system for normal behavioral modulation. If this is so, then pharmacologically elevated or lowered levels of serotonin (or other modulators of behavior) could produce similar or different behavioral phenotypes, depending on the species examined and the multiplicity of effects mediated by the pharmacologically altered substance. A novel model system: aggression in male fruit flies Despite the wealth of information gathered from inverte- brate models of aggression and that of the roles of amines such as serotonin and octopamine in this behavior, little, if any, information exists on exactly how amine neurons function during fighting. Do their firing rates change? If so, are the changes seen before, during or after fights? What are the consequences of such changes? The major difficulty that is shared by invertebrate and vertebrate models, is that the physiological activity in these neurons cannot easily be monitored while animals are fighting; thus, although hypotheses are abundant, we are left with the unsatisfactory conclusion that amines clearly are instrumental in aggression, but how they serve this role remains unknown. One experimental approach that is not readily available in most invertebrate models, is the use of genetics and the accompanying powerful genetic experimental methods; thus, there are no inbred lines of lobsters, crickets or crayfish, and their genomes have not been sequenced. Molecular approaches, including RNAi, probably can be used with these species but it would be on an individual animal basis and would not involve the generation of experimental lines of, for example, highly aggressive animals. In all respects, other than the size of their neurons, fruit flies would be an ideal species to use, providing that robust patterns of aggression can be trig- gered in these animals under ethologically acceptable experimental conditions. Aggression and territoriality are well-known in certain Hawaiian species of fruit flies [64,65]; what is less well- known, however, is that aggression also exists in common highly inbred species of fruit flies (Drosophila melano- gaster). Originally described by Sturtevant in 1915 [66], other investigators have also reported an aggression in fruit flies during the period 1960­1990 [67­70]. This early work described many of the comprising components of the behavior, but in experimentally difficult scenarios involving many individuals. This was simplified recently to only a pair of male flies in a chamber with a small food cup and a headless mated female, illuminated from above. The food, potential mate and light serve to attract males to the surface, and within a few minutes both flies move onto the food cup, where they commence a series of encounters [71 ]. The flies used in these fights are isolated as soon as they emerge as adults, and kept for 3­4 days in small test tubes containing food. Using these experimental conditions, an ethogram of the behavior was assembled, transition matrices were constructed and a Markov Chain analysis produced a quantitative analysis of the behavior. Flies meet and engage in encounters about once a minute. These encounters show varying intensity levels (Figure 1) and an average duration of 11 s; this varies proportionately with the intensity level. A hier- archical relationship is established relatively early in fights, but losers can continue to re-engage winners for several hours. With the behavior well characterized in a relatively simple experimental paradigm, one can now begin to take advantage of the powerful genetic methods that are available. Mutant studies of aggression in flies A few publications have explored the effects of selected classical mutations and chemical modifications of neuro- transmitter levels on fighting behavior of flies. Ebony flies fail to incorporate b-alanine into their cuticles and black mutants show decreased synthesis of the same amino acid. These defects, in normal cuticular tanning, result in an early onset of courtship and enhanced territoriality [68]. A more recent study [72] has corroborated the results of earlier studies, which used the b-alanine mutants, and has explored the roles of dopamine and serotonin (using precursors of the amine or blockers of amine synthesis), octopamine (using a null mutant) and mushroom bodies (examined using transgenic animals expressing tetanus Figure 1 (a) (b) (c) (d) The components of fighting behavior in adult male fruit flies: (a) wing threat, in which both wings are elevated for a sustained period of time. This is often seen early in fights; (b) fencing, in which animals push off with one of their legs in a sideward or forward direction; (c) boxing, in which both flies rise on their hind legs and thrash at each other with their forelegs; (d) chase, seen when a hierarchical relationship has been established, with the winner pursuing the loser who will either fly from the food surface or retreat to the edge of the food dish. Reprinted from [69], in a slightly modified form, with permission. 738 Neurobiology of behaviour Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2003, 13:736­743 www.current-opinion.com toxin in output neurons of the mushroom bodies). The experimental protocol that was used here was compli- cated, with six males and three mated females in a chamber at the same time. The scoring system used was also complicated; however, the results successfully indicated that octopamine null mutants and flies with reduced synaptic output from the mushroom bodies showed reduced aggression, whereas, flies with elevated dopamine showed somewhat reduced aggression. Altera- tion of serotonin levels had no effect on aggression. It should be noted that many important controls were not included in these studies, which, according to the authors, were preliminary. Repetition of these studies, using a more simplified experimental protocol, with conditional mutant lines of flies would greatly help to interpret the observed results. Much more powerful genetic methods are available for behavioral studies on aggression with flies, however, than have been reported thus far. These include conditional expression of mutations in the fly brain, whenever and wherever desired. One example is the GAL4/UAS sys- tem, originally described by Brand and Perrimon [73]. In this method, a cross between two transgenic lines of flies (one expressing the transcription factor GAL4 in subtypes of neurons, the other expressing the binding site for GAL4, driving expression of any desired gene) yields progeny in which desired genes are expressed in subtypes of neurons in the fly brain. In preliminary studies, we have expressed GAL4 in dopamine and serotonin neurons or selectively in dopamine neurons to drive expression of a temperature-sensitive mutant form of the protein dyna- min in those cells [74 ]. This protein behaves normally at 258, becoming mutated at 308 and leading to a rapid block of vesicle recycling and hence of synaptic transmission at the elevated temperature [75 ]. In preliminary studies (unpublished observations), we have observed decreased numbers of encounters between flies at the elevated temperature, using both GAL4 lines. Further experi- ments with this and related technology should yield valuable information about the role of subtypes of neu- rons in aggression in flies. Aggression in female flies As with male flies, Sturtevant was the first to mention aggression in female flies, in this case, directed towards males: `occasionally a female seems to frighten off a male by spreading her wings and moving quickly towards him' [66]. A second mention of aggression in females came in a study comparing mating success in ebony and light males of freshly isolated D. melanogaster [65]. A more complete study of aggression in females was published last year, however, by Ueda and Kidokoro, using the common Canton-S strain of D. melanogaster [76 ]. They observed and scored three patterns of female aggression, including `approach', `lunge' and `wings erect'. The authors report that these patterns `are similar to those of male aggression in this species' but that they differ from the patterns shown in female rejection behavior during courtship. Females show enhanced fighting if live yeast are growing on the surface of the food dish; conditioning female flies to the yeast food source $12 hours before their fights, reduced the extent of fighting. This was interpreted as a possible adaptation to the enriched food source. Higher levels of aggression were seen when newly emerged females were held in isolation before the fights, in com- parison with females held in groups of ten in a vial. Thus, both housing conditions and the quality of the food source influence fighting behavior in female flies, and, as with males, newly emerged females began to show aggression only a day after their emergence as adults. Recently, we confirmed those behavioral patterns reported by Ueda and Kidokoro [76 ], but also noted that specific differences are seen between the behavioral patterns in male and female D. melanogaster (unpublished observations). Certain patterns are seen predominantly in male flies, not in females (extended-duration wing-threat, boxing, tussling, holding and chasing), whereas, other patterns are seen mainly in females and not males (lung- ing with head butting, front limb fencing in an elevated posture). The remaining offensive and defensive patterns beyond these (for ethograms of male fighting behavior see [69]) are shared and seen in both males and females. Of particular relevance here, is a report by Lee and Hall of a new behavioral phenotype seen in fruitless ( fru) mutant male flies [77]: enhanced head interactions. Fruitless male flies court other males, forming long court- ship chains, and show abnormal patterns of wing vibra- tion during courtship, but otherwise appear capable of mating with females, depending on the fru mutant sub- type [78]. In addition to enhanced head interactions, fru mutant males reportedly do not display boxing, which is a common high intensity component of male fruit fly fighting behavior. The head­head interactions are not seen during the first day after eclosion; their occurrence increases to a maximum 4­5 days later, in parallel with the chaining behavior. These results raise an interesting question: does the fru mutation, with its well-studied effects on mating beha- vior, also direct the expression of female patterns of fighting behavior in the brains of male fru mutants? Are male- and female-specific patterns of fighting beha- vior specified in the brains of fruit flies after gender has been defined by the sex-determination hierarchy of genes? Is there an aggression-determination hierarchy of genes? Are male- and female-specific patterns of aggression defined as units in the brains of flies or can components of the behavior (like head butting in females and tussling in males) be transferred as individual mod- ules into male and female brains, using methods of genetic manipulation? The fruit fly model offers exciting Aggression in invertebrates Kravitz and Huber 739 www.current-opinion.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2003, 13:736­743 Table 1 Aggression studies in various species of invertebrates (a partial listing, including only articles cited in this publication) Ethology Physiology Amines References INSECTS (social) Bees Apis mellifera [2,13,14,15 ,19] Apis florea [3] Apis cerana [4] Andrena scotica [7] Panurgus calcaratus [7] Bombus terrestris [22] Ants Formica pratensis [5] Formica exsecta [12] Linepithema humile [6,10,11,17] Soleenopsis invicta [16,18] Cataglyphis niger [20] Odontomachus brunneus [21] Termites Reticulitermes spp [9] Wasps Vespa crabro [8] INSECTS (non-social) Dragon flies Pachidiplex longipennis [28] Crickets Gryllus bimaculatus [29­34] Fruit flies Drosophila melanogaster y [67-70,71 ,72,76 ,77,78] Drosophila simulans [70] Drosophila sylvestris [64,65] Drosophila heteroneura [64,65] Drosophila ampelophila [66] CRUSTACEANS Lobsters Homarus americanus [36 ,37 ,44,49,58,60 ,61,63] Munida quadrispina [59] Crabs Carcinus maenas [42] Crayfish Orconectes rusticus [45,46,48,50­52,55] Astacus astacus [60 ,62] Procambarus clarkii [39,41 ,43,56] Pacifastacus leniusculus [40,54] OTHER ARTHROPODS Spiders Dolomedes triton [26] Misumena vatia [27] Portia spp. [23] Latrodectus hasselti [24] The table lists species in which: (i) ethological studies on aggression have been conducted (ethology); (ii) physiological and/or anatomical studies directly relating to aggression have been conducted (physiology); a role of amines in aggression has been demonstrated (amines). Anatomical and/or electrophysiological studies using intracellular and/or extracellular recording methods have been conducted in these species, but these are not directly related to aggression. y Genetic studies related to aggression have been conducted. 740 Neurobiology of behaviour Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2003, 13:736­743 www.current-opinion.com possibilities for the identification of genes involved in producing patterns of a complex behavior like aggression in nervous systems. The shaping of these patterns, by experience and by hormones, throughout the lives of such organisms can then be fully elucidated. Conclusions Aggression is seen and has been studied behaviorally in many invertebrate species (Table 1), including social and non-social insects, other arthropods and crustaceans. In many of these species, detailed physiological and anato- mical studies are now possible; they have been performed in relatively few of the insect species (mainly crickets and bees), but more widely among the crustaceans. Crusta- cean species are exciting because they enable the study of aggression to the level of the individual synapses, neurons and circuits that are key to this behavior. In all inverte- brate systems examined thus far, altering the levels or function of amine neurons causes important changes in aggression; in one particular model, changes in status are accompanied by alterations in the function of certain synaptic regions relating to amines. Despite this progress, which goes far beyond our knowledge of vertebrate sys- tems, no clear picture has yet emerged of exactly how amines modulate or alter the behavior. Recent experi- ments using fruit flies as a model for aggression have introduced the possibility of adding powerful genetic methods to the armamentarium of tools that are available for the study of aggression in invertebrates. Ultimately, this will allow investigators more insight into how this complex pattern of behavior is assembled in the nervous system and should shed further light on the role of modulators in the behavior. Acknowledgements It is difficult to summarize as large a field as aggression in invertebrates in a short review of this type, particularly one that tries to concentrate on recent literature. Moreover, studies in this large field use many species of animals and range from purely ethological examinations of agonistic behavior to physiological, molecular and genetic studies viewed from a neuroethological perspective. With the space constraints facing us, we recognized in advance that we could not do justice to the studies of all the excellent investigators who work in this field, and apologize to those of our colleagues who feel slighted by our somewhat amine-centric review of the field. EA Kravitz was supported by grants from NSF (IBN 90730), NIGMS (GM65595) and a starter grant from the Gund Fund of the Mind, Brain Behavior Program at Harvard University. R Huber was supported by grants from NSF (IBN 9874608) and NIMH (62557). Mostly, we acknowledge the outstanding contributions of the dedicated young undergraduate and graduate students and post-doctoral fellows who have worked with us over the years on the studies discussed here. References and recommended reading Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of review, have been highlighted as: of special interest of outstanding interest 1. Haspel G, Libersat F: Wasp venom blocks central cholinergic synapses to induce transient paralysis in cockroach prey. J Neurobiol 2003, 54:628-637. 2. Bernasconi G, Ratnieks FLW, Rand E: Effect of `spraying' by fighting honey bee queens (Apis mellifera L.) on the temporal structure of fights. Insectes Sociaux 2000, 47:21-26. 3. Halling LA, Oldroyd BP, Wattanachaiyingcharoen W, Barron AB, Nanork P, Wongsiri S: Worker policing in the bee Apis florea. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2001, 49:509-513. 4. Oldroyd BP, Halling LA, Good G, Wattanachaiyingcharoen W, Barron A, Nanork P, Wongsiri S, Ratnieks FLW: Worker policing and worker reproduction in Apis cerana. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2001, 50:371-377. 5. Beye M, Neumann P, Chapuisat M, Pamilo P, Moritz RFA: Nestmate recognition and the genetic relatedness of nests in the ant Formica pratensis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1998, 43:67-72. 6. Roulston TH, Buczkowski G, Silverman J: Nestmate discrimination in ants: effect of bioassay on aggressive behavior. Insectes Sociaux 2003, 50:151-159. 7. Paxton RJ, Kukuk PF, Tengo¨ J: Effects of familiarity and nestmate number on social interactions in two communal bees, Andrena scotica and Panurgus calcaratus (Hymenoptera, Andrenidae). Insectes Sociaux 1999, 46:109-118. 8. Ruther J, Sieben S, Schricker B: Nestmate recognition in social wasps: manipulation of hydrocarbon profiles induces aggression in the European hornet. Naturwissenschaften 2002, 89:111-114. 9. Polizzi JM, Forschler BT: Factors that affect aggression among the worker caste of Reticulitermes spp. subterranean termites (isoptera: rhinotermitidae). J Insect Behav 1999, 12:133-146. 10. Liang D, Blomquist GJ, Silverman J: Hydrocarbon-released nestmate aggression in the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile, following encounters with insect prey. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol 2001, 129:871-882. 11. Suarez AV, Holway DA, Liang D, Tsutsui ND, Case TJ: Spatiotemporal patterns of intraspecific aggression in the invasive Argentine ant. Anim Behav 2002, 64:697-708. 12. Brown W, Liautard C, Keller L: Sex-ratio dependent execution of queens in polygynous colonies of the ant Formica exsecta. Oecologia 2003, 134:12-17. 13. Giray T, Robinson GE: Common endocrine and genetic mechanisms of behavioral development in male and worker honey bees and the evolution of division of labor. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996, 93:11718-11722. 14. Pearce AN, Huang ZY, Breed MD: Juvenile hormone and aggression in honey bees. J Insect Physiol 2001, 47:1243-1247. 15. Schulz DJ, Sullivan JP, Robinson GE: Juvenile hormone and octopamine in the regulation of division of labor in honey bee colonies. Horm Behav 2002, 42:222-231. Clear evidence of endocrine control of caste in honeybees, with emphasis of specific roles for octopamine and juvenile hormone. Treatment with either octopamine or juvenile hormone induces foraging behavior. The results suggest further that juvenile hormone influences foraging behavior in part by increasing levels of octopamine in the brain, but octopamine also exerts actions that are independent of juvenile hormone. 16. Klobuchar EA, Deslippe RJ: A queen pheromone induces workers to kill sexual larvae in colonies of the red imported fire ant (Solenopsis invicta). Naturwissenschaften 2002, 89:302-304. 17. Reuter M, Balloux F, Lehmann L, Keller L: Kin structure and queen execution in the Argentine ant Linepithema humile. J Evol Biol 2001, 14:954-958. 18. Vander Meer RK, Alonso LE: Queen primer pheromone affects conspecific fire ant (Solenopsis invicta) aggression. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2002, 51:122-130. 19. Gilley DC: The behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera ligustica) during queen duels. Ethology 2001, 107:601-622. 20. Nowbahari E, Fé né ron R, Malherbe MC: Effect of body size on aggression in the ant, Cataglyphis niger (hymenoptera; formicidae). Aggressive Behav 1999, 25:369-379. 21. Powell S, Tschinkel WR: Ritualized conflict in Odontomachus brunneus and the generation of interaction-based task allocation: a new organizational mechanism in ants. Anim Behav 1999, 58:965-972. Aggression in invertebrates Kravitz and Huber 741 www.current-opinion.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2003, 13:736­743