free G␤␥ propagate the signal through interactions with downstream effectors. Deactivation of GPCR signaling depends on the rate of G␣ GTP hydrolysis. Studies of GPCR desensitization have led to the discovery of a large and evolutionarily conserved family of GTP-activating (GAP) factors for G␣ termed ‘‘regulators of G protein signaling’’ (RGS). RGS proteins share an RGS domain (Ϸ120 amino acids) capable of accelerating G␣-dependent GTP hydrolysis and the conversion of active GTP-bound G␣ to its inactive GDP-bound form (2–7). The first RGS identified was the yeast protein Sst2, isolated in a screen for mutants that failed to down-regulate the GPCR-mediated response to mating pheromone (8–9). Over 20 mammalian RGS proteins have been identified (7, 10). Recently it has been shown that RGS9–1 controls the photosensitization response in the mouse eye (11). Axin, which contains a RGS domain, negatively regulates the Wnt-signaling pathway, and axin-mutant mice display defects in embryonic axis formation (12). However, Axin has no detectable GAP activity for G␣ (10). The physiological roles of other mammalian RGS proteins are yet to be established. The RGS family member rgs2 was cloned as an early response gene up-regulated in activated T cells (13–14). RGS2 is also up-regulated in central nervous system (CNS) neurons after stimuli that evoke long-term neuronal plasticity (15–16). In vitro, RGS2 interacts with G␣q/11 and stimulates its GTPase activity (17–18), and RGS2 overexpression in cells inhibits G␣i- (15) and G␣s-dependent (19) signaling. To investigate the role of RGS2 in vivo, we generated rgs2-deficient mice. rgs2 deficiency in mice leads to impaired T cell activation in vitro and in vivo. Interestingly, mice lacking RGS2 also exhibit reduced male aggressive behavior and increased anxiety, whereas morphological and electrophysiological analyses indicate a role for RGS2 in hippocampal CA1 neuron function. Materials and Methods rgs2 Mutant Mice. RGS2 exons 4 and 5 encoding the RGS domain were replaced with PGK-NeoR in antisense orientation. G418-resistant E14K embryonic stem (ES) cells were screened by PCR and Southern blot for homologous recombination. Targeted ES cell clones were injected into C57BL͞6 blastocysts and heterozygous rgs-2ϩ/Ϫ mice obtained by mating chimeras with C57BL͞6J mice. PCR primers: wildtype allele (5Ј-CCGAGTTCTGTGAAGAAAACATTG3Ј; 5ЈGGGACTCCTGGTCTCATGTAGCAT3Ј) and mutant allele (5Ј-GCTAAAGCGCATGCTCCAGAC-3Ј; 5Ј-GGCCCACATTTACACGAACC-3Ј). All experiments were carried out by using littermate mice on a C57BL͞6J background (F5 generation), and mice were maintained in accordance with institutional guidelines. Immunocytometry. Single-cell suspensions of thymi, lymph nodes, and spleens were stained with FITC-, phycoerythrin-, or biotinconjugated Abs reactive to CD3␧, TCR␣␤, CD4, CD8, CD25, CD44, CD28, CD69, CD11b (Mac-1), CD11c, B220, CD43, sIgM, sIgD, or Gr-1. Biotinylated antibodies were visualized by using streptavidin-RED670. Samples were analyzed by flow cytometry by using a FACScan (Becton Dickinson). T and B Cell Assays. For proliferation assays, freshly isolated T cells were activated with PMA (10 ng͞ml, phorbol myristate acetate) plus Caϩ2 ionophore (100 ng͞ml, ionomycin), soluble anti-CD3␧ (0.1–1 ␮g͞ml, clone 145–2C11), anti-CD28 (0.02–0.2 ␮g͞ml, clone 37.51), or recombinant mouse IL-2 (50 units͞ml) in triplicate for 12, 24, 48, 72, or 120 h, followed by a 12-h pulse with [3 H]thymidine (1 ␮ Ci͞ml). Cytokine production was determined by ELISA. For B cell analysis, splenic B lymphocytes were purified (90–95% B220ϩ cells) by negative magnetic sorting and activated with various stimuli. Basal serum Ig levels (IgM, IgG1, IgG2a, IgG2b, IgG3, and IgA) were determined by ELISA (20). For lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus (LCMV) infections, Abbreviations: GPCR, G protein-coupled receptors; GAP, GTP-activating; RGS, regulators of G protein signaling; CNS, central nervous system; ES, embryonic stem; LCMV, lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus; EPSP, excitatory postsynaptic potentials. **To whom reprint requests should be addressed. E-mail: jpenning@amgen.com. The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page charge payment. This article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact. Article published online before print: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 10.1073͞pnas.220414397. Article and publication date are at www.pnas.org͞cgi͞doi͞10.1073͞pnas.220414397 12272–12277 ͉ PNAS ͉ October 24, 2000 ͉ vol. 97 ͉ no. 22 mice were infected with LCMV (700 plaque-forming units, s.c.) into the hind footpad. Cellular infiltration was measured daily as described (21). Behavior Studies. Four- to five-month-old sex-matched littermate mice were examined for neuromotor functions (22): vibrissae, eyes, rearing͞support͞standing, muscle tone, tail hanginginduced turns and limbs͞digits extension, ear reflex, eye blink and pupillary response to light, sweet͞bitter tasting acceptance͞ rejection, and orientation toward sound. Circadian and openfield activities (i.e., cage crosses, total distance, vertical movements) were measured in cages fitted with infrared sensors. Exploration test was performed by putting the mice in the center of a white disk (183-cm diameter), with or without objects distributed in four quadrants. The mice were video tracked (seven trials). Fecal pellets and rears were counted. Motor coordination was analyzed by a rotating rod task. For a balance task, a mouse was held by the tail and allowed to grasp a suspended horizontal wooden rod (3 mm Ø) with its hind limbs, with recording of the time for the mouse to pull itself into balance and the number of falls. Mice were studied in a spatial water maze for 7 days with a hidden platform in the southwest quadrant (22). On day 7, a final probe trial (60 s in the pool without platform) was applied. From day 8 to 16, tests were reassumed with the platform moved to the northeast quadrant (reversal, 2 trials per day). Time to find the platform and total distance were measured. In the T-maze test, on day 1 the mice were placed in an empty maze for 10 min with access only to its central and right arms. Several objects were placed as cues around the maze. On day 2, each subject was again placed in the T-maze and given access to the left arm of the maze. The mice were observed for 5 min, with recording of time spent in the new area and first choice. Step-down avoidance test was also carried out, in which the mice were required to remember that prior stepping off a safe platform resulted in a foot shock (1 s, 0.12 mA). Latency on the platform was recorded in each trial. Aggression was tested by using displacement from a plastic tube (30 cm long and 3.05 cm Ø). A rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and littermate control mouse were released into either end of the tube, with the first mouse to exit being deemed the ‘‘loser,’’ that is, this mouse was deemed to be the less aggressive of the two. For light͞dark preference tests, cages were divided into dark and light halves and the time spent by each mouse in each half measured. Electrophysiology. Electophysiology studies were carried out as described (23). Single hippocampal slices (400 ␮m) were superfused with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (2 ml͞min; saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 at 30°C). Field potentials were sampled at 10 kHz and analyzed with PCLAMP6 software (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP) were recorded with a micropipette (2–4 M⍀) placed in the stratum radiatum of CA1 area. Stimulation was delivered at Schaffer collateral. Test stimulus frequency was 0.1 Hz. The tetanic stimulation consisted of four trains of 100-Hz stimulation lasting 500 ms at 10-s intervals. EPSP slope was calculated from linear regression of the rising phase 10–65% of the peak response. The baseline value of EPSP slope was that from the first 5-min period and was defined as 100%. Input͞output relationship was constructed by plotting the amplitude of presynaptic volley (input) vs. EPSP slope (output). Results and Discussion rgs2 Gene Targeting. The rgs2 locus was inactivated in ES cells by replacing exons 4 and 5 corresponding to the RGS domain (Fig. 1A). Chimeric mice were bred to obtain germ-line transmission of the mutated rgs2 allele (Fig. 1 B and C). The rgs2 mutation proved to be null, as shown by Northern blot analyses of brain tissue (Fig. 1D). rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice were born at the expected Mendelian frequency, were fertile, and displayed normal growth. No anatomical or histological abnormalities were observed. Differentiation of all hematopoietic lineages, including neutrophils, macrophages, erythrocytes, and platelets, was normal in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. Fig. 1. Generation of rgs2-deficient mice. (A) rgs2 targeting vector. (B) Southern blot (see 3Ј flanking probe in A) from rgs2 mutant ES cell clones. Wild-type (WT) and mutant (KO) bands are shown. (C) Genotyping of rgs2 mice. WT and KO PCR primers are shown in A (arrows). Loss of the wild-type rgs2 allele was confirmed by genomic Southern blot. (D) Northern blot analysis of brain mRNA from rgs2 WT (ϩ͞ϩ), heterozygous (ϩ͞Ϫ), and KO (Ϫ͞Ϫ) mice (full-length rgs2 cDNA probe). Oliveira-dos-Santos et al. PNAS ͉ October 24, 2000 ͉ vol. 97 ͉ no. 22 ͉ 12273 MEDICALSCIENCES Defective Proliferation and IL-2 Production by rgs2؊/؊ T Cells. Because RGS2 expression is induced in activated T cells after TCR engagement or stimulation by mitogens such as ConA or PMA plus Ca2ϩ ionophore (PMA ϩ Iono) (ref. 14 and data not shown), we determined whether RGS2 plays a role in T cell activation. rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice displayed normal numbers and subsets of CD4ϩ and CD8ϩ T cells and B cells in lymphoid organs (Fig. 2A). The surface levels of TCR␣␤, CD3, CD4, CD8, CD28, CD45, CD5, H2Kb , CD44, LFA1, CD25, and CD69 on both splenic and lymph node peripheral CD4ϩ and CD8ϩ T cells were comparable in rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice (not shown). To examine peripheral T cell function, T cells from the lymph nodes of rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ littermates were stimulated with anti-CD3␧ mAb, anti-CD3␧ plus anti-CD28 mAbs, or PMA plus Ca2ϩ ionophore. The proliferation in vitro of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells was significantly reduced in response to TCR engagement, a deficit that could not be overcome by CD28 costimulation (Fig. 2B). Consistent with the identification of RGS2 as an early response gene in activated T cells (13–14), the proliferative defect was most obvious at 24 h of stimulation. Induction and kinetics of apoptosis were comparable among rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells (not shown). Thus, RGS2 acts as an immediate early gene downstream of TCR activation in T cells. In addition to their proliferative defect, rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells produced significantly lower levels of the T cell growth factor IL-2 compared with controls (Fig. 2C). IL-2 insufficiency per se does not explain the proliferative deficit because IL-2 supplementation did not restore proliferation to normal (Fig. 2B). The high-affinity IL-2 receptor ␣ chain (CD25) and the activation markers CD69, CD44, ICAM1, and CTLA4 were up-regulated to a normal extent and with normal kinetics in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells in response to TCR engagement or mitogen treatment (not shown). The functional defect in T cell proliferation (Fig. 2B) and cytokine production (Fig. 2C) can also be observed in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells treated with PMA͞Ca2ϩ ionophore, a stimulus that bypasses the proximal TCR signal. Consistent with this finding, the kinetics and extent of early TCR-mediated signaling events, including Ca2ϩ mobilization, overall tyrosyl phosphorylation, and activation of MAPK and SAPK͞JNKs, were normal in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ lymph node T cells (not shown), suggesting that their proximal TCR signaling pathways are intact. With regards to B cells, rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice displayed normal numbers and differentiation (not shown). In vitro proliferation of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ B cells in response to treatment with anti-IgM Ab, the F(abЈ)2 fragment of the anti-IgM Ab, anti-CD40, or LPS was similar to that of rgs2ϩ/Ϫ B cells (Fig. 2D and data not shown). Furthermore, baseline serum Ig levels were equivalent in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and rgs2ϩ/Ϫ mice. Thus, RGS2 is not required for B cell activation. Our results show that antigen receptor-triggered RGS2 expression in T cells is required for proliferation and IL-2 production. Chemokines are critical regulators of T cell function, signaling via GPCRs expressed on the surface of resting and activated T cells (24). It has also been shown that overexpression of RGSfamily members in cell lines can modulate chemokine receptor signaling (18). However, no differences were observed between both resting and preactivated (6-h activation with anti-CD3␧ Fig. 2. Impaired proliferation and IL-2 production by rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells. (A) Normal populations of CD4ϩ and CD8ϩ T cells and B220ϩ B cells in lymph nodes of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. Numbers in each quadrant represent percentages of each subset. (B) Proliferation. Lymph node T cells (2 ϫ 105͞well) from rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and rgs2ϩ/Ϫ mice were activated by using PMA [10 ng͞ml]ϩCaϩ2-ionophore [100 ng͞ml], TCR stimulation [anti-CD3␣ mAb (0.1 ␮g͞ml)] with and without CD28 costimulation [anti-CD28 mAb (0.02 ␮g͞ml)] or IL-2 [50 units͞ml]. Results are shown as mean [3H]thymidine uptake Ϯ SD. Difference in proliferation between rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells was statistically significant (t test, P Ͻ 0.05). (C) IL-2 production. Lymph node T cells were activated as above, and IL-2 was measured by ELISA at 24 and 48 h poststimulation. Mean values of IL-2 production Ϯ SD are shown. (D) Activation of B cells. Purified splenic B cells (1 ϫ 105͞well) were incubated in triplicate for 24 and 48 h in medium alone (None) or medium containing anti-IgM (20 ␮g͞ml), anti-IgM (FabЈ)2 (15 ␮g͞ml) with or without anti-CD40 (5 ␮g͞ml). Results are shown as mean [3H]thymidine uptake Ϯ SD. 12274 ͉ www.pnas.org Oliveira-dos-Santos et al. before chemokine treatment) rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ CD4ϩ or CD8ϩ T cells in either proliferation or cell migration in response to the chemokines SDF-1␣, RANTES, or MIP-1␣ (not shown). These results do not preclude regulation by RGS2 of T cell responsiveness to signals mediated by other chemokine receptors. Furthermore, rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T lymphocytes showed comparable responses to the GPCR agonists lysophosphatidic acid, histamine, and adenosine (not shown). The GPCRs whose functions are under RGS2 control in T cells remain to be identified. Impaired Immunity in rgs2؊/؊ Mice. To determine whether RGS2 plays a role in T cell responses in vivo, rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice were injected in the footpad with LCMV (21). rgs2ϩ/Ϫ mice developed an effective LCMV-induced footpad swelling reaction starting from day 6 postinfection (Fig. 3). In contrast, the footpad swelling reaction was significantly decreased and delayed in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice, indicating that RGS2 expression is required for an efficient antiviral response in vivo. Thus, the in vitro deficit in proliferation and IL-2 production by rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ T cells translates into an impaired anti-virus response in vivo, indicating that RGS2 expression is physiologically relevant for normal T cell function. These results provide the first genetic evidence that RGS proteins play a role in the activation of lymphocyte. RGS2 Controls Male Aggression and Anxiety. During our evaluation of the mutant mice, we noted unusual fighting between male rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ littermates that sometimes resulted in severe injuries. Stimuli triggering long-term plasticity in the CNS induce the up-regulation of RGS2 in the hippocampus, cortex, caudate putamen, striatum, and amygdala (15–16). Because these regions regulate a variety of behavioral pathways, we investigated whether RGS2 plays a role in CNS function. We first determined whether RGS2 deficiency in mice leads to alterations in motor functions. Both rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and rgs2ϩ/Ϫ littermate mice showed normal motor responses (tests listed in Materials and Methods). Circadian activity was normal in the mutant mice (Fig. 4A), as was exploratory behavior during two independent open field and exploration tests (not shown). rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice also exhibited appropriate performances in rotating rod (Fig. 4B) and balance tests (not shown), confirming the integrity of the neurological pathways responsible for motor coordination and balance. Spatial and conditional learning, as determined by the hidden platform, water maze, and passive avoidance tests, were comparable between rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and rgs2ϩ/Ϫ littermates. Long-term potentiation in the Schaffer collateral CA1 pathway of individual hippocampus slices was also equivalent in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and rgs2ϩ/Ϫ mice (Fig. 4C). These data show that RGS2 deficiency has no apparent effect on motor responses, circadian activity, exploratory behavior, motor coordination, or spatial learning and memory. Social dominance and territorial aggression are wellcharacterized behaviors in male mice (25). Intriguingly, rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ male mice exhibited significantly reduced aggressive behavior compared with their rgs2ϩ/Ϫ littermates, whereas the aggressive behavior of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ female mice was normal (Fig. 5A). Because aggression correlates with anxiety (25), we subjected rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ male mice to the light͞dark preference test. rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice showed a greater preference for the dark compared with rgs2ϩ/Ϫ control littermates (Fig. 5B), indicating increased anxiety. Consistent with this behavioral phenotype, rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice were found to drop significantly more fecal pellets than rgs2ϩ/Ϫ littermates during the light͞dark preference test (2.3 Ϯ 0.6 vs. 0.8 Ϯ 0.3 pellets) and Fig. 3. Reduced LCMV-induced footpad swelling in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. Mice were infected in the footpad with LCMV and swelling measured. Average footpad swelling (percentage increase compared with footpads thickness before infection) of six animals per group. Fig. 4. Normal circadian activity, motor functions, and long-term potentiation (LTP) in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. (A) Circadian activity was monitored during a 24-h period (12-h light͞dark cycles). Mean crosses accumulated per 30-min intervals Ϯ SD are shown. (B) Normal motor function in a rotarod test. Mean values of retention times (latency) Ϯ SD are shown. Trials indicate a stepwise increase in rotarod speed (10–40 rpm, 5-rpm increase per trial). (C) Tetanus-induced LTP in CA1 neurons. Averaged normalized field EPSP (fEPSP) slope measurements obtained from 11 hippocampus slices of 4 rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and 4 rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. Oliveira-dos-Santos et al. PNAS ͉ October 24, 2000 ͉ vol. 97 ͉ no. 22 ͉ 12275 MEDICALSCIENCES exploration test (3.5 Ϯ 0.6 vs. 1.3 Ϯ 0.6 pellets). In addition, rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice exhibited an increased response to acoustic startling. These data show that RGS2 controls neuronal circuits for aggression and anxiety. RGS2 Role in Synapse Development and Basal Electrical Activity of Hippocampal CA1 Neurons. Fear and anxiety usually involve the amygdala, and certain components of fear can be affected by hippocampal inputs into the amygdala (26). Neurons from the CA1 region of the hippocampus project to the amygdala and removal of the hippocampus affects fear-based behavior (27). Moreover, it has also been proposed that RGS2 can regulate the activity of the G protein-coupled metabotropic glutamate reFig. 5. RGS2 regulates male aggression and anxiety. (A) Reduced male aggressive behavior. Aggression was measured in paired groups of male or female rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ and rgs2ϩ/Ϫ mice by using a replacement test. Results represented as percentages of displacement per group (‘‘wins’’). Eighteen trials were carried out for each group. (B) Increased anxiety. The anxiety response was measured by using a dark͞light preference test. Note the increased preference for the dark environment in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. The difference in mean dark͞light latency Ϯ SD was statistically significant (t test; P Ͻ 0.05). Fig. 6. Reduced spine density in CA1 hippocampal neurons of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. (A–D) Histology of hippocampus of rgs2ϩ/Ϫ (A and B) and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ (C and D) mice. Blue lines demarcate the CA1 neuron region. Normal structure of the CA1 region in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. Golgi–Cox staining, ϫ4 and ϫ200. (E) Reduced numbers of spines in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ CA1 neurons. Densities (spines͞10 ␮m Ϯ SD) of apical and basilar spines were determined by camera lucida. Differences in apical and basilar spine densities were statistically significant (t test, P Ͻ 0.005). Fig. 7. Impaired basal electric activity in CA1 neurons of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. (A and B) Reduction of input͞output relationship in CA1 pathway in rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. Superimposed recordings of CA1 field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSP) evoked by electrical stimulation of Schaffer collateral input. Input: peak amplitude of presynaptic volley. Output: field EPSP slope measured at 10–65% rising phase of EPSP. Data are from seven hippocampus slices of four rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and four rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice. 12276 ͉ www.pnas.org Oliveira-dos-Santos et al. ceptor 1, a receptor implicated in long-term synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus (28). Detailed examination of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice revealed that the number of neurons in the hippocampus (Fig. 6 A–D), in other brain nuclei, the brainstem, cerebellum, and cortex (not shown) were not affected by RGS2 deficiency. Interestingly, whereas dendritic morphology and branching were comparable among cortical neurons of rgs2ϩ/Ϫ and rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice, hippocampal CA1 neurons of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice showed a significant decrease in the density of apical and basilar spines compared with those of rgs2ϩ/Ϫ control littermates (Fig. 6E). The number of spines correlates with synapse numbers and neuronal plasticity (29). Importantly, the morphological deficit in spine numbers of rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ CA1 neurons correlated with significantly reduced electrical input͞output relationship in these cells (Fig. 7 A and B). These data show that RGS2 has a role in synaptic development and basal electrical activity of hippocampal CA1 neurons. Alterations in aggressive behavior and fear responses are important manifestations in highly prevalent psychiatric disorders such as depression or addictive behaviors. Because these disorders are responsible for a large social-economic burden, it is critical to identify gene products potentially involved in the initiation and͞or progression of these behavioral ‘‘traits’’ into clinically manifest ‘‘disease.’’ Multiple G protein-coupled receptors can regulate these behaviors, and pharmacological drugs that modulate these receptors have been developed to mitigate anxiety disorders and aggression. Our study provides genetic evidence that loss of a molecular regulators of G-protein signaling, RGS2, leads to impaired synaptic development in the hippocampus, reduced male aggressive behavior, and increased fear. Thus, RGS-family member such as RGS2 could be involved in the genetic predisposition to human neuropsychiatric disorders and could be potential targets for the generation of novel neuropharmacological drugs. rgs2Ϫ/Ϫ mice also provide a useful animal model to elucidate molecular mechanisms of fear and male aggression. G protein-coupled receptors regulate fundamental processes of development, cell activation, and behavior. The present study shows that RGS2, a member of the RGS family, has important roles in T cell proliferation and IL-2 production. The T cell activation deficit observed in vitro translates into an impaired anti-virus response in vivo, indicating that RGS2 is physiologically relevant for normal T cell function and the establishment of an effective antiviral immune response. In addition, mice lacking RGS2 exhibit significantly reduced aggressive behavior and increased anxiety in the absence of any measured cognitive and motor deficits. Furthermore, morphological and electrophysiological analyses indicate that RGS2 expression is required for the generation of synaptic plasticity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Thus, RGS2 is a critical regulator of T cell activation, neuronal function, and emotive fear, and aggression in vivo. We are most grateful to K. Bachmaier, Y.-Y. Kong, and I. Kozieradzki for help and to M. Saunders for editing of the manuscript. J.M.P. and J.R. are supported by grants from the Medical Research Council of Canada. D.P.S. is supported by National Institutes of Health Grant AA11605. 1. Gilman, A. G. (1987) Annu. Rev. Biochem. 56, 615–649. 2. Berman, D. M., Wilkie, T. M. & Gilman, A. G. (1996) Cell 86, 445–452. 3. Watson, N., Linder, M. E., Druey, K. M., Kehrl, J. H. & Blumer, K. J. (1996) Nature (London) 383, 172–175. 4. Hunt, T. W., Fields, T. A., Casey, P. J. & Peralta, E. G. (1996) Nature (London) 383, 175–177. 5. Snow, B. E., Hall, R. A., Krumins, A. M., Brothers, G. M., Bouchard, D., Brothers, C. A., Chung, S., Mangion, J., Gilman, A. G., et al. (1998) J. Biol. Chem. 273, 17749–17755. 6. Koelle, M. R. & Horvitz, H. R. (1996) Cell 84, 115–125. 7. Berman, D. M. & Gilman, A. G. (1998) J. Biol. Chem. 273, 1269–1272. 8. Chan, R. K. & Otte, C. A. (1982) Mol. Cell. Biol. 2, 11–20. 9. Dohlman, H. G., Song, J., Ma, D., Courchesne, W. E. & Thorner, J. (1996) Mol. Cell. Biol. 16, 5194–5209. 10. Siderovski, D. P., Strockbine, B. & Behe, C. I. (1999) Crit. Rev. Biochem. Mol. Biol. 34, 215–251. 11. Chen, C. K., Burns, M. E., He, W., Wensel, T. G., Baylor, D. A. & Simon, M. I. (2000) Nature (London) 403, 557–560. 12. Zeng, L, Fagotto, F., Zhang, T., Hsu, W., Vasicek, T. J., Perry 3rd, W. L., Lee, J. J., Gumbiner, B. M. & Costantini, F. (1997) Cell 90, 181–192. 13. Siderovski, D. P., Heximer, S. P. & Forsdyke, D. R. (1994) DNA Cell Biol. 13, 125–147. 14. Heximer, S. P., Cristillo, A. D. & Forsdyke, D. R. (1997) DNA Cell Biol. 16, 589–598. 15. Ingi, T., Krumins, A. M., Chidiac, P., Brothers, G. M., Chung, S., Snow, B. E., Barnes, C. A., Lanahan, A. A., Siderovski, D. P., Ross, E. M., et al. (1998) J. Neurosci. 18, 7178–7188. 16. Burchett, S. A., Volk, M. L., Bannon, M. J. & Granneman, J. G. (1998) J. Neurochem. 70, 2216–2219. 17. Heximer, S. P., Watson, N., Linder, M. E., Blumer, K. J. & Hepler, J. R. (1997) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94, 14389–14393. 18. Beadling, C., Druey, K., Richter, G., Kehrl, J. & Smith, K. A. (1999) J. Immunol. 162, 2677–2682. 19. Tseng, C-C. & Zhang, X. Y. (1998) Endocrinology 139, 4470–4475. 20. Liu, Q., Oliveira-Dos-Santos, A. J., Mariathasan, S., Bouchard, D., Jones, J., Sarao, R., Kozieradzki, I., Ohashi, P., Penninger, J. M. & Dumont, D. (1998) J. Exp. Med. 188, 1333–1342. 21. Penninger, J. M., Fischer, K. D., Sasaki, T., Kozieradzki, I., Le, J., Tedford, K., Bachmaier, K., Ohashi, P. S. & Bachmann, M. F. (1999) Eur. J. Immunol. 29, 1709–1718. 22. Whishaw, I. Q., Haun, F. & Kolb, B. (1999) in Modern Techniques in Neuroscience, eds. Windhorst, U. & Johansson, H. (Springer, New York). 23. Lu, Y. M., Roder, J. C., Davidow, J. & Salter, M. W. (1998) Science 279, 1363–1367. 24. Kehrl, J. H. (1998) Immunity 8, 1–10. 25. Parmigiani, S., Ferrari, P. F. & Palanza, P. (1998) Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 143–153. 26. Rogan, M. T. & LeDoux, J. E. (1996) Cell 85, 469–475. 27. White, N. M. & McDonald, R. J. (1993) Behav. Brain. Res. 55, 269–281. 28. Kammermeier, P. J. & Ikeda, S. R. (1999) Neuron 22, 819–829. 29. Moser, M. B. (1999) Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 55, 593–600. Oliveira-dos-Santos et al. PNAS ͉ October 24, 2000 ͉ vol. 97 ͉ no. 22 ͉ 12277 MEDICALSCIENCES