BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS NATURE|Vol 438|24 November 2005 442 *Biophysics Program, University of California, San Francisco, California 94143, USA e-mail: cavoigt@picasso.ucsf.edu Center for Systems and Synthetic Biology and Institute for Cell and Molecular Biology, and Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, University of Texas, Austin, Texas 78712, USA §Department of Synthetic Biology, Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory, Berkeley, California 94720, USA ||Department of Pharmaceutical Chemistry, University of California, San Francisco, California 94107, USA 1. Yeh,K.-C.,Wu,S.-H.,Murphy,J.T.&Lagarias,J.C.Science 277,1505­1508(1997). 2. Schmitz,O.,Katayama,M.,Williams,S.B.,Kondo,T.& Golden,S.S.Science289,765­768(2000). 3. Davis,S.J.,Vener,A.V.&Vierstra,R.D.Science286, 2517­2520(1999). 4. Utsumi,R.etal.Science245,1246­1249(1989). 5. Jin,T.&Inouye,M.J.Mol.Biol.244,477­481(1994). 6. Kwon,O.,Georgellis,D.&Lin,E.C.C. J.Biol.Chem.278, 13192­13195(2003). 7. Shimizu-Sato,S.,Huq,E.,Tepperman,J.M.&Quail,P.H. NatureBiotechnol.20,1041­1044(2002). 8. Gambetta,G.A.&Lagarias,J.C.Proc.NatlAcad.Sci.USA 98,10566­10571(2001). Supplementary information accompanies this communication on Nature's website. Competing financial interests: declared none. doi:10.1038/nature04405 used by Pharaoh's ants in trail choice, or they could prevent strong positive feedback by attractive pheromones from locking the sys- tem into suboptimal solutions1,8 . Elva J. H. Robinson*, Duncan E. Jackson, Mike Holcombe, Francis L. W. Ratnieks* *Laboratory of Apiculture and Social Insects, Department of Animal and Plant Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S10 2TN, UK e-mail: e.robinson@dcs.sheffield.ac.uk Department of Computer Science, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S1 4DP, UK 1. Sumpter,D.J.T.&Beekman,M.Anim.Behav.66,273­280 (2003). 2. Camazine,S.etal.Self-OrganizationinBiologicalSystems (PrincetonUniv.Press,PrincetonandOxford,2001). 3. Stickland,T.R.,Britton,N.F.&Franks,N.R.inInformation ProcessinginSocialInsects (edsDetrain,C., Deneubourg,J.L.&Pasteels,J.M.)83­100(Birkhäuser, Basel,1999). 4. Britton,N.F.,Stickland,T.R.&Franks,N.R.J.Biol.Syst.6, 315­336(1998). 5. Jackson,D.E.,Holcombe,M.&Ratnieks,F.L.W.Nature 432,907­909(2004). 6. Sudd,J.H.Br.J.Anim.Behav.5,104­109(1957). 7. Jeanson,R.,Deneubourg,J.L.&Ratnieks,F.L.W.Physiol. Entomol.28,192­198(2003). 8. Beckers,R.,Deneubourg,J.L.,Goss,S.&Pasteels,J.M. InsectesSoc.37,258­267(1990). Supplementary information accompanies this communication on Nature's website. Competing financial interests: declared none. doi:10.1038/438442a BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS ARISING online www.nature.com/bca see Nature contents. Forager ants lay attractive trail pheromones to guide nestmates to food1,2 , but the effective- ness of foraging networks might be improved if pheromones could also be used to repel foragers from unrewarding routes3,4 . Here we present empirical evidence for such a negative trail pheromone, deployed by Pharaoh's ants (Monomorium pharaonis) as a `no entry' signal to mark an unrewarding foraging path. This finding constitutes another example of the sophisticated control mechanisms used in self- organized ant colonies. To investigate whether foragers lay a nega- tive signal on the unrewarding branch of a trail bifurcation, we removed paper substrate from immediately after the fork on the unrewarding branch (the other branch led to a sucrose feeder) after it had been used by a trail-laying colony of ants. This paper substrate was trans- ferred to the entrance of one branch of a similar set-up, in which both branches had previously led to sucrose and had been used by a second colony of ants. The other branch of the second set-up received a neutral control paper substrate (for details, see supplementary information). Foragers walking from the nest could choose either of the test branches or make a U-turn. We found that 69% continued to walk away from the nest and make a branch choice. Of these, most (71%) chose the branch with the control substrate ( 2 =22.1, d.f.=1, n=137, P<0.001); the remainder U-turned towards the nest on reaching the trail bifurcation. U- turns were more than four times as likely if the ant had contacted the unrewarding-branch substrate (55%) as opposed to the neutral-con- trol substrate (13%) ( 2 =40.9, d.f.=1, n=200, P<0.0001). Neither substrate came from a previously rewarding trail, so this result can- not be attributed to differences in positive-trail pheromone concentrations. We next investigated the negative signal's location by taking substrate from five locations on a bifurcating trail that had one rewarding and one unrewarding branch. These sections, along with neutral controls, were tested on unbranched foraging trails (see supplemen- tary information) by noting whether individ- ual foragers walking over them did a U-turn. Compared with ants on the control substrate, almost twice as many ants U-turned when walking on substrate from the unrewarding branch near the bifurcation (Nb) (19% and 34%, respectively; P<0.001) (Fig. 1a). How- ever, U-turns were as frequent on substrate from the unrewarding branch end (Ne) (27%) as on the control (27%) (NS) (Fig. 1a). Ants U- turned less often on sections from the reward- ing trail (stem S, 12%; feeder branch close to the bifurcation Fb, 12%; and feeder-branch end Fe, 13%). These values are significantly lower than those for the relevant control (S, P<0.001; Fb, P<0.05; Fe, P<0.001) (Fig. 1a). In the same experiment, we also determined whether foragers could detect the negative signal before reaching the substrate on which it had been laid, using walking behaviour (zigzagging versus walking straight) as a bio- assay. Our results show that significantly more ants zigzagged when approaching substrate from an unrewarding branch just after the bifurcation (P<0.01) or at the branch end (P<0.05) than did controls (Fig. 1b). Con- versely, significantly fewer zigzagged when approaching substrate leading to the feeder (S, P<0.01; Fb, P<0.05; Fe, P<0.05) (Fig. 1b). Our results show that Pharaoh's ants use a sophisticated trail system with a negative, repellent pheromone to mark unrewarding branches. The signal is concentrated at deci- sion points -- trail bifurcations5 . As it is volatile, it provides advance warning -- like human road signs situated before junctions. Across a trail network, the pheromone could help direct foragers to food by closing off unrewarding sections. Exactly how negative pheromones enhance foraging efficiency in trail networks is not known, but they might complement attractive trail pheromones6,7 ­20 ­10 ­20 ­30 ­10 0 10 20 0 S S Fb Fe Nb Ne P<0.01 P<0.001 P<0.05 P<0.001 P<0.001 (NS) Fb P<0.05 Fe P<0.05 Nb P<0.01 Ne P<0.05 Percentagezigzagorstraight- walkingantsrelativetocontrols NumberofantsU-turning relativetocontrols 10 30 20 40 a b ­15 ­5 5 15 Figure 1 | Identifying the location of the negative pheromone. The ants' response is monitored by their walking behaviour, with U-turning or zigzagging on unbranched trails indicating detection. Test sections: S, 1 mm before bifurcation; Fb and Nb, 3 mm after bifurcation on feeder and non-feeder branches, respectively; Fe and Ne, 60 mm from bifurcation at the ends of feeder and non-feeder branches, respectively. (For details and chi-squared tests, see supplementary information.) a, Number of ants that U-turned while walking on different test sections, relative to controls. b, Percentage of straight-walking (left bars) or zigzagging (right bars) ants, relative to controls. INSECT COMMUNICATION `No entry' signal in ant foraging NaturePublishing Group2005